Next Article in Journal
Erratum: Del Barrio, E. et al. From Active Aging to Active Citizenship: The Role of (Age) Friendliness. Social Sciences, 2018, 7, 134
Previous Article in Journal
Gender Equality in Europe and the Effect of Work-Family Balance Policies on Gender-Role Attitudes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive-Based E-Learning Design for Older Adults

by
Marios A. Pappas
*,
Eleftheria Demertzi
,
Yannis Papagerasimou
,
Lefteris Koukianakis
,
Nikitas Voukelatos
and
Athanasios Drigas
Institute of Informatics & Telecommunications, N.C.S.R. “Demokritos”, 15310 Agia Paraskevi, Greece
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2019, 8(1), 6; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8010006
Submission received: 1 November 2018 / Revised: 17 December 2018 / Accepted: 17 December 2018 / Published: 4 January 2019

Abstract

:
Aging is highly correlated with a decline in cognitive abilities. Information and communication technologies are nowadays increasingly used for knowledge acquisition, education, cognitive development, etc. Older adults should be prepared to adopt these technologies and take advantage of their capabilities. The purpose of this study was to analyze the cognitive profile of older adults in order to identify the ways that they learn, as well as to analyze older adults’ attitudes, to aid in the development of an e-learning platform adapted to their needs. The sample of the study consisted of 103 older adults, aged 55+, from Greece. According to their responses, older adults seemed to prefer e-learning modules that presented the educational content step-by-step and contained practice questions and examples. In addition, respondents had positive attitudes toward the existence of assessment tests for after the completion of each module. Finally, the utilization of explanatory videos and special graphics in the modules was imperative, according to older adults’ preferences.

1. Introduction

The improvement of the quality of life of older adults in an aging society is one of the main priorities of researchers in recent years. Aging is highly correlated with psychological changes and especially with a decline in various cognitive procedures (Baudouin et al. 2009). There is evidence that after the fourth decade of a person’s life, cognitive skills stop improving and gradually decline (Clark et al. 2006). Most older adults have impaired cognitive control, which is related to the slump of the prefrontal brain regions (Mather and Carstensen 2005). In addition, short-term memory performance in healthy individuals is positively correlated with gray matter volume (Taki et al. 2011).
It has been reported that more than 50% of individuals aged 60 and over are in danger of cognitive decline. At the same time, the limited amount of education for older adults is considered a risk factor for dementia (Ball et al. 2002). Thus, education during late adulthood could provide protection against dementia (Ardila et al. 2000). Deficits on executive function as well as attention deficits could increase the risk of future falls for older adults (Mirelman et al. 2012). Furthermore, adults show high levels of anxiety as they get older, as they are afraid of the possible consequences of aging (Hertzog et al. 2008). Stress-related factors, as well as environmental factors could affect cognitive health during aging (Scott et al. 2015).
Information and communication technologies (ICTs) have significantly contributed to the improvement of people’s everyday activities. As in other sectors, such as education, ICTs are integrated in order to support, enhance and optimize the delivery of knowledge. In recent years, there has been a variety of computerized training programs, which target the cognitive abilities of older adults. These intervention programs are considered more cost effective and can be used anytime and in any place, without specific instruction (Kueider et al. 2012). In addition, the adoption of ICTs in education has opened new horizons for older adults as e-learning can positively affect their social and political life and generally enhance their self-development. The purpose of this study was to identify specific characteristics for the design of a cognitive-based e-learning platform for older adults. For this reason, this study aimed to analyze the familiarization of older adults with ICTs, their cognitive characteristics, as well as their attitudes towards the learning approach used.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Cognition during Aging

Neuroimaging studies, including diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) or voxel-based morphometry (VBM), provide evidence of structural age-related brain changes (Freiherr et al. 2013). Age-related changes in human brain function are considered factors of lower activation in specific brain regions of older adults (Kramer et al. 2004). Normal aging is related to changes in the frontal lobes (frontal lobe hypothesis), which negatively affect executive function in late adulthood (Dahlin et al. 2008). Executive function, which is responsible for inhibition, shifting and updating, has been related to fall risk in older adults (Springer et al. 2006). In addition, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies provide evidence of age-related changes in white matter, the brain regions which affect executive functioning (Bugos et al. 2007). There is evidence that executive function could affect the self-care capacity of older adults (Insel et al. 2006). Recent studies have shown that every day physical activities, such as basic walking tasks, are associated with poor executive function among older adults (Ble et al. 2005; Coppin et al. 2006; Erickson and Kramer 2009). The results of a study with 909 well-functioning older adults in the US revealed that poor performance in executive function, as well as in verbal memory, could predict gait speed decline (Watson et al. 2010). The study of Ball et al. (2007) revealed that the speed of process training via computer-based nonverbal exercises could improve executive function in older adults. In addition, the study of McAuley et al. (2011) revealed that training of self-efficacy in older adults could improve executive function levels and, especially, task-coordination and inhibition. A study of 128 participants (mean age 69.3 years) revealed that intervention based on helping elementary school students with academic and behavioral issues could improve the executive function and memory of the elderly (Carlson et al. 2008).
Recent studies support the hypothesis that older adults show significantly lower performance on attention-dependent tasks in comparison with younger adults (Gola et al. 2012, 2013). According to Clapp et al. (2011), it is more likely for older adults to get distracted compared to younger adults, especially when multitasking. This susceptibility to distraction could lead to decreased performance in older adults. There is also evidence that visual attention impairment in older adults is associated with mobility problems (Owsley and McGwin 2004). In addition, the promotion of selective attention for the elderly could contribute to fall prevention (Chen et al. 2012). A study of 242 older adults (mean age 75.6 years) showed that brain plasticity-based computerized training containing auditory information processing could improve the attentional skills of the participants (Smith et al. 2009).
Compared to younger individuals, older adults often presented a decline in higher-order cognitive abilities, especially in working memory (Berry et al. 2010; Buschkuehl et al. 2008). Working memory (WM) is the ability to temporarily store and manipulate data for a short period of time (Zinke et al. 2014). In addition, a lack of encoding strategies in late adulthood as well as a lack of environmental support could cause a decline in episodic memory (Bherer et al. 2013). Training studies revealed that the working memory performance of older adults could be improved with appropriate interventions, indicating the existence of brain plasticity in aging (Borella et al. 2010). Furthermore, there is evidence that working memory training can also improve cognitive skills given that there is correlation between working memory capacity and fluid intelligence, as well as with executive functioning (Richmond et al. 2011).

