1. Introduction
This paper discusses the dative (recipient) markers in the Sinitic languages spoken in the Hunan Province.
Before embarking on the examination of the dative markers, it is pertinent to offer a brief introduction to the languages spoken in the Hunan Province. Hunan is located in the south-central region of China. According to
Bao and Li (
1985), the Hunan Sinitic languages can be classified into five broad areas: Xiang varieties spoken in the center of Hunan; Southwestern Mandarin varieties spoken in the west and south; Gan and Hakka varieties spoken in the east; Waxiang spoken in the west; and Tuhua
1 spoken in the south (both within the Mandarin-speaking regions). Additionally, there are several non-Sinitic languages spoken in Hunan: Tujia in the northwest; Miao in the west; Dong in the southwest; and Yao in the south. Of these languages, Tujia exhibits SOV word order, while other languages use SVO order. Hunan is classified as a transitional zone for Sinitic languages in
Chappell (
2015), such that an examination of the Sinitic languages in Hunan can shed light on the refinement of this linguistic area and contribute to a better understanding of the linguistic development within Sinitic languages.
Dative usually refers to a morphological case that prototypically marks the recipient or indirect object in a ditransitive construction (cf.
Haspelmath 2016). In this paper, we use
dative marker to indicate the element that introduces the recipient argument, as the morpheme
to in
I gave a pen to Paul in English. Most studies on the Sinitic ditransitive constructions focus on the sentence structures, especially on the relative word order between the recipient and the theme (see
Hashimoto 1976;
Zhu 1979;
Yue-Hashimoto 1993, for example).
Chin (
2010) firstly identified two types of dative markers in Sinitic languages (i.e., the
go-type and the
give-type) and discussed the chronological development of the two types of dative markers. See also
Li and Wu (
2015) for the development of dative markers in Yichun Gan.
In this paper, we present five types of dative markers from a synchronic perspective on the one hand, and we explore four grammaticalization pathways underlying these various types of dative markers on the other. Grammaticalization is defined as the development from lexical to grammatical forms and from grammatical to even more grammatical forms (
Kuteva et al. 2019, p. 3). Cross-linguistically, the dative markers are frequently derived from allative markers, GIVE verbs, and benefactive markers (
Kuteva et al. 2019).
In the languages that we investigated, we observe all these three patterns: namely, the dative marker shares the same form as either the allative marker, the GIVE verb, or the benefactive marker in each language. However, in order to clarify how the dative use was developed from these sources, especially for the GIVE verbs which are generally TAKE verbs in origin, we take the passive markers and differential object markers which are closely related to GIVE/TAKE verbs into consideration and refine the three main patterns into five: DAT = ALL; DAT = GIVE = OM ≠ PASS; DAT = GIVE = OM = PASS; DAT = GIVE = PASS ≠ OM; DAT = BEN. The detail of our methodology is presented in
Section 2.
The layout of this article is as follows: this introduction leads into
Section 2, which presents our definitions for the markers and relevant constructions, our terminology and methodology. We take allative markers, GIVE verbs, benefactive markers, passive markers, and differential object markers into consideration, and classify the 30 languages into five types. They will be discussed in detail in
Section 3 with a map showing their areal distribution. The relevant diachronic developments of dative markers are found in
Section 4 and are followed by a conclusion in
Section 5.
2. Methodology and Definitions
The sample of 30 languages spoken in the Hunan Province covers the five groups of Sinitic languages (i.e., Xiang, Gan, Southwestern Mandarin, Hakka, and Tuhua) and one unclassified one (i.e., Waxiang). Both fieldwork data and data from descriptive grammars and journal articles are used. Data are glossed and translated by the author when not provided in the original literature. The locations of all the languages can be found in
Figure 1.
In this paper, only the dative marker in the main
2 ditransitive construction of the language is considered. As indicated in
Zhang (
2011), the Southern Sinitic languages
3 use the postverbal dative construction
4 [V + T(heme) + DAT + R(ecipient)] to encode transfer. This is the case for most of our sample languages. However, the preverbal dative construction [DAT + R+V + T] is also attested as the main type in some languages spoken in northwestern Hunan, such as in Waxiang. For the languages that use postverbal dative markers, we can identify two primary patterns: DAT = ALL and DAT = GIVE; for the languages that use preverbal dative markers, we can find a third pattern: DAT = BEN.