2.2. E-Learning for Older Adults

The process of learning is understood as an internal one, controlled by the learners themselves. This process includes intellectual, emotional, and physiological functions. Learners are not seen as empty containers ready to be filled with facts. Learning is seen as a dynamic process, based on the learning objectives that can be accomplished and the learning experiences, which are defined as interactions between learners and their environments. E-learning, which is considered a relatively new and untraditional instruction method (Nasr 2010), has facilitated educational processes for people around the globe (El-Soud et al. 2010). Originally, the term ‘e-learning’ (electronic learning) derived from advertising industries and described learning arrangements and scenarios where information or communication technology was used to support the learning process or to provide learning materials and contents. According to Berners-Lee et al. (2001), the power of the Internet, and thus Internet-based activities such as e-learning, lies with its universality. In today’s transition to an information society, new methods of learning emerge and the provision for equal access for all individuals is imperative (Bakaev et al. 2008). Nevertheless, and despite the fact that information and communication technologies can have an important influence on the life of the elderly (Vandebosch et al. 2005), it appears that older adults lag as far as digital skills, Internet access, and engagement in online activities (e-learning, etc.) are concerned (Quan-Haase et al. 2016).
Bearing in mind that the proportion of elderly adults is increasing in countries throughout the world, many challenges are emerging in regards to the elderly and the ability to provide them with equal opportunities. One of these challenges, aimed at social inclusion, concerns the connection of older adults with the rest of today’s society through information technology (Lenić et al. 2013). Barriers blocking older adults to participate in online learning activities appear to be the same as those that prevent them from using computers in general (Notess and Lorenzen-Huber 2007). Individuals that make small or no use of technology are at risk of social exclusion and loss of participation in and interaction with society (Pecino et al. 2012). Advanced age is often associated with the phenomenon of the “digital divide” (Rice and Katz 2003; Vicente and López 2006), which induces inequalities concerning access, use and intensity of use of ICTs (Jackson et al. 2008; Torres et al. 2011).
New technologies can improve older adults’ autonomy and quality of life by providing opportunities for them to develop and maintain social relationships, to have access to services and care, to improve their lifelong learning habits, and to entertain themselves (Tsang 2012). The elderly are able to acquire new knowledge and to develop new skills, including ICT skills that will help them participate in current innovative processes (Repetto and Trentin 2008). In fact, contrary to popular belief, older adults are positive about their capablility of using computers (González et al. 2015) and tend to adopt new technology when they feel it is beneficial to their way of life (Kok et al. 2012; Rogers 2003). As Wellman and Haythornthwaite (2008) have argued, the Internet, and thus Internet-based activities such as e-learning, is not a standalone, separate sphere but rather part of the everyday routines and regular behaviors of its users. As a result, adoption is not binary (Quan-Haase et al. 2016; DiMaggio and Hargittai 2001) but rather it depends on the characteristics, behaviors, habits, and interests of the learners, which although commonly referred to as “the elderly” or “older adults” cannot be considered a homogenous group (Vandebosch et al. 2005).
Online learning can play an important role in helping older adults become integrated with the rest of society and, as they do, their use of e-learning will gradually increase (Githens 2007). The key conditions for older adults to engage with ICTs, and thus consent to e-learning, may be identified as having some practical computer skills that benefit their lifestyles and overcoming the belief that computers are too complex and not for the elderly (Bakaev et al. 2008). This can be done by acquiring basic ICT knowledge and gaining self-confidence. Understanding and addressing the challenges faced by older adults acquiring computer and Internet-related skills is a necessary prerequisite for designing an effective instructional intervention (Wood et al. 2010).
Older adults usually have great time availability that, along with the absence of spatial and temporal constraints, characterize online learning activities and allow high flexibility in their participation (Repetto and Trentin 2008). There are obvious advantages of e-learning for the elderly such as overcoming physical remoteness, providing up to date and constantly available learning, making educational activities less expensive, and providing a more individualized (personalized) educational approach to students (Bakaev et al. 2008). The latter is particularly important, given that older adults usually need more time and repetition to acquire knowledge, tend to make more mistakes, and generally need greater support through educational activities (Granger et al. 2002; Jones and Bayen 1998; Githens 2007). Repetto and Trentin (2008) highlight the fact that older adults have different response times, educational needs and motivation in online courses, compared to other users. These differences should be taken into account when designing the course material and choosing the learning strategies and tutoring style for an e-learning course addressing older adults.
If such parameters are taken into account, along with the acquisition of basic ICT skills, older adults will undoubtedly become participants of e-learning programs. Although some view older adult learning as entertainment or a leisure activity, Fisher and Wolf (2000) highlight the need to move beyond such purposes and create an educational model that contributes to greater societal good through self-empowerment, self-control, and self-determination. In this way, distance learning programs for older adults can help foster personal growth, civic engagement, and social action and inclusion (Githens 2007).

3. Method

3.1. Procedure

The purpose of this study was to investigate the learning profile of older adults in order to design a cognitive-based e-learning platform adapted to their needs. For this reason, this study aims to analyze the way that older adults learn, their familiarization with ICTs, and their attitudes toward the different learning modules presented to them. During the first phase, the literature review revealed the state-of-the-art use of ICTs and the cognitive profile of older adults. Based on these findings, we designed the questionnaire “Older Adults and Information and Communication Technologies”. The questionnaire was sent to the students enrolled in the Master of Education, Specialization in ICT and Special Education, of the National Center for Scientific Research, Demokritos. Students were asked to forward the questionnaire to their family members over 55 years or to guide them as to how to answer it, in case they had no previous online experience. All valid data collected during the online survey were used for analysis, with the use of the IBM SPSS Statistics Subscription software (Armonk, New York, NY, USA), version 1.0.0.800.

3.2. Participants

The sample of this study consisted of 103 older adults from Greece who answered the online questionnaire on their own or with support of a family member or project team member. A total of 78.6% of the participants surveyed were between 55 and 64 years old, 18.4% were aged between 65 and 74 years old, and only a mere 2.9% were 75 years old or older (Table 1). In addition, two thirds (65%) of the participants were female and the majority live in a civil center (76.7%). In terms of their health conditions, participants had a little difficulty with their vision (mean = 2.068, SD = 0.855) and little or no difficulty with their hearing (mean = 1.379, SD = 0.756) or touch (mean = 1.146, SD = 0.550). Regarding the educational status of the participants, 71.8% had graduated from higher educational institutes while 15.5% did not continue their studies after primary school. The majority of participants had moderate knowledge of the English language, in terms of reading (mean = 3.262, SD = 1.462), writing (mean = 3.019, SD = 1.400), and speaking skills (mean = 2.961, SD = 1.386). Table 2 shows that 57.3% of the participants were still in the labor force, as opposed to 31.1% who were retired and 11.7% who were unemployed. Finally, regarding the type of employment the majority (53.4%) were civil servants while only 11.7% were private employees. A possible limitation of the study is the fact that most participants were less than 65 years old, as well as the fact that most participants were graduates of higher educational institutions.