The dative markers that are related to allative markers and benefactive markers are relatively straightforward to identify and analyze, but when the dative marker shares the same form as the GIVE verb, we need to further confirm how the dative use is developed from the GIVE verb. Regarding the GIVE verbs, it refers to the general-purpose verb of giving in this paper, such as
gěi 给 in Standard Mandarin or
give in English. GIVE verbs have been extensively discussed in the literature due to their polyfunctionality in Sinitic languages (see
Lai 2001;
Chin 2011;
Ngai 2015;
Lu and Szeto 2023). For instance, in Standard Mandarin,
gěi can act as a benefactive marker, dative marker, causative verb, passive marker, and differential object marker. However, in the Southern Sinitic languages, the GIVE verbs are more diverse in forms, such as
pa41 把 in Changsha,
te22 得 in Hengyang, and
lɛ44 拿 in Lianyuan from our sample, and they are TAKE
5 verbs in origin. Furthermore, in most cases, they manifest different syntactic behaviors from
gěi in Northern Sinitic languages. See the two example sentences below in Standard Mandarin and in Huarong.
(1) Standard Mandarin (Zhu 2009, p. 170) |
给 | 我 | 一 | 枝 | 笔。 |
gěi | wǒ | yì | zhī | bǐ |
give |
1sg | one | clf | pen |
‘Give me a pen.’ |
(2) Huarong (SWM; own fieldwork) |
把 | 本 | 书 | 得 | 我。 |
pɑ21 | pən21 | ɕy53 | tɛ13 | ŋo21 |
give | clf | book | dat | 1sg |
‘Give me a book.’ |
The GIVE verbs which can take an R argument (in addition to a T argument) will be considered as a genuine GIVE verb in this analysis (cf.
Zhang 2011), like the case in Standard Mandarin; while some GIVE verbs can only take a T argument but not an R argument, they solely have the semantic meaning of giving, like the case in Huarong
6. When we consider that a dative marker is grammaticalized from a GIVE verb, it is generally the genuine GIVE verbs that we talk about, since it can precede an R argument and can be easily reanalyzed as a dative marker from V
2 position in a serial verb construction, as illustrated by (3).
(3) Standard Mandarin (Zhu 1979, p. 83) |
我 | 送 | 一 | 张 | 票 | 给 | 小李。 |
wǒ | song | yì | zhāng | piào | gěi | Xiǎolǐ |
1sg | offer | one | clf | ticket | dat | Xiaoli |
‘I offered a ticket to Xiaoli.’ |
In Hunan, most GIVE verbs are originally TAKE verbs, and they form postverbal dative constructions but not double object constructions, which means they take a T argument but not R argument. However, some of these GIVE
<TAKE verbs can also be used as a dative marker. Take Changsha, for example: the GIVE
<TAKE verb and the dative marker are both
pa41 把. The process of how the dative marker is developed from a GIVE
<TAKE verb will be discussed in detail in
Section 4.2.
Zhang (
2011) claims Southern Sinitic languages lack genuine GIVE verbs, and the ditransitive constructions in Southern Sinitic languages are formed by the combination of TAKE verbs and a directional element which can be templated as [TAKE + T + ALL + R]. It is exactly in this construction that TAKE verbs obtained the semantic meaning of giving. On the basis of this construction, when the T argument shows preverbally (could be topicalized or marked by an object marker) or simply mentioned in the previous context, we have [TAKE + ALL + R], and according to
Zhang (
2011), [TAKE-ALL] becomes a genuine GIVE verb, which can precede the R argument directly, and form a serial verb construction with the GIVE
<TAKE verb in ditransitive constructions, whence it can be reanalyzed as a compound dative marker: [GIVE
<TAKE + T + DAT
<[TAKE-ALL] + R]. Then, due to the frequent use, the allative marker might be omitted from the compound form, leaving the TAKE verb in the language as a dative marker alone: [GIVE
<TAKE + T + DAT
<TAKE + R].