3.3. Measures

The method used for the present study included a literature review, containing relative articles from the last 15 years, as well as an online survey by means of a questionnaire. The literature review was based on published studies from IEEE, dblp, ERIC, Elsevier, CiteSeerX and PsycINFO. For the online survey, an online questionnaire platform was used in order to develop a questionnaire exploring the perception, attitudes and level of familiarity of older adults (55+) with ICTs. The questionnaire (Appendix A), which consisted of 24 questions in Greek (ten multiple-choice, nine Likert scale, four dichotomous and one check-box), was divided into the following categories:
  • Demographic Questions
  • Familiarization to Information and Communication Technologies
  • Learning Approach
  • Specifications of e-learning modules

4. Results

The structural validity of the questionnaire was guaranteed by measuring the strength of correlation between individual results. For this, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated for questions related to participants’ attitudes toward the preferred learning approach to the e-learning modules (Table 3), as well as for questions related to the specifications of the modules (Table 4). As shown, all correlation coefficients were moderate to high and statistically significant at the 0.01 level, proving the validity of the questionnaire used for data collection. In addition, for the internal reliability of the questionnaire, Cronbach’s Alpha coefficient was calculated. For the eight items related to the preferred learning approaches, Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.892, while for the six items related to the specifications of the e-learning modules it was 0.895. The overall Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.926, indicating the reliability of the questionnaire.
Questions 9 through 21 targeted the familiarization of older adults with information and communication technologies (Table 5). According to the results, participants were more confidence in their use of simple mobile phones (mean = 3.952, SD = 0.984) and personal computers or laptops (mean = 3.573, SD = 1.411), in comparison to the use of smartphones (mean = 3.301, SD = 1.441) and tablets (mean = 272, SD = 1.422). Older adults seemed to be more confident when they used a keyboard (mean = 3.971, SD = 1.256) and a mouse (mean = 3.845, SD = 1.239) rather than using a touch screen (mean = 3.678, SD = 1.239). The majority of the participants (79.6%) used the Internet every day, while only a mere 4.9% did not use the Internet at all. However, only 41.7% of the participants reported feeling confident while using the Internet.
Most participants (35.9%) learned how to use information and communication technologies from their family environment, while 29.1% of the participants were self-taught. It must be mentioned that almost 20% of the participants actually attended ICT classes in an ICT school. In addition, the majority of the participants (77.7%) stated that they have an e-mail account, while, notably, 9.7% stated that they are not sure whether they have an e-mail account or not. Moreover, 73.8% of the older adults participating in the online survey had experience in submitting electronic forms, while almost 4% stated that they were not sure if they had or not. Finally, the vast majority (91.3%) of the participants had used an online search engine at least once.
According to their responses, older adults seemed to spend more time searching for information on the Internet (mean = 3.398, SD = 1.174), sending and receiving e-mails (mean = 2.796, SD = 1.424), and reading the news (mean = 2.767, SD = 1.139) while online. In contrast, they did not spend much time e-shopping (mean = 1.757, SD = 1.071) or e-banking (mean = 1.971, SD = 1.256). In reference to mobile applications, older adults felt more confident when they used mobile devices for calling (mean = 4.078, SD = 1.326), sending and receiving text messages (mean = 3.660, SD = 1.525), checking the time (mean = 3.553, SD = 1.551), and for web browsing (mean = 3.495, SD = 1.553).
Table 6 represents the confidence level of the participants while using the Internet, in terms of their gender, age, residence and educational level.
A chi-square test of independence was performed, in order to examine the impact of gender, age, residence and educational level on older adults’ confidence towards the use of ICTs. The relation between the level of confidence using ICTs and the educational level was significant ( x 2 ( 9 ) = 26.990 ,   p = 0.001 ). More educated older adults were more confident with ICTs than their peers with lower educational levels.
In terms of the learning approach used for the design of the e-learning modules (Table 7), older adults were more positive regarding the step-by-step presentation of the educational content (mean = 4.301, SD = 0.895), the existence of exercises after each module (mean = 4.291, SD = 0.788), and the existence of assessment tests after the completion of each e-learning module (mean = 4.194, SD = 0.852).
One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied in order to identify which factors could significantly affect participants’ attitudes. According to the results, there was a significant relationship between the participants’ educational level and their attitude towards the existence of specialized/autonomous modules in the e-learning platform F ( 3 , 99 ) = 3.527 ,   p = 0.018 ,   η 2 = 0.097 ) . Older adults who had graduated from high school had the most positive attitudes (mean = 4.182, SD = 0.603), while participants who had graduated from primary school had the least positive attitudes (mean = 3.375, SD = 1.147). In addition, the type of employment seemed to significantly affect older adults’ attitudes regarding the learning approach used ( F ( 4 , 98 ) = 3.048 ,   p = 0.021 ,   η 2 = 0.111 ) . Freelancers seemed to express more positive attitudes towards specialized/autonomous modules (mean = 4.500, SD = 0.707) compared to private employees (mean = 3.250, SD = 1.138).
There was also a statistically significant relationship between the attitude, the level of confidence while using the Internet, the existence of autonomous modules ( F ( 3 , 99 ) = 4.471 ,   p = 0.005 ,   η 2 = 0.119 ), the grouping of relevant modules F ( 3 , 99 ) = 4.299 ,   p = 0.007 ,   η 2 = 0.115 ) , and the continuity between modules ( F ( 3 , 99 ) = 2.938 ,   p = 0.038 ,   η 2 = 0.082 ) . In particular, participants who were confident about the use of the Internet had more positive attitudes regarding the existence of specialized/autonomous modules M ( 4.349 ,   SD = 0.897 ) , the grouping of relevant modules ( mean = 4.279 ,   SD = 0.826 ) , and the continuity between modules ( M = 4.372 ,   SD = 0.817 ) . This was in comparison with their peers who did not feel confident at all while using the Internet (mean = 3.222, SD = 1.394; mean = 3.444, SD = 1.333; and mean = 3.667, SD = 1.414, respectively).
In reference to the specifications of the modules, older adults were more positive towards the existence of examples in the modules (mean = 4.524, SD = 0.778), as well as to the utilization of special graphics (mean = 4.447, SD = 0.905) and explanatory videos (mean = 4.437, SD = 0.788). The results revealed a significant relationship between the work situation and the attitudes towards the existence of revision after the completion of a module ( F ( 2 , 100 ) = 3.914 ,   p = 0.023 ,   η 2 = 0.073 ) . Unemployed participants had more positive attitudes (mean = 4.833, SD = 0.389) compared to their peers in the labor force (mean = 4.186, SD = 1.042).