These developmental stages can still be observed in many Southern Sinitic languages, such as in Shanghai Wu (
Qian 1997), Yichun Gan (
Li and Wu 2015), and Liancheng Hakka (
Ye 2023). Take Yichun as an example. At first, the directional element
ku42 过 is used to mark the recipient:
(4) Yichun (Gan; Li and Wu 2015) |
我 | 把 | 本 | | 书 | 过 | 你。 | [GIVE<TAKE + T + ALL + R] |
ŋo34 | pa42 | pun42 | | ɕy34 | ku42 | ɲi34 | |
1sg | give | clf | | book | dat | 2sg | |
‘I gave a book to you.’ |
Then, the GIVE
<TAKE verb combines with the directional element and forms a compound genuine GIVE verb as
pa42-ku42 把过 which can take a recipient as its argument. This compound form is then reanalyzed as a dative marker in a serial verb construction:
(5) | 我 | 把 | 本 | | 书 | 把过 | 你。 | [GIVE<TAKE + T + DAT<[TAKE-ALL] + R] |
| ŋo34 | pa42 | pun42 | | ɕy34 | pa42-ku42 | ɲi34 | |
| 1sg | give | clf | | book | dat | 2sg | |
| ‘I gave a book to you.’ |
Finally, the directional element dropped off and the GIVE
<TAKE verb alone becomes a new dative marker in Yichun:
(6) | 把 | 本 | | 书 | 把 | 你。 | [GIVE<TAKE + T + DAT<TAKE + R] |
| pa42 | pun42 | | ɕy34 | pa42 | ɲi34 | |
| give | clf | | book | dat | 2sg | |
| ‘Give a book to you.’ |
This is the first possible diachronic development for the dative markers that have the same form as the GIVE<TAKE verbs. Note that although the dative marker has the same form as the GIVE<TAKE verb in the language, it does not mean the GIVE<TAKE has become a genuine GIVE verb, because the dative marker is developed by dropping off the allative element from a genuine compound GIVE verb, but is not developed from the GIVE<TAKE verb itself. (Nevertheless, it is possible for the GIVE<TAKE verb to develops further into a genuine GIVE verb on this basis.)
The second possible explanation for a dative marker sharing the same form as the GIVE
<TAKE verbs is the that GIVE
<TAKE verb becomes a genuine GIVE verb through
relexicalization (
Güldemann 2012), and the dative use is developed from the genuine GIVE verb. This is a case mentioned in Shaowu (
Ngai 2015;
2021, p. 384).
To help us tell if the GIVE<TAKE has become a genuine GIVE verb in the language, we need to take two other makers into consideration: passive markers and differential object makers.
Chappell and Peyraube (
2006) argued “GIVE > permissive causative > passive” is a common grammaticalization pathway in Sinitic languages, such as
gěi in Beijing Mandarin or 俾
pei35 in Cantonese. Below is an example of the bridging stage for the reanalysis. In (7),
gěi can actually be interpreted as ‘to give’ in a pivot construction, a permissive causative marker, or a passive marker.
(7) Beijing Mandarin (Xu 1992) |
车 | 给 | 小王 | 修好 | 了。 |
chē | gěi | Xiǎowáng | xiū-hǎo | le |
car | give/let/pass | Xiaowang | repair-be.good | crs |
‘(Someone) gave the car to Xiaowang (and he) repaired it.’ |
or ‘(Someone) let Xiaowang have the car repaired.’ |
or ‘The car was repaired by Xiaowang.’ |
The key point for the reanalysis is that the argument after the GIVE verb has to be an R argument, which can be considered as a causee in the causative construction or an agent in the passive construction. If the GIVE<TAKE verb in a language can be used as a passive marker, we can consider that the GIVE<TAKE verb in this language is a genuine GIVE verb, thus making it also possible to develop a dative use. On the contrary, if the GIVE<TAKE verb in a language cannot be used as a passive marker, we can consider that it might still stay as a TAKE verb, or at least its GIVE use is not yet well developed.
As for the differential object marker,
Chappell (
2007) outlines two common grammaticalization pathways for the object markers in Sinitic languages: (i) TAKE (>instrumental) > OM; (ii) GIVE > benefactive > OM. If an object marker is developed from the GIVE verb, generally it has to undergo an intermediate stage as a benefactive marker. While the pathway from TAKE to object marker is much more common, such as
bǎ 把 in many Northern Sinitic languages. For the languages in Hunan, if the object marker has the same form as the GIVE
<TAKE verb, and we cannot find an identical benefactive marker, then we can consider the object marker in these languages to be more likely to be developed from the TAKE use of their GIVE
<TAKE verbs in the history.
In summary, we take passive makers and differential object markers into consideration to help us decide whether the GIVE
<TAKE verbs in our sample are genuine GIVE verbs that can develop a dative use, or they are still a TAKE verb that has gained a dative use by dropping off the allative element in a compound GIVE verb. When DAT = GIVE = PASS ≠ OM, it is more likely to be the former case; when DAT = GIVE = OM ≠ PASS, it is perhaps the latter case especially when a compound GIVE verb form can be found in the language; finally for the syncretism DAT = GIVE = PASS = OM, both cases are possible. All the grammaticalization pathways will be explained in detail in
Section 4.
Therefore, in this paper, we investigate six elements: dative markers, allative markers, benefactive markers, passive markers, differential object markers, and GIVE verbs. According to the data, we can classify the languages in our sample into five types: DAT = ALL; DAT = GIVE = OM ≠ PASS; DAT = GIVE = PASS = OM; DAT = GIVE = PASS ≠ OM; DAT = BEN.
The definitions for the five grammatical markers are given in the following part.
A dative marker or an indirect object marker marks the recipient in a ditransitive construction. It can be a postverbal marker, as in (8), or a preverbal marker, as in (9).
(8) Ningyuan (SWM; Y. Wu 2009, p. 321) |
他 | 送 | 一 | 杆 | 笔 | 给 | 我。 |
t’a33 | soŋ213 | i21 | kan45 | pi21 | kə45 | ŋo213 |
3sg | offer | one | clf | pen | dat | 1sg |
‘He offered a pen to me.’ |
(9) Ningyuan (SWM; Zhang 2009) |
有 | 话 | 就 | 和 | 牛 | 讲。 |
iəu45 | fa213 | tɕiəu213 | xo21 | liəu21 | tɕiaŋ45 |
have | speech | then | dat | buffalo | say |
‘(He) talks to the buffalo when (he) has something to say.’ |
In (8), the dative marker is
kə45 给, whereas in (9), the dative marker is
xo21 和. Note that the preverbal
xo21 can have other interpretations as well, such as being a benefactive marker, as demonstrated by an example in (10).
(10) Ningyuan (SWM; Zhang 2009) |
这里 | 景子 | 好, | 你 | 和 | 我 | 照 | 个 | 相。 |
tɕi213li45 | tɕin45tsɿ45 | xau45 | li45 | xo21 | ŋo45 | tɕiau213 | ko213 | ɕiaŋ213 |
here | view | good | 2sg | ben | 1sg | take | clf | photo |
‘The view is great here, take a photo for me.’ |
A benefactive marker is a preverbal marker, which marks the beneficiary. In this paper, we consider it as a distinct class of markers from dative markers. However, it is worth noting that a benefactive marker can develop into a preverbal dative marker, as is the case in Ningyuan.
An allative marker expresses “the meaning of motion ‘to’ or ‘towards’ a place” (
Crystal 2003, p. 19). As such, it marks the goal in a theme-goal construction, for example, as
tau45 in Ningyuan.
(11) Ningyuan (SWM; Y. Wu 2009, p. 321) |
我 | 给 | 毛毛崽 | 给 | 倒 | 床 | 高头 | 要 | 不 | 要得? |
ŋo45 | kə45 | mau21mau21tsæ45 | kə45 | tau45 | ts’uan21 | kau33t’əu21 | iau213 | pu21 | iau213tə21 |
1sg | om | baby | put | all | bed | on | ok | neg | ok |
‘Is it ok that I put the baby on the bed?’ |
A passive marker is used to mark the agent in a passive construction. Sometimes, the passive marker shares the same form as the dative marker in the Sinitic languages. For instance, the dative marker
kə45 can also be used as a passive marker.
(12) Ningyuan (SWM; Y. Wu 2009, p. 321) |
杯子 | 给 | 他 | 打烂 | 呱 | 了。 |
pei33tsɿ45 | kə45 | t’a33 | ta45-lan213 | kua21 | liau45 |
cup | pass | 3sg | hit-be.broken | cmpl | crs |
‘The cup was broken by him.’ |
A differential object marker or disposal marker marks the object in a transitive sentence or the T argument in a ditransitive or theme-goal sentence. It might also share the same form as the dative marker. In Ningyuan,
kə45 is also used as a differential object marker, as already shown in (11). Another example is given in (13).