5. Discussion

The present study concerns literature and field study regarding older adults and new technologies, with emphasis on e-learning. The principle objective was to understand their cognitive characteristics and attitudes, as well as the way in which they learn best, in order to design and develop a suitable e-learning platform adapted to their needs. The results of the online survey targeting older adults, together with those already present in the literature review, allowed the following considerations.
As far as familiarization with devices is concerned, older adults seemed to be more confident using more “traditional” devices such as simple mobile phones and personal computers/laptops compared to smartphones and tablets. As a result, they also prefer using keyboards and mice over using touch screens. However, there are studies indicating that the use of touch screens instead of traditional input devices could reduce anxiety levels (Chung et al. 2010; Umemuro 2004). As far as mobiles are concerned, older adults mostly use them for calls, messages, checking the time, and browsing the Internet.
Concerning access to the Internet, the research indicated that only a minority of the participants does not use the Internet at all. Nevertheless, and aside from the fact that the vast majority appears to be online on a daily basis, less than half of them felt safe or confident while using Internet-based services. These findings are in accordance with recent studies on the use of the Internet by older adults (Choi and DiNitto 2013; Gatto and Tak 2008; Zickuhr and Madden 2012).
It appears that most participants learned how to use ICTs from their family environment, followed by the ones that were self-taught, whereas only one in five had actual computer and ICT training.
As far as the activities they engage in when they go online, older adults usually spend time searching for information (Wagner et al. 2010). This agrees with the fact that the vast majority of older adults state that they have used an online search engine at least once. Most of the participants also state that they have an email account and, in fact, sending and receiving emails was the second most common activity engaged in, followed by reading the news. Finally, older adults seem to still be very cautious with the use of e-shopping and e-banking (Xiong and Mathews 2005).
It appears that the level of education of older adults had a significant effect on their attitude towards use of and familiarization with ICTs. Higher educated participants seemed to be more confident compared to their peers with lower educational level, however, age, gender and residence did not have a significant effect.
As far as the learning approach of the e-learning course was concerned, participants considered the most important features to be the step-by-step presentation of the educational content and the existence of exercises and assessment at the end of each e-learning module.
The educational level as well as the type of employment of the older adult had a significant effect on their interest in specialized/autonomous e-learning modules. Participants with higher educational level seemed once again to be more confident and positive towards the autonomous modules compared to their peers with a lower education. The same applied to freelancers when compared with other types of employees.
Finally, the level of confidence while using the Internet had a significant effect on older adults’ attitudes towards the existence of autonomous/specialized modules, as well as the grouping of relevant modules. Participants that felt more confident using the Internet in general were more positive towards the latter compared to their peers who did not feel confident in either.

6. Conclusions

The present study attempted to investigate the cognitive functioning, profile, learning needs, and familiarization of older adults with ICTs. The aim of this study was to reach a final didactical approach that could be adapted to the target group’s learning needs. Older adults were more accustomed to using personal computers and/or laptops over mobile devices. In fact, in response to one question they clearly stated that they preferred using a keyboard and a mouse over touch screens. Hence, it was considered a certainty that the e-learning platform should be fully responsive and the creation of a mobile app should be considered.
The literature review revealed that the attention skills of older adults appeared very low, especially when multitasking. From this, the need for a simple graphical user interface (GCI) design, without using bright colors and excessive graphics, was established. In addition, a straightforward navigation design for the online modules would contribute to the elimination of distractors. Studies have revealed that aging is also correlated with a decline in cognitive procedures and, especially, in working memory. With aging, the brain changes, thus affecting executive function. However, it has been proven that participation in lifelong learning activities, along with intervention, can improve the cognitive skills of people and, consequently, their memory. Therefore, the e-learning modules should be short and comprehensive, breaking the educational material into small units. In addition, older adults find it hard to process large texts due to their deteriorating cognitive skills and to their growing anxiety, which comes as a consequence of age. Considering this, the learning modules should not contain much text in order to motivate older learners.
Studies have revealed that due to their lack of confidence and their growing anxiety, older adults prefer a more self-directed (or personalized-learning) approach (Reimann et al. 2012). This should be set in an informal learning setting with a flexible curriculum where they can follow the learning modules of their choice and at their own pace. An e-learning setting can provide such a setting by giving adequate time and space to repeat and absorb the newly acquired material, which is available anytime and anywhere. That being said, older adults sometimes appreciate the help of a peer or a teacher. Although they may be reluctant at first, studies show that older adults tend to make mistakes and at some stage will need support (Alonso et al. 2005), whether from a teacher or from a peer learner. An e-learning environment, which offers communication and social capabilities, can provide such a setting.
In regards to assessment procedures, the existence of exercises after the end of an online course is a practice that adults welcome, not so much to be marked but in order to understand their learning needs. In fact, they wish to have very clear learning goals set before they take a course. Therefore, an e-learning environment that provides an automated assessment system for self-evaluation, based on the successful completion of certain criteria, such as quizzes, comprehension exercises, practical questions, assignments, etc. could prove useful. It was clearly demonstrated in the field research that older adults showed positive attitudes towards the incorporation of practical exercises and assessment methods in general, both during and after the completion of an online module, which clearly shows that they prefer a more active and experience-based learning approach rather than a passive one. They preferred receiving feedback but not necessarily being marked. The reception of feedback can clearly contribute to better understanding of the material and the self-diagnosis of their learning needs. From the above, and based on the survey results, the conclusion that was drawn is that the target group prefers to spend time on practical-oriented learning material with examples and practical questions. In other words, they prefer to learn something that will be useful in their everyday lives and, apart from learning the theoretical background, they wish to put what they learned into practice. Therefore, learning content should not be isolated from its practical context. In order to achieve this, exercises that allow the target group to practice the newly acquired skills and knowledge should be provided.
Summing up, the learning modules hosted in the e-learning platform should be presented in a clear and logical way. Given the aforementioned cognitive limitations of older adults and based on the survey feedback, the overall conclusion is that the learning modules should have specific learning goals, be short in length, using mostly multimedia over text. Step-by-step presentation is the preferred way to provide educational content to the target group in order to ease the learning process as much as possible.