(13) Ningyuan (SWM; Zhang 2009) |
给 | 那 | 本 | 书 | 拿 | 过来。 |
kə45 | la213 | pən45 | ɕy33 | la21 | ko213-læi21 |
om | that | clf | book | take | pass-come |
‘Bring that book over here.’ |
The forms of the dative markers of the 30 Hunan Sinitic languages are listed in
Table 1. Except for the GIVE verbs, the other elements examined in this paper, namely benefactive, allative, object, and passive markers, generally have several forms that developed from different sources. Since we are investigating the dative markers in this paper, we only list the forms that are identical or related to the dative markers. However, note that an empty cell may also indicate that we have not found the relevant form in the literature.
In the next section, we discuss the five different types of dative markers and their distribution.
3. The Five Types of Dative Markers and Their Areal Distribution
Based on the analysis of 30 Sinitic languages spoken in Hunan, which include Xiang, Gan, Southwestern Mandarin, Hakka, Tuhua, and one unclassified Sinitic language, we identify five patterns for the dative markers, as detailed in
Table 2. The dative markers are given after each language. Following this table, the areal distribution of these four types is presented in
Figure 2.
3.1. Type I: DAT = ALL (4/30)
In Type I languages, the dative marker is the same as allative marker. In our sample, this type is mainly found in the northeast of Hunan: Huarong (SWM), Yueyang (Xiang), Linxiang (Gan), apart from Xintian (Tuhua) in the south. Take Huarong, for example.
(14) Huarong (SWM; own field work) |
Dative construction |
买 | 哒 | 个 | 褂子 | 得 | 我。 |
mai21 | ta21 | ko33 | kuɑ24tsɿ33 | tɛ13 | ŋo21 |
buy | pfv | clf | coat | dat | 1sg |
‘(He) bought me a coat.’ |
(15) Allative construction |
我 | 挂 | 哒 | 一 | 张 | 全家福 | 得 | 客厅 | 的 |
ŋo21 | kuɑ24 | tɑ21 | i45 | tsɑŋ53 | tɕɦyn13tɕia53fu13 | tɛ13 | kɦɛ24tɦiɛn53 | ti33 |
1SG | hang | PFV | one | CLF | family.photo | ALL | living.room | POSS |
墙上。 |
tɕɦiɑŋ13 = sɑŋ21 |
wall = on |
‘I hung a family photo on the wall in the living room.’ |
In this type of language, the GIVE verb is always
BA 把, and three of the four languages use
DE 得 as the dative marker, while one language (i.e., Xintian) uses
kəu35 whose etymological source is not clear. In
Section 4.1, we will discuss the developmental path of
DE as a dative marker in this type of language, using Huarong as an example.
3.2. Type II: DAT = GIVE = OM ≠ PASS (3/30)
There are three languages of Type II in our sample, they are Cenchuan (Xiang), Changsha (Xiang), and Pingjiang (Gan) which are in the north central region of the Hunan Province. They use
BA 把 as the GIVE verb as well as the dative marker. Take Changsha as an example.
(16) Changsha (Xiang; Wu 2011, p. 188; Y. Wu 2009, p. 309) |
Dative construction |
他 | 送 | 哒 | 三 | 只 | 鸡 | 把 | 我。 |
tha33 | sən45 | ta21 | san33 | tsa24 | tɕi33 | pa41 | ŋo41 |
3sg | offer | pfv | three | clf | chicken | dat | 1sg |
‘He gave me three chickens as a gift.’ |
(17) Differential object marking construction |
把 | 窗户 | 打开。 |
pa41 | tɕhyan33fu | ta41-khai33 |
om | window | make-open |
‘Open the window.’ |
(18) Passive construction |
杯子 | 把得/捞/听 | 他 | 打烂 | 哒。 |
pei33tsɿ | pa41tɤ24/lau33/t’in45 | t’a33 | ta41-lan21 | ta21 |
glass | pass | 3sg | hit-be.broken | crs |
‘The glass was broken by him.’ |
Note that, as shown in (18), the compound form pa41tɤ24 can be used as the passive marker, and tɤ24 得 is exactly the allative marker in Changsha. We can consider the dative use of pa41 把 is based on the compound GIVE verb pa41tɤ24, which gradually lost its allative element in it, hence pa41 alone is used as the dative marker. In Cenchuan and Pingjiang, we do not have the necessary data to tell if they have or had a compound form.