Author Contributions

All authors participated in research development and dissemination of research results.

Funding

We acknowledge support of this work by the project SYNTELESIS “Innovative Technologies and Applications based on the Internet of Things (IoT) and the Cloud Computing” (MIS 5002521) which is implemented under the “Action for the Strategic Development on the Research and Technological Sector”, funded by the Operational Programme “Competitiveness, Entrepreneurship and Innovation” (NSRF 2014-2020) and co-financed by Greece and the European Union (European Regional Development Fund).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Questionnaire “Older Adults and New Technologies”

Appendix A.1. Demographics

Question 1: How old are you?
  • 55–64
  • 65–74
  • 75+
Question 2: Gender
  • Male
  • Female
Question 3: Residence
  • Civil Center
  • Providence
Question 4: Education Level
  • I have not graduated from primary school
  • Primary School
  • High School
  • Higher Education
Question 5: Knowledge of English
Not at AllIncompleteModerateGoodVery Good
12345
Speaking
Reading
Writing
Question 6: Current Work Situation
  • Employee
  • Retired
  • Unemployed
Question 7: Employment
  • Civil Servant
  • Private Employee
  • Freelancer
  • Housekeeping
  • Other
Question 8: You have difficulties in
NeverA Little BitModerateMuchVery Much
12345
Vision
Hearing
Touch

Appendix A.2. Familiarization with Information and Communication Technologies

Question 9: Which of the following types of technology do you have access to? (Choose all that apply)
  • PC/Laptop
  • Mobile Phone
  • Smartphone
  • Tablet
  • None
Question 10: How would you rate your skills on the following?
Not at allIncompleteModerateGoodVery Good
12345
PC/Laptop
Tablet
Mobile Phone
Smartphone
Question 11: Which of the following devices would you like to learn to better use?
Not at allA Little BitModerateMuchVery Much
12345
PC/Laptop
Tablet
Smartphone
Question 12: Do you have someone to help you when you have questions while using the devices mentioned above?
  • Yes
  • No
Question 13: How often do you use the Internet?
  • Everyday
  • 2–3 times in a week
  • Once a week
  • 2–3 times a month
  • 2–3 times a year
  • Not at all
Question 14: How confident do you feel when using the Internet?
  • Not at all
  • Usually I need help
  • It depends on the use
  • I feel confident
Question 15: How did you learn to use ICTs?
  • Family Environment
  • Workplace
  • Computer School
  • Self-taught
Question 16: Do you have an e-mail account?
  • Yes
  • No
  • I am not sure
Question 17: Have you ever submitted an electronic form?
  • Yes
  • No
  • I am not sure
Question 18: Have you ever used an online search engine?
  • Yes
  • No
Question 19: How much time do you spend on the following online activities?
Not at AllA Little BitModerateMuchVery Much
12345
News
Internet Search
Communication
(Chat and Social Media)
E-Mail
Entertainment
E-Shopping
E-Learning
E-Banking
E-Government
Question 20: How confident do you feel while using the following mobile applications?
Not at AllA Little BitModerateMuchVery Much
12345
Calls
Text Messages
Web Browsing
E-Mail
Social Media
Games
Alarm Clock
Clock
Calculator
Reminders
Notes
Maps
Question 21: How confident do you feel when you use the following devices?
Not at AllA Little BitModerateMuchVery Much
12345
Keyboard
Touch Screens
Mouse

Appendix A.3. Learning Approach

Question 22: If you were attending an e-learning course, you would like:
Strongly DisagreeDisagreeUndecidedAgreeStrongly Agree
12345
Specialized/autonomous learning modules
Grouping relevant modules
Continuity between the modules
Step-by-step presentation of the modules
Exercises after each session
Questions during the sessions
My scores on each assessment available
Assessment after each session

Appendix A.4. Specifications of E-Learning Modules

Question 23: If you were attending an e-learning course, you would like:
Strongly DisagreeDisagreeUndecidedAgreeStrongly Agree
12345
Modules to be comprehensive and short
Examples
Descriptions for definitions and terms
Graphics
Explanatory videos
Revision after units
Question 24: Add a comment (Optional)
________________________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________________________
Thank You!

References

  1. Alonso, Fernando, Genoveva López, Daniel Manrique, and Jose M. Viñes. 2005. An instructional model for web-based e-learning education with a blended learning process approach. British Journal of Educational Technology 36: 217–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ardila, Alfredo, Feggy Ostrosky-Solis, Mónica Rosselli, and César Gómez. 2000. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: The complex effect of education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 15: 495–513. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  3. Bakaev, Maxim, Vladimir Ponomarev, and Larisa Prokhorova. 2008. E-learning and elder people: Barriers and benefits. Paper presented at the IEEE Region 8 International Conference on Computational Technologies in Electrical and Electronics Engineering (SIBIRCON 2008), Novosibirsk, Russia, July 21–25; pp. 110–13. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ball, Karlene, Daniel B. Berch, Karin F. Helmers, Jared B. Jobe, Mary D. Leveck, Michael Marsiske, John N. Morris, George W. Rebok, David M. Smith, Sharon L. Tennstedt, and et al. 2002. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288: 2271–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Ball, Karlene, Jerri D. Edwards, and Lesley A. Ross. 2007. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 62: 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Baudouin, Alexia, David Clarys, Sandrine Vanneste, and Michel Isingrini. 2009. Executive functioning and processing speed in age-related differences in memory: Contribution of a coding task. Brain and Cognition 71: 240–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Berners-Lee, Tim, James Hendler, and Ora Lassila. 2001. The semantic web. Scientific American 284: 34–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Berry, Anne S., Theodore P. Zanto, Wesley C. Clapp, Joseph L. Hardy, Peter B. Delahunt, Henry W. Mahncke, and Adam Gazzaley. 2010. The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older adults. PLoS ONE 5: E11537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Bherer, Louis, Kirk I. Erickson, and Teresa Liu-Ambrose. 2013. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. Journal of Aging Research 2013: 657508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Ble, Alesandro, Stefano Volpato, Giovanni Zuliani, Jack M. Guralnik, Stefania Bandinelli, Fulvio Lauretani, Bendetta Bartali, Cinzia Maraldi, Renato Fellin, and Luigi Ferrucci. 2005. Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: The InCHIANTI study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 410–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Borella, Erika, Barbara Carretti, Francesco Riboldi, and Rossana De Beni. 2010. Working memory training in older adults: Evidence of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychology and Aging 25: 767–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Bugos, Jennifer A., William M. Perlstein, Christina S. McCrae, Timothy S. Brophy, and Purvis H. Bedenbaugh. 2007. Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging and Mental Health 11: 464–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Buschkuehl, Martin, Susanne Jaeggi, Sara Hutchison, Pasqualina Perrig-Chiello, Christoph Däpp, Matthias Müller, Fabio Breil, Hans Hoppeler, and Walter J. Perrig. 2008. Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychology and Aging 23: 743–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Carlson, Michelle C., Jane S. Saczynski, George W. Rebok, Teresa Seeman, Thomas A. Glass, Sylvia McGill, James Tielsch, Kevin D. Frick, Joel Hill, and Linda P. Fried. 2008. Exploring the effects of an “everyday” activity program on executive function and memory in older adults: Experience Corps®. The Gerontologist 48: 793–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chen, Shang-Ti, I-Tsun Chiang, Eric Zhi-Feng Liu, and Maiga Chang. 2012. Effects of Improvement on Selective Attention: Developing Appropriate Somatosensory Video Game Interventions for Institutional-Dwelling Elderly with Disabilities. Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology-TOJET 11: 409–17. [Google Scholar]
  16. Choi, Namkee G., and Diana M. DiNitto. 2013. The digital divide among low-income homebound older adults: Internet use patterns, eHealth literacy, and attitudes toward computer/Internet use. Journal of Medical Internet Research 15: E93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chung, Min K., Dongjin Kim, Seokhee Na, and Donghun Lee. 2010. Usability evaluation of numeric entry tasks on keypad type and age. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 40: 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Clapp, Wesley C., Michael T. Rubens, Jasdeep Sabharwal, and Adam Gazzaley. 2011. Deficit in switching between functional brain networks underlies the impact of multitasking on working memory in older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 7212–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Clark, C. Richard, Robert H. Paul, Leanne M. Williams, Martijn Arns, Kamran Fallahpour, Carolyn Handmer, and Evian Gordon. 2006. Standardized assessment of cognitive functioning during development and aging using an automated touchscreen battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21: 449–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Coppin, Antonia K., Anne Shumway-Cook, Jane S. Saczynski, Kushang V. Patel, Alessandro Ble, Luigi Ferrucci, and Jack M. Guralnik. 2006. Association of executive function and performance of dual-task physical tests among older adults: Analyses from the InChianti study. Age and Ageing 35: 619–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dahlin, Erika, Lars Nyberg, Lars Bäckman, and Anna Stigsdotter Neely. 2008. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: Immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychology and Aging 23: 720–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. DiMaggio, Paul, and Eszter Hargittai. 2001. From the ‘digital divide’to ‘digital inequality’: Studying Internet use as penetration increases. Princeton: Center for Arts and Cultural Policy Studies, Woodrow Wilson School, Princeton University 4: 4–2. [Google Scholar]
  23. El-Soud, Mohammed Abo, A. E. Hassan, M. S. Kandil, and Samaa M. Shohieb. 2010. A proposed web based framework e-learning and dictionary system for deaf Arab students. IJECS 2828: 106401. [Google Scholar]
  24. Erickson, Kirk, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2009. Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults. British Journal of Sports Medicine 43: 22–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fisher, James C., and Mary Alice Wolf. 2000. Older adult learning. In Handbook of Adult and Continuing Education. Edited by Arthur L. Wilson and Elisabeth R. Hayes. Washington, DC: American Association for Adult and Continuing Education, pp. 480–92. [Google Scholar]