3.3. Type III: DAT = GIVE = PASS = OM (8/30)
The dative markers of the Type III languages are the same as their passive markers and differential object markers. In our sample, there are actually 10 languages mostly spoken in the central and south of Hunan that share the pattern. Nonetheless, as introduced in
Section 2, the Type II, III, and IV languages are refined from the pattern: DAT = GIVE, so, for this reason, we exclude Xiangxiang and Lianyuan, which use the same marker for dative, passive, and object marking but have a distinct verb form for GIVE. As a result, we can only classify eight languages into this type. These are three Xiang varieties: Shaoyang, Shaodong, and Lianyuan; one Southwestern Mandarin variety: Ningyuan; two Gan varieties: Liuyang and Changning; and one Hakka variety: Rucheng. Take Shaodong, for example.
(19) Shaodong (Xiang; Sun 2009, pp. 105–12) |
Dative construction |
咯 | 只 | 衣衫 | 其 | 送 | 把 | 小明 | 哩。 |
ko31 | tɕia55-31 | iɛn55san31 | tɕi31 | səŋ35 | pa31 | ɕio31min12 | li |
this | clf | clothes | 3sg | offer | dat | XiaomingNAME | sfp |
‘He gave this clothing to Xiaoming.’ |
(20) Passive construction |
其 | 只 | 脚 | 把 | 车子 | 闯 | 哩, | 现唧 | 还 | 在 |
tɕi31 | tɕia55-31 | to55 | pa31 | t’ei55tsɿ | ts’aŋ31 | li | ɣiɛn12·tɕi | ɣa12 | dzei12 |
3SG | clf | foot | pass | car | hit | sfp | now | still | LOC |
医院里 | 诊。 |
i55yɛn35-55 = li31 | taŋ3 |
hospital = inside | treat |
‘His foot was hit by a car, and he is still in the hospital.’ |
(21) Differential object marking construction |
要 | 妹妹 | 把 | 屋 | 扫 | 一 | 下 |
io35 | mei35·mei | pa31 | u55 | sau35 | i55 | ɣa24 |
make | little.sister | om | house | sweep | one | vcl |
‘Make little sister to clean up the house.’ |
Generally speaking, the dative marker in Type III languages also shares the same form as the GIVE verb. The GIVE verbs in this type are not limited to the types that are originally TAKE verbs (e.g., pa31 把 in Shaodong or te33 得 in Changning). In Ningyuan, it is kə45 给 (which shares the same etymology as gěi in Standard Mandarin) that is used as the GIVE verb, and it can also be used as a dative marker, a passive marker and a differential object marker.
For this type of language, there are three possibilities. The dative marker could be developed from a genuine GIVE verb, like the case of Ningyuan just mentioned; or either the GIVE<TAKE verbs have been shifted to genuine GIVE verbs, and then developed a dative use; or the GIVE<TAKE verb combines an allative form and becomes a compound genuine GIVE verb, then the compound form loses the allative element and, subsequently, the GIVE<TAKE verb itself becomes the dative marker.
3.4. Type IV: DAT = GIVE = PASS ≠ OM (4/30)
In Type IV languages, the dative markers are the same as passive markers, but different from the object markers. This type is mainly found in southern Hunan, such as in Hengshan (Xiang), Hengyang (Xiang), Youxian (Gan), Changning (Gan), and Suining (SWM).
(23) Passive construction |
小芳 | 得 | 爱婆 | 接走 | 哩。 |
ɕiau51faŋ44 | te44 | ŋəø11p’o213 | tɕie44-tsei51 | li |
XiaofangNAME | pass | grandmother | pick.up-be.away | sfp |
‘Xiaofang was picked up by her grandmother.’ |
(24) Differential object marking construction |
我 | 把 | 毛毛 | 放 | 到 | 床上 | 要得 | 不? |
ŋo11 | pa51 | mau213mau | faŋ11 | tau | t’aŋ213=ɕiaŋ | iau11te | pu |
1sg | om | baby | put | all | bed=on | ok | neg |
‘Is it okay if I put the baby on the bed?’ |
In all the six languages of this type, the GIVE
<TAKE verbs have developed both dative and passive uses. In this group, except for Suining, which uses
pa55 把 as a verb of giving, the other seven languages all use
DE 得. Both the dative marker and the passive marker share the same form with the GIVE
<TAKE verb. In
Section 4.2.2, we will take Hengyang as an example to discuss how
DE develops into a dative marker from GIVE
<TAKE in this type of languages.
3.5. Type V: DAT = BEN (6/30)
This type of language is very easy to differentiate from the other types because it uses a preverbal dative construction. Note that, for some of these languages, the dative marker may also have the same form as the object marker, but since it is a preverbal marker, and it is not derived from the GIVE verb, we do not classify these languages into Type II languages. There are six languages in our sample that share this pattern, and they are found in the northwest of Hunan. Their GIVE verbs are more diversified, and sometimes the etymological sources for the GIVE verbs are not so clear. For instance, Guiyang (Tuhua) uses ta45 带 or uã33 弯 as the GIVE verb, but when it forms a ditransitive construction, it has to use [ta45 + R + uã33 + T]; Fenghuang (SWM) and Jishou (SWM) use fən55 分 as the GIVE verb, Waxiang (unclassified) uses tɤ55 得, Tasha (SWM) uses ko24 过, and Changde (SWM) uses pa21 把.
For the six languages of Type V, the dative markers have not developed from the GIVE verbs. The main ditransitive constructions for them are formed on the basis of the benefactive construction. Take Jishou, for instance.
(25) Jishou (SWM; Li 2002, p. 318) |
Benefactive construction |
医生 | 倒 | 帮 | 他 | 看 | 病? |
i55sən55 | tau | paŋ55 | t’a55 | k’an35 | pin35 |
doctor | prog | ben | 3sg | see | illness |
‘The doctor is treating him.’ |
(26) Dative construction |
我 | 一 | 到 | 学校, | 就 | 帮 | 屋里 | 打 |
ŋo42 | i11 | tau35 | ɕio11ɕiau35 | tɕiəu35 | paŋ5 | u11li | ta42 |
1SG | once | arrive | school | then | dat | home | make |
了 | 个 | 电话。 | | | | | |
lə | ko | tian35xua3 | | | | | |
pfv | clf | phone.call | | | | | |
‘As soon as I got to school, I called home.’ |
The verbs that are used in the benefactive construction are transitive verbs, but they do not necessarily indicate transfer (
Zhu 1979). The argument introduced by the benefactive marker is a beneficiary. As we can see from (25), the verb is ‘to see (the patient), to treat (the illness)’, and 3rd person singular is the beneficiary, while for the dative construction, the verbs are either intrinsically ditransitive verbs that express transfer or verbs of saying that concern an addressee. As shown in (26), the verb is ‘to call’, and the preverbal argument is an addressee.
In this group of languages, dative markers are developed from preverbal benefactive markers and have little to do with GIVE verbs in the given language. Four of these languages can use
GEN 跟 as a preverbal dative marker, and one uses
GEI 给. In
Section 4.3, we will discuss the grammaticalization path of
kai55 跟 in Waxiang as an example.
3.6. Interim Summary: The Areal Distribution
Except for Xiangxiang and Lianyuan, which we exclude from the Type III languages, there are other three languages in our sample that do not fit into any types that we have classified. They are three Xiang varieties: Loudi, Xinhua, and Chenxi. In Loudi, the dative marker is another verb of giving
sɿ5 赐, while the GIVE
<TAKE verb that is used in the ditransitive construction is
nõ44 拿, which can also be used as a differential object marker. The postverbal dative construction in Loudi can be templated as [
nõ44 + T+
sɿ5 + R], and the passive marker is the compound form
nõ44sɿ5. Note that in Modern Chinese, the compound form
bǎyǔ 把与 which combines a TAKE verb and a former GIVE verb has also existed (
Mao 2022).
sɿ5 no longer has a verbal use in Loudi, which suggests that it is probably a GIVE verb belonging to an older layer. In Xinhua at an earlier stage, the dative marker is
læ13 来, and it is different from the GIVE verb
lɔ21 拿or the allative marker
tɔ45 到. In some varieties of Wu, 来 is identical to the allative marker that is also used as dative marker (
Chin 2010). It is possible that
læ13 was the allative marker in Xinhua before, but since we do not have any record in the literature, we cannot include Xinhua into Type I language. In Chenxi, the GIVE verb and the dative marker are both
ko324 过, producing the dative construction is [
ko324 + T+
ko324 + R]. At the same time, the double object construction is also attested in Chenxi as [
ko324 + R + T], but the detailed uses of
ko324 and its developments cannot be found in the literature, meaning that we cannot draw a credible conclusion as to how it developed the dative use. So, in our discussion, we exclude these three languages.
To conclude, we classify the languages in our sample into five main groups according to the dative constructions and the polyfunctionality of dative markers. The Type I languages have a dative marker that is identical to the allative marker, and they are mainly found in northeastern Hunan. In the Type II, III, and IV languages, the dative markers have the same form as the GIVE verbs. The dative markers in Type II languages are identical to their differential object makers. There was probably a compound GIVE verb in the history for these languages. As for the Type III languages, they are spread over central and southern Hunan. Their dative markers share the same form as the passive markers and the differential object markers. This pattern can be explained by two possible grammaticalization pathways. The type IV languages are mostly found in southern Hunan, and their dative markers are the same as the passive markers but different from the object markers. The Type V languages have various sources for GIVE verbs, and their main type of ditransitive constructions is the preverbal dative constructions. They are found in northwestern Hunan.
5. Conclusions
In this paper, we investigated the dative markers of 30 Sinitic languages in the Hunan Province, including 14 Xiang varieties, five Gan varieties, seven SWM varieties, one Hakka variety, two Tuhua varieties, and one unclassified Sinitic language in the Hunan Province. Five patterns have been observed, and the patterns show a regular distribution according to their geographic locations instead of the affiliations. We also identified three main grammaticalization pathways behind the patterns. See
Table 3 for the summary of the pathways behind each type of pattern.
For the Type II, III, and IV languages, the dative markers have the same form as their GIVE<TAKE verbs, but it concerns two different grammaticalization pathways. One is that the original TAKE verb combines with an allative element and this compound form becomes a genuine GIVE verb, which can become a dative marker to introduce the R argument, then the allative dropped from the compound form and left the original TAKE verb as a dative marker. The second pathway is that the TAKE verb first became a genuine GIVE verb through relexicalization, then the dative use was developed from the genuine GIVE verb. Type III languages may involve these two different pathways.
We can see that the most used dative marker is DE 得 (attested in seven languages) and BA 把 (found in nine languages). The dative use of DE in northern Hunan is developed from its allative use, while in southern Hunan, DE grammaticalized into dative marker from the GIVE verb use. BA as a GIVE verb is widely spread in Hunan, but in northern Hunan, BA only gained the semantic meaning of giving and cannot be really considered as a GIVE verb which can take an R argument; however, in the south, the GIVE use of BA becomes mature: it can not only be used as a dative marker to introduce the recipient, but also develops a passive use. We claim that in the Hunan Province, from north to south, BA is gradually shifting from TAKE to GIVE.
Finally, we remarked on a pattern that might be induced by language contact from Tujia, i.e., in northwestern Hunan; different from other Sinitic languages spoken in Hunan, four languages in our data tend to use a preverbal dative construction to encode transfer, and the source for the dative marker is the benefactive marker.
Hunan has been regarded as a transitional zone in
Chappell (
2015) by three constructions: differential object marking, passive, and comparative constructions. How to refine the linguistic areas there relies on other features. Our examination of the distribution and origin of patterns with dative markers helps the further finer classification of the Sinitic languages in Hunan, as well as to probe the historical layers and developments for those languages. In addition, double object constructions as well as GIVE verbs have long been important in the research for linguistic geography, e.g., (
Hashimoto 1976;
Szeto 2019). However, the literature has not explored much the development of the dative markers. We hope this paper will be an important addition to the exploration of dative marking patterns by combining dative markers with GIVE verbs, allative markers, passive markers, and differential object markers.
Further extensive inquiry and investigation into the patterns of dative markers will undoubtedly serve to rigorously test and refine the grammaticalization chains outlined in our analysis, not only for other Sinitic languages and regions but also for broader linguistic contexts.