  26. Freiherr, Jessica, Johan N. Lundström, Ute Habel, and Kathrin Reetz. 2013. Multisensory integration mechanisms during aging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Gatto, Susan L., and Sunghee H. Tak. 2008. Computer, internet, and e-mail use among older adults: Benefits and barriers. Educational Gerontology 34: 800–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Githens, Rod P. 2007. Older adults and e-learning: Opportunities and barriers. Quarterly Review of Distance Education 8: 329. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gola, Mateusz, Jan Kamiński, Aneta Brzezicka, and Andrzej Wróbel. 2012. Beta band oscillations as a correlate of alertness—Changes in aging. International Journal of Psychophysiology 85: 62–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Gola, Mateusz, Mikołaj Magnuski, Izabela Szumska, and Andrzej Wróbel. 2013. EEG beta band activity is related to attention and attentional deficits in the visual performance of elderly subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology 89: 334–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. González, Antonio, María Paz Ramírez, and Vicente Viadel. 2015. ICT learning by older adults and their attitudes toward computer use. Current Gerontology and Geriatrics Research 2015: 849308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Granger, Colette A., Mary Leigh Morbey, Heather Lotherington, Ronald D. Owston, and Herbert H. Wideman. 2002. Factors contributing to teachers’ successful implementation of IT. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 18: 480–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hertzog, Christopher, Arthur F. Kramer, Robert S. Wilson, and Ulman Lindenberger. 2008. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9: 1–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Insel, Kathleen, Daniel Morrow, Barbara Brewer, and Aurelio Figueredo. 2006. Executive function, working memory, and medication adherence among older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 61: P102–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jackson, Linda A., Yong Zhao, Anthony Kolenic III, Hiram E. Fitzgerald, Rena Harold, and Alexander Von Eye. 2008. Race, gender, and information technology use: The new digital divide. CyberPsychology & Behavior 11: 437–42. [Google Scholar]
  36. Jones, Brett D., and Ute J. Bayen. 1998. Teaching older adults to use computers: Recommendations based on cognitive aging research. Educational Gerontology: An International Quarterly 24: 675–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kok, Adrian, Kate Williams, and Hui Yan. 2012. Seniors Skyping: A professional, academic, and community collaboration. The Journal of Community Informatics 8. Available online: http://ci-journal.org/index.php/ciej/article/view/896 (accessed on 4 January 2019).
  38. Kramer, Arthur F., Louis Bherer, Stanley J. Colcombe, Willie Dong, and William T. Greenough. 2004. Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 59: M940–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kueider, Alexandra M., Jeanine M. Parisi, Alden L. Gross, and George W. Rebok. 2012. Computerized cognitive training with older adults: A systematic review. PLoS ONE 7: E40588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lenić, Marija, Anita Kustura, Ida Jurković, and Nives Mikelić Preradović. 2013. Students for Seniors: Basic ICT Education for the Elderly. Paper presented at the 4th International Conference “The Future of Information Sciences: INFuture2013—Information Governance”, Zagreb, Croatia, November 6–8. [Google Scholar]
  41. Mather, Mara, and Laura L. Carstensen. 2005. Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. McAuley, Edward, Sean P. Mullen, Amanda N. Szabo, Siobhan M. White, Thomas R. Wójcicki, Emily L. Mailey, Neha P. Gothe, Erin A. Olson, Michelle Voss, Kirk Erickson, Ruchika Prakash, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2011. Self-regulatory processes and exercise adherence in older adults: Executive function and self-efficacy effects. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 41: 284–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mirelman, Anat, Talia Herman, Marina Brozgol, Moran Dorfman, Elliot Sprecher, Avraham Schweiger, Nir Giladi, and Jeffrey M. Hausdorff. 2012. Executive function and falls in older adults: New findings from a five-year prospective study link fall risk to cognition. PLoS ONE 7: E40297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Nasr, Mona M. 2010. An enhanced e-learning environment for Deaf/HOH pupils. Paper presented at the 2010 2nd International Conference on Computer Technology and Development (ICCTD), Cairo, Egypt, November 2–4; pp. 724–27. [Google Scholar]
  45. Notess, Mark, and Lesa Lorenzen-Huber. 2007. Online learning for seniors: Barriers and opportunities. ELearn 2007: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Owsley, Cynthia, and Gerald McGwin Jr. 2004. Association between visual attention and mobility in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 52: 1901–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Pecino, Roberto Martinez, María J. Lera, and Magdalena Martinez-Pecino. 2012. Active seniors and mobile phone interaction. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal 40: 875–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Quan-Haase, Anabel, Kim Martin, and Kathleen Schreurs. 2016. Interviews with digital seniors: ICT use in the context of everyday life. Information, Communication & Society 19: 691–707. [Google Scholar]
  49. Reimann, Ralph, Yvonne Prinzellner, Claus Rainer Michalek, Ulrich Haas, Thomas Guggenberger, Mogens Andersen, Pauline Mieleszko, Milan Slavik, Veronika Pfaffenberger, Eqward Pierzgalski, and et al. 2012. E-Learning: Didactical Recommendations and Quality Assurance: An Overview. Available online: https://www.uni-hohenheim.de/fileadmin/einrichtungen/ells/QA-documents/ELLS-eLearning-Didactical-Recommendation-final-version.pdf (accessed on 4 January 2019).
  50. Repetto, Manuela, and Guglielmo Trentin. 2008. ICT and lifelong learning for senior citizens. Journal of e-Learning and Knowledge Society 4: 189–98. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rice, Ronald E., and James E. Katz. 2003. Comparing internet and mobile phone usage: Digital divides of usage, adoption, and dropouts. Telecommunications Policy 27: 597–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Richmond, Lauren L., Alexandra B. Morrison, Jason M. Chein, and Ingrid R. Olson. 2011. Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychology and Aging 26: 813–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Rogers, E. M. 2003. The Diffusion of Innovation, 5th ed. London: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  54. Scott, Stacey B., Jennifer E. Graham-Engeland, Christopher G. Engeland, Joshua M. Smyth, David M. Almeida, Mindy J. Katz, Richard B. Lipton Jacqueline A. Mogle, Elizabeth Munoz, Nilam Ram, and Martin J. Sliwinski. 2015. The effects of stress on cognitive aging, physiology and emotion (ESCAPE) project. BMC Psychiatry 15: 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Smith, Glenn E., Patricia Housen, Kristine Yaffe, Ronald Ruff, Robert F. Kennison, Henry W. Mahncke, and Elizabeth M. Zelinski. 2009. A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: Results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 57: 594–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Springer, Shmuel, Nir Giladi, Chava Peretz, Galit Yogev, Ely S. Simon, and Jeffrey M. Hausdorff. 2006. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: The role of aging, falls, and executive function. Movement Disorders 21: 950–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Taki, Yasuyuki, Shigeo Kinomura, Kazunori Sato, Ryoi Goto, Kai Wu, Ryuta Kawashima, and Hiroshi Fukuda. 2011. Correlation between gray/white matter volume and cognition in healthy elderly people. Brain and Cognition 75: 170–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Torres Diaz, Juan Carlos, and Alfonso Infante Moro. 2011. Desigualdad digital en la universidad: Usos de Internet en Ecuador. Scientific Journal of Media Literacy 37: 81–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Tsang, Monique. 2012. Connecting and caring: Innovations for healthy ageing. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 90: 162–63. [Google Scholar]
  60. Umemuro, Hiroyuki. 2004. Lowering elderly Japanese users’ resistance towards computers by using touchscreen technology. Universal Access in the Information Society 3: 276–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Vandebosch, Heidi, Kathleen Beullens, and J. V. D. Bulck. 2005. Elderly and ICT: Scenarios for the future. In Digital Utopia in the Media: From Discourses to Facts. Barcelona: Facultat de Ciències de la Comunicacío Blanquerna, pp. 951–62. [Google Scholar]
  62. Vicente, María Rosalía, and Ana Jesús López. 2006. Patterns of ICT diffusion across the European Union. Economics Letters 93: 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Wagner, Nicole, Khaled Hassanein, and Milena Head. 2010. Computer use by older adults: A multi-disciplinary review. Computers in Human Behavior 26: 870–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Watson, N.L., C. Rosano, R. M. Boudreau, E. M. Simonsick, L. Ferrucci, K. Sutton-Tyrrell, S. E. Hardy, H. H. Atkinson, K. Yaffe, S. Satterfield, and et al. 2010. Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences 65: 1093–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wellman, Barry, and Caroline Haythornthwaite, eds. 2008. The Internet in Everyday Life. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  66. Wood, Eileen, Catherine Lanuza, Iuliana Baciu, Meagan MacKenzie, and Amanda Nosko. 2010. Instructional styles, attitudes and experiences of seniors in computer workshops. Educational Gerontology 36: 834–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Xiong, Liya, and Claire Mathews. 2005. Seniors and Electronic Banking. Paper presented at the 10th AIBF Banking and Finance Conference, Melbourne, Australia, September 29–30; pp. 29–30. [Google Scholar]
  68. Zickuhr, Kathryn, and Mary Madden. 2012. Older Adults and Internet Use. Pew Internet & American Life Project 6. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  69. Zinke, Katharina, Melanie Zeintl, Nathan S. Rose, Julia Putzmann, Andrea Pydde, and Matthias Kliegel. 2014. Working memory training and transfer in older adults: Effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Developmental Psychology 50: 304–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic data of the participants (n = 103).
Table 1. Demographic data of the participants (n = 103).
FrequencyPercentage (%)
Age
 55–648178.6
 65–741918.4
 75+32.9
Gender
 Male 3635
 Female6765
Residence
 Civil Center7976.7
 Providence2423.3
Table 2. Educational level and professional status of the participants.
Table 2. Educational level and professional status of the participants.
FrequencyPercentage (%)
Educational Level
 Not graduated from primary school21.9
 Primary school1615.5
 High school1110.7
 Higher education7471.8
Work Situation
 Working5957.3
 Retired 3231.1
 Unemployed1211.7
Type of Employment
 Civil Servant5553.4
 Private Employee1211.7
 Freelancer109.7
 Housekeeping1615.5
 Other109.7
Table 3. Bivariate correlation between the questions related to the learning approach.
Table 3. Bivariate correlation between the questions related to the learning approach.
12345678
1. Autonomous learning modules1.000
2. Grouping relevant modules0.429 **1.000
3. Continuity between modules0.620 **0.658 **1.000
4. Step by step presentation 0.587 **0.521 **0.636 **1.000
5. Exercises after each session0.563 **0.379 **0.635 **0.667 **1.000
6. Questions during the modules0.527 **0.319 **0.581 **0.621 **0.832 **1.000
7. Scores on each assessment0.563 **0.388 **0.460 **0.446 **0.552 **0.542 **1.000
8. Assessment after each session0.511 **0.364 **0.423 **0.488 **0.543 **0.487 **0.452 **1.000
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level.
Table 4. Bivariate correlation between the questions related to the specifications of e-learning modules.
Table 4. Bivariate correlation between the questions related to the specifications of e-learning modules.
123456
1. Comprehensive and short modules1.000
2. Examples0.425 **1.000
3. Descriptions of definitions and terms0.342 **0.724 **1.000
4. Graphics0.422 **0.807 **0.691 **1.000
5. Explanatory videos0.424 **0.662 **0.600 **0.824 **1.000
6. Revision after units0.408 **0.582 **0.693 **0.692 **0.773 **1.000
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level.
Table 5. Familiarization of older adults with information and communication technologies.
Table 5. Familiarization of older adults with information and communication technologies.
nmeanSD
Frequency of Use of Online Activities
 News1032.7671.139
 Internet Search1033.3981.174
 Chat and Social Media1032.4661.274
 E-Mail1032.7961.424
 Entertainment1032.4951.220
 E-Shopping1031.7571.071
 E-Learning1032.2821.294
 E-Banking1031.9711.256
 E-Government1032.2431.356
Confidence on Mobile Applications
 Calls1034.0781.326
 Text Messages1033.6601.525
 Web Browsing1033.4951.533
 E-Mail1033.1261.678
 Social Media1032.6211.652
 Games1031.9221.334
 Alarm Clock1033.2521.643
 Clock1033.5531.551
 Calculator1033.2331.567
 Reminders1032.7771.639
 Notes1032.4761.558
 Maps1032.9221.576
Table 6. Impact of demographic characteristics and educational level on participants’ confidence towards the use of the Internet.
Table 6. Impact of demographic characteristics and educational level on participants’ confidence towards the use of the Internet.
CharacteristicNot at AllUsually Need HelpDepends on the UseFeel ConfidentChi Square Test
Gender n(%) x 2 ( 3 ) = 4.307
p =   0.230
φ = 0.204
n = 103
 Male1(2.8)3(8.3)13(36.1)19(52.8)
 Female8(11.9)5(7.5)30(44.8)24(35.8)
Age n(%) x 2 ( 6 ) = 7.441
p =   0.282
φ =   0.269
n = 103
 55–647(8.6)4(4.9)34(42)36(44.4)
 65–742(10.5)4(21.1)8(42.1)5(26.3)
 75+0(0)0(0)1(33.3)2(66.7)
Residence n(%) x 2 ( 3 ) = 4.364
p = 0.225
φ =   0.206
n = 103
 Civil Center6(7.6)4(5.1)35(44.3)34(43)
 Providence3(12.5)4(16.7)8(33.3)9(37.5)
Educational Level n(%) x 2 ( 9 ) = 26.990
p =   0.001
φ =   0.512
n = 103
 Not graduated from primary school0(0)1(50)1(50)0(0)
 Primary school5(31.3)2(12.5)8(50)1(6.3)
 High school1(9.1)2(18.2)4(36.4)4(36.4)
 Higher education3(4.1)3(4.1)30(40.5)38(51.4)
Table 7. Older adults’ attitudes towards the preferred learning approach and the specifications of the e-learning modules.
Table 7. Older adults’ attitudes towards the preferred learning approach and the specifications of the e-learning modules.
Participants’ AttitudesnmeanSD
Learning Approach
 Existence of specialized/autonomous e-learning modules1034.0290.954
 Grouping of the relevant e-learning modules1033.9130.941
 Continuity between the content of the modules1034.1070.839
 Step-by-step presentation of the content of the modules1034.3010.895
 Existence of exercises after each session1034.2910.788
 Existence of practice questions during the modules1034.1850.905
 Availability of scores on each assessment test 1033.5631.265
 Existence of assessment test after each e-learning module1034.1940.852
Specifications of Modules
 Modules to be comprehensive and short1034.1550.998
 The modules to include examples1034.5240.778
 Descriptions for definitions and terms in the modules1034.2520.947
 Utilization of special graphics for the design of modules1034.4470.905
 Utilization of explanatory videos in the modules1034.4370.788
 Existence of revision after the completion of a module1034.3880.910

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pappas, M.A.; Demertzi, E.; Papagerasimou, Y.; Koukianakis, L.; Voukelatos, N.; Drigas, A. Cognitive-Based E-Learning Design for Older Adults. Soc. Sci. 2019, 8, 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8010006

AMA Style

Pappas MA, Demertzi E, Papagerasimou Y, Koukianakis L, Voukelatos N, Drigas A. Cognitive-Based E-Learning Design for Older Adults. Social Sciences. 2019; 8(1):6. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8010006

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pappas, Marios A., Eleftheria Demertzi, Yannis Papagerasimou, Lefteris Koukianakis, Nikitas Voukelatos, and Athanasios Drigas. 2019. "Cognitive-Based E-Learning Design for Older Adults" Social Sciences 8, no. 1: 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8010006

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop