Previous Article in Journal
Political Communication in the Age of Platforms
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Measuring Money Attitudes and Beliefs

Department of Leadership and Organisational Behaviour, Norwegian Business School (BI), Nydalveien, 0484 Olso, Norway
Encyclopedia 2025, 5(2), 78; https://doi.org/10.3390/encyclopedia5020078
Submission received: 12 March 2025 / Revised: 26 April 2025 / Accepted: 3 June 2025 / Published: 5 June 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Social Sciences)

Abstract

:
Over the last 50 years, there have been many attempts to devise and validate questionnaires to assess an individual’s attitude toward money and related issues. This paper attempts to conduct a comprehensive review of those measures. In all, this paper documents 54 salient studies and consults many other references from diverse and multi-disciplinary literature. There is evidently a great deal of overlap in the money attitudes dimensions assessed. Research has concentrated on the correlates of these attitudes (cultural, demographic, financial, and personality). Suggestions for the choice and development of measures are made, along with propositions for future research.

1. Introduction

People undoubtedly differ in their money attitudes, beliefs, and habits, which can have serious consequences for their well-being [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. Over the last 50 years, psychologists have attempted to construct and validate measures of attitudes to, and beliefs about, money [11]. Various research groups are involved in this literature, including economists; clinical, economic, and social psychologists; financial advisors; and sociologists [12,13]. All are interested in how people think about, and use, their money in many different ways [14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22]. Topics of interest have ranged from the relationship of money beliefs to Bitcoin [23], personality and money [24], as well as money as a symbol in the relationship between financial advisors and their clients [25]. Some researchers are interested in parental financial education [26] and others in spending and saving [4,27]. There is now a great deal of interest in financial literacy and how that is influenced by money attitudes [28,29,30,31]. It has been recognized that money attitudes and financial literacy are clearly related to financial well-being, which is the concern of many [32]. Studies have been conducted around the world and the interest in the area is truly universal [33].
This disciplinary divergence in this area makes it particularly difficult to review and keep up to date with the literature. Moreover, it highlights certain disciplinary disparities in values and expertise. Thus, psychologists seem more interested in, and competent at, questionnaire designs and evaluation; financial advisors are more intrigued by changing money beliefs, and sociologists are fascinated by the distribution of these beliefs in society. There are also detectable and different ABC (Affectively, Behavioural, and Cognitively) models and some tension between quantitative and qualitative researchers.
The literature has grown exponentially over this half-century, and there now exist several noteworthy reviews [34,35,36]. Some researchers, like Thomas Tang, have worked in the area for over 30 years, publishing well over one hundred papers and are concerned with ethical and business correlates of monetary beliefs [37,38]. There are also many specific interest areas like gender differences in the use of money, student understanding and use of money, cultural differences in money attitudes, and how best to help people with financial problems [39].
This paper documents over 50 research studies from various countries and disciplines that explore money attitudes, an issue of both practical and theoretical interest. This review will inevitably miss critical papers because of the complexity of the topic, but it serves to partly update the reviews by Furnham [40] and Sesini and Lozza [34,35]. The essential aim of this review is to assist researchers in their choice of instruments to assess money beliefs. Furthermore, it indicates what individual difference factors have been investigated with respect to the various measures, to what extent results (e.g., sex differences) have been replicated, and, therefore, what remains to be explored. It should be stressed that this paper is not an attempt to provide a comprehensive review of the many and growing studies on attitudes to money; rather, the aim is to examine the different questionnaires that have been designed to assess those attitudes.
There are many questionnaires, probably now over 20, which attempt to assess money-related attitudes and behaviours [6,9,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50]. These are being developed all the time, often for very specific purposes [51]. Many have been critically appraised [52,53,54].
When researchers select an instrument to measure money attitudes and beliefs, they nearly always have similar specific criteria: they want it to be comprehensive to the extent that it measures all known and salient dimensions; they want it to be as short as possible (to encourage participant retention); and, most of all, they want it to be reliable (internally, and test–retest) as well as valid (concurrent, construct, convergent, discriminant, face, incremental, and predictive). Ideally, they would also like it to be cross-culturally valid, prone to little impression management, and appropriate for a wide variety of people in terms of demography. These criteria are a “tall order” for non-experts attempting to construct a useful measure of a construct they understand. This paper is not a psychometric review, though there is clearly a need for one to help researchers make choices about which measure to use.
This paper will first document the relevant papers in a table and compare the approaches and findings. Then the issue of dimensions will be discussed, which is often concerned with the theory or model underlying monetary attitudes and beliefs. The final section of the paper will discuss some important research questions in this area and what important future research needs to be conducted.

2. Scales Available

Sesini and Lozza [35] conducted a very helpful, comprehensive review of the scales used in this area, ranking 10 measures by the percentage of papers they found using them. The top four were the Money Attitudes Scale (29 items), measuring four dimensions [50]; the Love of Money Scale (17 items), assessing four dimensions [55]; the Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (60 items), by Furnham [41]; and the Money Ethics Scale (30 items) [47]. However, the percentage of papers quoting the measure is not a good evaluation of value but more of longevity. There are many more significant criteria to consider when evaluating the utility of a questionnaire [56].
Until the millennium, two scales dominated the literature. The first was the Money Attitude Scale devised by Yamauchi and Templer [52], which has been thoroughly investigated [53,54] and translated into different languages [57]. This scale identified three broad content areas of psychological money aspects (security/insecurity, retention, and power/prestige). The test has been used consistently over the past 40 years. The second test was developed by Furnham [41] and is the Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS). Google Scholar indicates that the scale has been quoted over 900 times. It, too, has been subject to analysis, modification, and translation [58,59]. Yet, the original questionnaire is perhaps too long for administration in many studies.
Most questionnaires regarding this topic have appeared in the academic literature with more frequency this millennium. However, some have appeared in books, usually of a clinical nature, and have been adopted by researchers, for example, the Money Sanity/Pathology Scale [60], which consists of 20 dichotomous (Yes/No) items with relatively high reliability (α = 0.75). In a study of over 100,000 participants, the scale depicted a clear and interpretable multiple-factor structure with acceptable alphas [42,61]. The first subscale contains four items describing compulsive hoarding (α = 0.64). The second subscale defines careless spending attitudes (α = 0.52) and includes three items. The third subscale refers to worried spending behaviours (α = 0.74). The final subscale consists of three items describing money used as compensation for other frustrations (α = 0.55).
However, importantly, the above-mentioned review by Sesini and Lozza does not evaluate a number of more recent scales, such as the New Money Attitudes Questionnaire (NMAQ) [9], the Money Mindset [62], the New Money Behaviour Quiz (NMBQ) [43], the German Gamblers Scale of Money Attitudes (SMAG) [63], and the Importance of Money Scale (IMS) [64].
Some recent measures are the outcome of data collection opportunities, such as the Money Mindset and the New Money Behaviour Quiz, where researchers investigated data collected for PR and commercial purposes. A well-designed scale by Lay and Furnham [9] is a good example of research in the area. They developed a 30-item questionnaire with a 5-factor model labelled Achievement and Success, Power and Status, Mindful and Responsible, Saving Concerns, and Financial Literacy Worries. The results showed that demography (sex, age, and education), ideology (politics and religious practices), and self-rated happiness, success, and adjustment were all significantly and logically related to the five factors, especially the first two. They noted that Worries about Financial Literacy were an important, neglected factor in money attitudes research. However, this evaluation could, and no doubt will, be challenged. The literature requires some “questionnaire housekeeping” and the more general use of a scale, which is easily obtainable and translatable. This issue occurs in many areas of research where an increase in the scales used makes it progressively difficult and inconvenient to compare and evaluate findings.
Table 1 exhibits the measures used to assess money beliefs and the findings from some relevant studies. There has been a steady increase in published papers, particularly after the 1990s, which has shown a substantial increase in interest in this area. The table reveals that most studies used students, though there is a growing interest in young adults [65]. What the table does not show are the reported psychometric properties of each measure as performed by Furnham and Robinson [56], which would be a different review. That is, there are often unanswered questions about reliability (test–retest) as well as validity (concurrent, construct, convergent, discriminant, predictive). Indeed, it seems that researchers in this area have been less concerned with the psychometric properties of the scales than they should have been. However, for those interested in selecting a particular instrument, it may be best to choose one that has been used in a number of previous published studies so that it would be possible to obtain test norms as well as information about construct and predictive validity.
As with so many studies, it is difficult to obtain large representative samples that produce more robust and reliable findings. Furthermore, most (but not all) studies were conducted in America and Europe, though some recent studies have been reported from other English-speaking countries [62] and large cross-cultural studies [48,83,93].
Indeed, there has been a lot of curiosity in translating and comparing money attitude questionnaires, which were mainly developed in the English-speaking world by groups from other countries, like Austria and Albania [94,95], Ghana [96], Malaysia [88], Mexico [97], Poland [70], Singapore [49], and Turkey [72]. Some of these studies seek to replicate others to ascertain their cross-cultural validity [71,97]. However, relatively few use any cultural models to test their theories or the effect of a country’s Gross Domestic Product on money attitudes [13]. It is rare to find a longitudinal study on adults involving a range of relevant variables.
Perhaps the most noteworthy feature of Table 1 is the correlates of the money beliefs. Many studies reveal that money attitudes are related to gender [6,8,42,47,66,82,87,98,99,100,101], culture [13,73,97], education level [6,41], and political and religious values [42,47]. Some studies have also examined the all-important socialisation factors, like the intergenerational transfer of attitudes [102]. Many studies have looked at cultural correlates of the attitudes [67,69,79]; others looked at money attitudes and personality variables [74,85,86,103,104].
Based on the results, it seems males tend to associate money with achievement, power, and freedom more than women [42], who, in turn, are more inclined to see money as a source of anxiety and associate it with retention [66] and budgeting [47].
There is also considerable evidence that other factors correlate with money attitudes and beliefs [40]. Variables like financial anxiety have been extensively researched by Klontz and colleagues [6,49,105,106]. Others include subjective ratings of success and happiness and the simple categorisation of whether one is a spender or a saver [107]. Some studies have even related monetary beliefs to spirituality [108].
The most surprising aspect of the final column in Table 1 is the range of variables associated with money beliefs and behaviours. Many have considered age, gender, and education, seeking to determine whether these classic demographic variables influence the establishment of money attitudes and beliefs. In contrast, others display a particular interest in correlates of money pathology, like compulsive buying, and economic variables like saving, wealth, and investing. Surprisingly, only a few studies have related these attitudes to intelligence or the classic personality variables [109].
Hence, Table 1 indicates the vitality of the field while highlighting that it is dominated by piecemeal studies rarely based on a particular theory.

3. Dimensions

Just as it has taken years for personality psychologists to develop an agreed and comprehensive Five Factor Model, researchers in this area have also struggled to agree on the essential “higher order” factors and to label them. The problem for the reviewer is that studies have found different factors (ranging from four to eight) through factor analytic techniques but have also labelled them very differently. There are, however, clear patterns; for instance, money as power/prestige, power/spending, status vs. anxiety, worry, and inadequacy. Three questionnaires have a clear and similar Four Factor Model: Furnham et al. [42] depict money as representing love, power, security, and freedom, much like Rose and Orr’s [46] description of money as worry, status, security, and freedom, and Klontz et al.’s [6] portrayal as avoidance, status, vigilance, and worship.
The scale frequently used that is distinct from many others is that by Tang and Chui [55], namely motivator, success/rich, and importance, along with the longer scales, such as that of Furnham [41], which distinguishes between inadequacy and obsession. In terms of length, the Money Types Questionnaire [42] has 16 items, and the Money Motivation Scale [46] includes 17 items.
Sesini and Lozza [36] offer helpful commentary on 10 scales, mainly noting how frequently the scales have been utilised in previous studies, which is often more a measure of when the scale was published and its availability than its psychometric properties. They note that, for instance, the Money Ethics Scale [55] is perhaps most applicable to studies concerning job-related and ethical variables, while the Klontz Money Behavior Inventory [6] has a clinical and therapeutic feel associated with giving financial advice. Both scales by Rose and Orr [46], with 29 citations, and by Furnham et al. [42], with 55 citations, seem simple and beneficial as short measures with comprehensible and overlapping dimensions.
On the other hand, some measures have “gone the other way”, trying to be more specific. Furnham and Horne [89] developed a new facet-level questionnaire to measure “money madness” based on a well-established Four Factor Model. There were three facets for each dimension. For example, money as security had three facets, namely, compulsive saver, self-denier, hunter, and collector.
In a recent paper, Kraemer et al. [12] examine the sociological literature on money as freedom, a status signal, a means for community-building, and a means to regulate and discipline behaviour. They devised a questionnaire to assess these four dimensions and found seven factors labelled freedom, community, status, institutional control, conflict, work-related control, and household control. After a lengthy discussion regarding the classic sociological works on money, they concluded that the ambiguity of money can be only partly resolved by distinguishing the concrete earmarking of money for specific social purposes from the potentially unspecific, open usability of money for alternative concrete or fictional purposes.

4. Research Questions

Most research in this area, excluding the development of questionnaires, concerns the causes/origins of money attitudes and beliefs, their many correlates, and, most importantly, their consequences. Thus, clinicians have long been fascinated by the childhood origins of money obsessionality and miserliness, the behaviours of these individuals, and how they might be cured. In contrast, those interested in financial literacy have been eager to understand how money beliefs influence literacy and vice versa. Some others are also curious about the relationship between money beliefs and happiness.
There is also the question of how money attitudes and beliefs have changed over time as most people have become wealthier. There are also questions about the effect of credit cards and “electronic money” on attitudes. It is also apparent that there are probably cultural differences based on socio-economic, structural, and religious differences between countries. Finally, different disciplines all interested in money, such as economics, finance, psychology, and sociology, appear to understand how people think about and use their money very differently. Interest in these issues partly explains why research on money attitudes and beliefs is growing so rapidly.
Essentially, there appear to be three research issues. First is agreeing on a model of money beliefs and attitudes involving a parsimonious description of all salient dimensions and how they relate. This should be followed by the development and psychometric appraisal of a widely adopted test. Second is the question of how beliefs develop and change over time. It would consider the stability of attitudes and the efficacy of techniques to change them to be more adaptive. Third, are the correlates and consequences of these attitudes, particularly concerning work factors, financial behaviour, and happiness.
There are several under-explored topics in this field, like money belief responses to changes in economic conditions and the efficacy of economic socialisation programmes. Whilst a few papers and chapters attempt to integrate related ideas and research, with modest attempts at a model, these are relatively scarce [34,35,110].
Researchers in many fields call for the end of, or at least a reduction in, the development of new measures of similar phenomena [111]. The same must be true of research in this area. It is not apparent why so many instruments have been devised; it may have to do with the accessibility, translatability, or tone (clinical vs. business) of questionnaires, which are not always available. Evidently, there are four to six factors associated with money attitudes, which may be usefully supplemented with measures of financial literacy. Researchers have lamented that the “toothbrush” problem in questionnaire development suggested that too many are used only once by the person who constructs them [112].Similarly, the efficacy of questionnaires over other methods, like interviews, has been questioned [113].
Questionnaire development and evaluation also need to be performed by skilled psychometricians who understand the issues. Part of the problem is that many people interested in the area, including those in finance, banking, and economics, as well as developmental and clinical psychologists, do not always have the skilled knowledge to construct validated questionnaires.
One issue for different groups in this field (clinical psychologists and financial advisors) is how to change money beliefs and “scripts” [7,114]. We have known for a long time that “unhealthy beliefs” and scripts can lead to a range of very unhealthy behaviours, from impulse over-buying and spending to extremely poor money management. Thus, therapists from different backgrounds attempt to alter these beliefs to make them more adaptive and healthy. The question is, which techniques to use and how effective are they? It is a fundamental and difficult aspect of research, which, thus far, is missing from this literature. There is indeed a growing interest in all aspects of financial literacy [115,116].
Unsurprisingly, it would be desirable to move on from exclusively self-report studies, with all the problems associated with method invariance, impression management, and inflated correlations. Thus, studies should observe money beliefs through actual behaviours, like investing, saving, stock-market involvement, and personal relationships. The issue is both the causes and consequences of these beliefs, which are not always reliably derived from self-reports. Clearly, given the extent to which our everyday behaviour is electronically monitored, particularly the use of credit cards, valuable behavioural evidence is now available to help validate money attitude questionnaires. That is, researchers could study the relationship between money attitudes and the actual financial behaviours of individuals using, for instance, individual bank transactions, given, of course, that ethical guidelines are followed.
The use of longitudinal studies, which observe the consequences of beliefs over time, would be ideal. This, and only this, method enables further understanding of causal relationships, which are fundamental to this whole research area. Financial institutions may have some such data, but establishing a rich data set is vital to uncovering the causes and consequences of money attitudes.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing does not apply to this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Astriti, N.-E.; Prodromitis, G. Death Anxiety Among Urban Middle Class: The Predictive Role of Income and Symbolic Money Attitudes. J. Humanist. Psychol. 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chen, Y.; Lee, V.; Tang, T.L. Does thinking about making money help people save money? Behavioral economics—Monetary wisdom across genders: Ardent love of money aspirations and $1 million resource allocation. Asian J. Bus. Ethics 2024, 13, 323–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Furnham, A. The personality disorders and money beliefs and behaviors. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2020, 2, e1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kelley, H.H.; Chandler, A.B.; LeBaron-Black, A.B.; Li, X.; Curran, M.A.; Yorgason, J.B.; James, S. Spenders and Tightwads Among Newlyweds: Perceptions of Partner Financial Behaviors and Relational Well-Being. J. Financ. Ther. 2022, 13, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Kim, M.J.; Huang, Y.; Chen, H. A penny saved is a penny earned: How money-view and self-view jointly influence consumer financial behaviour. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2022, 46, 449–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Klontz, B.; Britt, S.L.; Mentzer, J.; Klontz, T. Money beliefs and financial behaviors: Development of the Klontz Money Script Inventory. J. Financ. Ther. 2011, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Klontz, B.; Britt, S.L.; Archuleta, K.; Klontz, P.T. Disordered Money Behaviors: Development of the Klontz Money Behavior Inventory. J. Financ. Ther. 2012, 3, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Klontz, B.T.; Seay, M.C.; Sullivan, P.; Canale, A. The psychology of wealth: Psychological factors associated with high income. J. Financ. Plan. 2014, 27, 46–53. [Google Scholar]
  9. Lay, A.; Furnham, A. A New Money Attitudes Questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 35, 813–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Zabelina, E.; Kurnosova, S.; Shchukina, K. Attitude to Money as a Factor of Life Satisfaction of the Northern Indigenous Peoples. In Management, Innovation and Entrepreneurship in Challenging Global Times. Lecture Notes in Management and Industrial Engineering; Prostean, G.I., Lavios, J.J., Brancu, L., Şahin, F., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wernimont, P.F.; Fitzpatrick, S. The meaning of money. J. Appl. Psychol. 1972, 56, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kraemer, K.; Jakelja, L.; Brugger, F.; Nessel, S. The social ambiguity of money: Empirical evidence on the multiple usability of money in social life. Rev. Soc. Econ. 2024, 82, 98–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lynn, R. The Secret of the Miracle Economy: Different National Attitudes to Competitiveness and Money; Social Affairs Unit: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bailey, W.; Gustafson, W. An examination of the relationship between personality factors and attitudes to money. In Handbook of Behavioural Economics; Frantz, R., Singh, H., Gerber, J., Eds.; JAI Press: Stamford, CT, USA, 1991; pp. 271–285. [Google Scholar]
  15. Christopher, A.N.; Marek, P.; Carroll, S.M. Materialism and Attitudes toward Money: An Exploratory Investigation. Individ. Differ. Res. 2004, 2, 109–117. [Google Scholar]
  16. de Almeida, F.; Ferreira, M.B.; Soro, J.C.; Silva, C.S. Attitudes Toward Money and Control Strategies of Financial Behavior: A Comparison Between Overindebted and Non-overindebted Consumers. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 566594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Furnham, A. Attitudinal correlates and demographic predictors of monetary beliefs and behaviours. J. Organ. Behav. 1996, 17, 375–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Furnham, A.; Cuppello, S.; Fenton-O’Creevy, M. Correlates of Stock Market Investment. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2024, 17, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gasiorowska, A.; Zaleskiewicz, T. The psychology of money. In Handbook of Research Methods in Behavioural Economics; Altman, M., Ed.; Edward Elgar Publishing: Northampton, MA, USA, 2023; Chapter 12; pp. 209–225. [Google Scholar]
  20. Harnish, R.J.; Bridges, K.R.; Nataraajan, R.; Gump, J.T.; Carson, A.E. The impact of money attitudes and global life satisfaction on the maladaptive pursuit of consumption. Psychol. Mark. 2018, 35, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ngcamu, L.J.; Quaye, E.S.; Horvey, S.S.; Jaravaza, D.C. Personality traits, money attitudes and consumer decision-making styles as predictors of investment products choice in South Africa. J. Consum. Behav. 2023, 22, 618–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tatarko, A.; Schmidt, P. Social capital and attitudes towards money. Soc. Sci. Res. Netw. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kim, M. A Psychological Approach to Bitcoin Usage Behavior in the Era of COVID-19: Focusing on the Role of Attitudes toward Money. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2021, 62, 102606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. McCann, S.J.H. Personality and Money Matters: The Relation of State Resident Neuroticism to Common State-Level Financial and Economic Indicators. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 43, 3307–3327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lozza, E.; Castiglioni, C.; Bonanomi, A.; Poli, F. Money as a symbol in the relationship between financial advisors and their clients: A dyadic study. Int. J. Bank Mark. 2022, 40, 613–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. LeBaron, A.B.; Holmes, E.K.; Jorgensen, B.L.; Bean, R.A. Parental financial education during childhood and financial behaviors of emerging adults. J. Financ. Couns. Plan. 2020, 31, 42–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kappes, H.B.; Gladstone, J.J.; Hershfield, H.E. Beliefs about whether spending implies wealth. J. Consum. Res. 2021, 48, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Liu, T.; Fan, M.; Li, Y.; Yue, P. Financial literacy and household financial resilience. Financ. Res. Lett. 2024, 63, 105378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lusardi, A.; Mitchell, O.S. The Importance of Financial Literacy: Opening a New Field. J. Econ. Perspect. 2023, 37, 137–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Michiels, A.; Binz Astrachan, C. It’s (not) all about the money: Financial decisions at the intersection of business and family. J. Fam. Bus. Manag. 2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nuwailah, E.J.; Ermawati, W.J. Young Muslims’ Financial Behavior: Money Attitudes and Financial Literacy by Debt Status. Rev. Islam. Account. 2025, 4, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Brüggen, E.C.; Hogreve, J.; Holmlund, M.; Kabadayi, S.; Löfgren, M. Financial well-being: A conceptualization and research agenda. J. Bus. Res. 2017, 79, 228–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Neto, J.; Neto, F.; Furnham, A. Correlates of money attitudes among Portuguese people. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2024, 7, e1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sesini, G.; Lozza, E. La rappresentazione sociale del denaro e della crisi economica: Differenze per ciclo di vita [The social representation of money and the economic crisis: Differences by life cycle]. Sist. Intelligenti 2023, 35, 611–631. [Google Scholar]
  35. Sesini, G.; Lozza, E. Understanding Individual Attitude to Money: A Systematic Scoping Review and Research Agenda. Collabra: Psychol. 2023, 9, 77305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sesini, G.; Manzi, C.; Lozza, E. Is psychology of money a gendered affair? A scoping review and research agenda. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2023, 47, 2701–2723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tang, T.L.-P. The meaning of money: Extension and exploration of the money ethic scale in a sample of university students in Taiwan. J. Organ. Behav. 1993, 14, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Tang, T.L.-P. Monetary Wisdom: A Measure of Attitude Toward Money—Constructs and Items. In Encyclopedia of Business and Professional Ethics; Poff, D.C., Michalos, A.C., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020; pp. 1344–1350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. White, K.J.; McCoy, M.; Watkins, K.; Chen, X.; Koposko, J.L.; Mizuta, M. “We Don’t Talk About That”: Exploring Money Conversations of Black, Hispanic, and White Households. Fam. Consum. Sci. Res. J. 2021, 49, 328–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Furnham, A. The New Psychology of Money, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  41. Furnham, A. Many sides of the coin: The psychology of money usage. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1984, 5, 501–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Furnham, A.; Wilson, E.; Telford, K. The meaning of money: The validation of a short money-types measure. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 707–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Furnham, A.; Grover, S. A new money behaviour quiz. J. Individ. Differ. 2020, 41, 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Khare, A. Money attitudes, materialism, and compulsiveness: Scale development and validation. J. Glob. Mark. 2014, 27, 30–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lim, V.K.G.; Teo, T.S.H. Sex, money and financial hardship: An empirical study of attitudes towards money among undergraduates in Singapore. J. Econ. Psychol. 1997, 18, 369–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Rose, G.M.; Orr, L.M. Measuring and exploring symbolic money meanings. Psychol. Mark. 2007, 24, 743–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Tang, T.L.-P. The meaning of money revisited. J. Organ. Behav. 1992, 13, 197–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Tang, T.L.-P.; Sutarso, T.; Ansari, M.A.; Lim, V.K.G.; Teo, T.S.H.; Arias-Galicia, F.; Garber, I.E.; Chiu, R.K.-K.; Charles-Pauvers, B.; Luna-Arocas, R.; et al. Monetary intelligence and behavioral economics across 32 cultures: Good apples enjoy good quality of life in good barrels. J. Bus. Ethics 2018, 148, 893–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Taylor, C.D.; Klontz, B.T.; Britt, S.L. Internal consistency and convergent validity of the Klontz Money Behavior Inventory (KMBI). J. Financ. Ther. 2016, 6, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Yamauchi, K.T.; Templer, D.J. The development of a money attitude scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1982, 46, 522–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Beutler, I.F.; Gudmunson, C. New adolescent money attitude scales: Entitlement and conscientiousness. J. Financ. Couns. Plan. 2012, 23, 18–31. [Google Scholar]
  52. Baker, P.M.; Hagedorn, R.B. Attitudes to money in a random sample of adults: Factor analysis of the MAS and MBBS scales, and correlations with demographic variables. J. Socio-Econ. 2008, 37, 1803–1814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Spinella, M.; Lester, D.; Yang, B. Consistency of the Yamauchi/Templer Money Attitude Scale. Psychol. Rep. 2005, 97, 962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yang, B.; Lester, D. Internal Consistency of the Yamauchi/Templer Money Attitude Scale. Psychol. Rep. 2002, 91, 994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Tang, T.L.P.; Chiu, R.K. Income, Money Ethic, pay satisfaction, commitment, and unethical behavior: Is the love of money the root of evil for Hong Kong employees? J. Bus. Ethics 2003, 46, 13–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Furnham, A.; Robinson, C. Around a hundred measures of the major personality disorders: An expert review and practitioner’s guide. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Roberts, J.; Sepulveda, C.M. Money attitudes and compulsion buying. J. Int. Consum. Mark. 1999, 11, 53–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bailey, W.; Gustafson, W. Gender and gender-role orientation differences in attitudes and behaviors toward money. In Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Conference of the Association for Financial Counselling and Planning Education; Kitt, K., Ed.; CUNY: New York, NY, USA, 1986; pp. 11–20. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hanley, A.; Wilhelm, M.S. Compulsive buying: An exploration into self-esteem and money attitudes. J. Econ. Psychol. 1992, 13, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Forman, N. Mind Over Money, 1st ed.; Doubleday: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  61. von Stumm, S.; Fenton-O’Creevy, M.; Furnham, A. Financial capability, money attitudes and socioeconomic status: Risks for experiencing adverse financial events. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2013, 54, 344–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Furnham, A.; Murphy, T.-A. Money types, money beliefs, and financial worries: An Australian study. Aust. J. Psychol. 2019, 71, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sleczka, P.; Braun-Michl, B.; Kraus, L. Gamblers’ attitudes towards money and their relationship to gambling disorder among young men. J. Behav. Addict. 2020, 9, 744–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Franzen, A.; Mader, S. The Importance of Money Scale (IMS): A new instrument to measure the importance of material well-being. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2022, 184, 111172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Utkarsh; Pandey, A.; Ashta, A.; Spiegelman, E.; Sutan, A. Catch them young: Impact of financial socialization, financial literacy and attitude towards money on financial well-being of young adults. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2020, 44, 531–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Gresham, A.; Fontenot, G. The differing attitudes of the sexes toward money: An application of the money attitude scale. Adv. Mark. 1989, 8, 380–384. [Google Scholar]
  67. Bailey, W.; Lown, J. A cross-cultural examination of the aetiology of attitudes toward money. J. Consum. Stud. Home Econ. 1993, 17, 391–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Wilhelm, M.S.; Varcoe, K.P.; Fridrich, A.H. Financial satisfaction and assessment of financial progress: Importance of money attitudes. J. Financ. Couns. Plan. 1993, 4, 181–199. [Google Scholar]
  69. Bailey, W.; Johnson, P.; Adams, C.; Lawson, R.; Williams, P.; Lown, J. An exploratory study of money beliefs and behaviours scale using data from 3 nations. In Consumer Interests Annual; ACCZ: London, UK, 1994; pp. 178–185. [Google Scholar]
  70. Górniak, J. Attitudes towards money and dealing with money: Selected results of the research in Poland. Innov. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. Res. 1999, 12, 633–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Masuo, D.M.; Lakshmi Malroutu, Y.; Hanashiro, R.; Kim, J.H. College students’ money beliefs and behaviors: An Asian perspective. Early Child. Educ. J. 2004, 25, 469–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Ozgen, O.; Bayoglu, A. Turkish college students’ attitudes towards money. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2005, 29, 493–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Burgess, S.M. The importance and motivational content of money attitudes: South Africans with living standards similar to those in in-dustrialised Western countries. S. Afr. J. Psychol. 2005, 35, 106–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Engelberg, E.; Sjoberg, L. Money attitudes and emotional intelligence. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 36, 2027–2047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Tang, T.L.P. Income and quality of life: Does the love of money make a difference? J. Bus. Ethics 2007, 72, 375–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Cunningham, P.H.; Tang, T.L.-P.; Frauman, E.; Ivy, M.I.; Perry, T.L. Leisure Ethic, Money Ethic, and Occupational Commitment among Recreation and Park Professionals: Does Gender Make a Difference? Public Pers. Manag. 2012, 41, 421–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Tang, T.L.P.; Liu, H. Love of money and unethical behavior intention: Does an authentic supervisor’s personal integrity and character (ASPIRE) make a difference? J. Bus. Ethics 2012, 107, 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Chen, J.Q.; Tang, T.L.P.; Tang, N.Y. Temptation, monetary intelligence (love of money), and environmental context on unethical intentions and cheating. J. Bus. Ethics 2014, 123, 197–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Kim, S. Does a money-is-all attitude cause alienation? A cross-cultural comparison of Korea, the US and Sweden. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2014, 38, 650–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Sardžoska, E.G.; Tang, T.L.-P. Monetary Intelligence: Money attitudes—Unethical intentions, intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfaction, and coping strategies across public and private sectors in Macedonia. J. Bus. Ethics 2015, 130, 93–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Tang, T.L.P. Theory of monetary intelligence: Money attitudes— Religious values, making money, making ethical decisions, and making the grade. J. Bus. Ethics 2016, 133, 583–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Moloi, V. Gender Perspectives on Money Attitudes and Materialism Amongst Upwardly Mobile South Africans; Gordon Institute of Business Science University of Pretoria: Sandton, South Africa, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  83. Tang, T.L.-P.; Sutarso, T.; Ansari, M.A.; Lim, V.K.G.; Teo, T.S.H.; Arias-Galicia, F.; Garber, I.E.; Chiu, R.K.-K.; Charles-Pauvers, B.; Luna-Arocas, R.; et al. Monetary intelligence and behavioral economics: The Enron Effect—Love of money, corporate ethical values, Corruption Perceptions Index (CPI), and dishonesty across 31 geopolitical entities. J. Bus. Ethic 2018, 148, 919–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Tang, N.Y.; Chen, J.Q.; Zhang, K.L.; Tang, T.L.P. Monetary wisdom: How do investors use love of money to frame stock vol atility and enhance stock happiness? J. Happiness Stud. 2018, 19, 1831–1862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Fenton-O’Creevy, M.; Furnham, A. Personality, ideology, and money attitudes as correlates of financial literacy and competence. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2020, 3, e1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Fenton-O’creevy, M.; Furnham, A. Money attitudes, personality and chronic impulse buying. Appl. Psychol. 2020, 69, 1557–1572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Rimple, M. A Gendered Study of Attitude Towards Money. Stud. Bus. Econ. 2020, 15, 115–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Ong, Z.; Lau, J.; Zainudin, N. Money attitude, materialism and compulsive buying among Malaysian young adults. Manag. Sci. Lett. 2021, 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Furnham, A.; Horne, G. A facet measure of money madness: A preliminary analyses of a new questionnaire. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2021, 4, e1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Chaudary, S.; Zaar, S.; Tang, T.L.P. Investors’ financial aspirations excite investment decisions. Int. J. Emerg. Mark. 2022, 19, 1306–1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Tang, N.Y.; Li, Z.; Chen, J.; Tang, T.L.-P. Behavioral economics: Who are the investors with the most sustainable stock happiness, and why? Low aspiration, external control, and country domicile may save your lives—Monetary wisdom. Asian J. Bus. Ethics 2022, 11, 359–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Chen, J.Q.; Tang, T.L.P.; Wu, C.R. Holistic thinking enhances risk perceptions and curbs risk- taking intentions. Asian J. Bus. Ethics 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Tang, T.L.-P.; Li, Z.; Özbek, M.F.; Lim, V.K.G.; Teo, T.S.H.; Ansari, M.A.; Sutarso, T.; Garber, I.; Chiu, R.K.; Charles-Pauvers, B.; et al. Behavioral economics and monetary wisdom: A cross-level analysis of monetary aspiration, pay (dis)satisfaction, risk perception, and corruption in 32 nations. Bus. Ethics Environ. Responsib. 2023, 32, 925–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Furtner, R. Personality traits, biographical variables, and attitudes to money among Austrian students. Corvinus J. Sociol. Soc. Policy 2020, 11, 51–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Furtner, R.; Motro-Iljazi, E.; Jančíková, E. Money attitudes comparison between Austrian and Albanian students. Econ. Res. Guard. 2021, 11, 14–26. [Google Scholar]
  96. Bonsu, S.K. Ghanaian attitudes towards money in consumer culture. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2008, 32, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Medina, J.F.; Saegert, J.; Gresham, A. Comparison of Mexican-American and Anglo-American attitudes toward money. J. Consum. Aff. 1996, 30, 124–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Furnham, A.; von Stumm, S.; Fenton-O’creevy, M. Sex differences in money pathology in the general population. Soc. Indic. Res. 2014, 123, 701–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Furnham, A.; von Stumm, S.; Milner, R. Moneygrams: Recalled Childhood Memories about Money and Adult Money Pathology. J. Financ. Ther. 2014, 5, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Prince, M. Women, men and money styles. J. Econ. Psychol. 1993, 14, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Sahi, K.S. Understanding gender differences in money attitudes: Biological and psychological gender perspective. Int. J. Bank Mark. 2023, 41, 619–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Britt, S.L. The intergenerational transference of money attitudes and behaviors. J. Consum. Aff. 2016, 50, 539–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Fenton-O’Creevy, M.; Furnham, A. Financial distress and money attitudes. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2021, 14, 138–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Fenton-O’creevy, M.; Furnham, A. Money attitudes, financial capabilities, and impulsiveness as predictors of wealth accumulation. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0278047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Britt, S.L.; Klontz, B.; Tibbetts, R.; Leitz, L. The financial health of mental health professionals. J. Financ. Ther. 2015, 6, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Taylor, C.D.; Klontz, B.T.; Lawson, D. Money disorders and locus of control: Implications for assessment and intervention. J. Financ. Ther. 2017, 8, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Furnham, A.; Robinson, C.; Grover, S. Spenders and Savers, Tightwads and Spendthrifts: Individual correlates of personal ratings of being a spender or a saver. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2022, 15, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Pong, H.-K. Money Attitude and Spiritual Well-Being. J. Risk Financ. Manag. 2022, 15, 483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Furnham, A.; Cuppello, S. Exploring the relationship between personality and money scripts while controlling for demography, ideology, and self-esteem. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2023, 6, e1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Furnham, A.; Grover, S. Beliefs and Behaviours about investing, saving and spending. In De Gruyter Handbook of Personal Finance, 1st ed.; Grable, J.E., Chatterjee, S., Eds.; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2022; pp. 491–508. [Google Scholar]
  111. Passarelli, M.; Casetta, L.; Rizzi, L.; Chiorri, C.; Cassina, F.; Voi, S.; Rocco, D. Short and sweet: Comparing strategies for the reduction of questionnaires on self-criticism and social safeness while preserving construct validity. Int. J. Psychol. 2024, 59, 1234–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Elson, M.; Hussey, I.; Alsalti, T. Psychological measures aren’t toothbrushes. Commun. Psychol. 2023, 1, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Einola, K.; Alvesson, M. Behind the Numbers: Questioning Questionnaires. J. Manag. Inq. 2020, 30, 102–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Klontz, B.T.; Britt, S.L. How clients’ money scripts predict their financial behaviors. J. Financ. Plan. 2012, 25, 33–43. [Google Scholar]
  115. Chowdhry, N.; Dholakia, U.M. Know thyself financially: How financial self-awareness can benefit consumers and financial advisors. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2020, 3, e1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Stella, G.P.; Filotto, U.; Cervellati, E.M. A Proposal for a New Financial Literacy Questionnaire. Int. J. Bus. Manag. 2020, 15, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Empirical studies: methodological characteristics and factors related to money attitudes.
Table 1. Empirical studies: methodological characteristics and factors related to money attitudes.
Empirical StudyScale UsedNSampleLocationFactors That Influence Money Attitudes
Wernimont & Fitzpatrick, 1972 [11]Modified Semantic Differential (MSD)533College students, engineers, religious sisters, etc.U.S. Midwestern CityWork experience, socioeconomic level, and gender
Yamauchi & Templer, 1982 [50]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)300Adults from different professionsLos Angeles and Fresno, CA
Furnham, 1984 [41]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)256College studentsEngland, Scotland & WalesIncome, gender, age, and education
Bailey & Gustafson, 1986 [58]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)NACollege studentsU.S. Southwestern cityGender
Gresham & Fontenot, 1989 [66]Modified Money Attitude Scale (MMAS)557College students and their parentsU.S. Southwestern citiesGender
Bailey & Gustafson, 1991 [14]Modified Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MMBBS)472College studentsU.S. Southwestern citySensitivity and emotional stability
Hanley & Wilhelm, 1992 [59]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)143NAPhoenix, Tucson, Denver, and DetroitCompulsive behaviour
Tang, 1992 [47]Money Ethic Scale (MES)249 and 689Full-time employeesMiddle Tennessee cityAge, income, work ethic, social, political, and religious values
Bailey & Lown, 1993 [67]Money in the Past and Future Scale654College students, their relatives, and other professionalsWestern U.S. StatesAge
Tang, 1993 [37]Money Ethic Scale (MES)68 and 249College studentsTaiwan
Wilhelm et al., 1993 [68]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)559Adult AmericansUSAGender, financial progress
Bailey et al., 1994 [69]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)344, 291, and 328Employed adults related to college studentsUSA, Australia, CanadaGeographical location
Lim & Teo, 1997 [45]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS) & Money Attitude Scale (MAS)200StudentsSingaporeGender differences
Roberts & Sepulveda, 1999 [57]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)273AdultsMexicoCompulsive buying
Górniak, 1999 [70]Various questionnaires1007AdultsPolandSocial & personal background, achievement orientation
Christopher et al., 2004 [15]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)204StudentsUSAMaterialism
Masuo et al., 2004 [71]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)290StudentsKorea, Japan & USGeographical location
Ozgen & Bayoglu, 2005 [72]Money in the Past and Future Scale300StudentsAnkara (Turkey)Gender, age, family type
Burgess, 2005 [73]Modified Money Attitude Scale (MMAS)221Urban South AfricansMajor metropolitan citiesValues & culture
Engelberg & Sjoberg, 2006 [74]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)212StudentsSwedenEmotional intelligence
Tang, 2007 [75] The Love of Money 458Full-time employeesUSAAge
Baker & Hagedorn, 2008 [52]Money Attitude Scale (MAS) & Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)200AdultsCanadaAge, sex, income & education
Klontz et al., 2011 [6]Klontz Money Script Inventory (KMSI)422AdultsUSASex, age, race, education, gross income
Cunningham et al., 2012 [76]The Money Ethic Scale397Professional members of the National Recreation and Park AssociationUSAGender
Tang et al., 2012 [77]The Money Ethic Scale397Members of the Nat Recreation and Park AssociationUSAAge (baby boomers vs. gen-xers)
Tatarko & Schmidt, 2012 [22]Money Beliefs and Behaviour Scale (MBBS)634AdultsRussiaSocial capital
Furnham et al., 2012 [42]Short Money Type Measure (SMTM)400AdultsUKAge, ethnicity, salary, education, politics
Beutler & Gudmunson, 2012 [51]New Adolescent Money Attitude Scales
(NAMAS)
265StudentsUSAGender, age, race
von Stumm et al., 2013 [61]Short Money Type Measure (SMTM)109,472AdultsUKEducation, income, financial habits
Chen et al., 2014 [78]The Love of Money492,
256
StudentsAmerican, ChineseGender, temptation
Kim, 2014 [79]Money-is-all attitude1500AdultsKorea, USA & SwedenAlienation, socio-demographic factors
Khare, 2014 [44]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)409AdultsIndianSelf-esteem
Sardžoska & Tang, 2015 [80]Monetary Intelligence515Public and private managersMacedoniaAge, gender, income
Taylor et al., 2016 [49]Klontz Money Behavior Inventory (KMBI)232StudentsUSASex, age, education
Tang, 2016 [81]Monetary Intelligence, The Love of Money791Business studentsUSAAge, sex, education, grades
Moloi, 2018 [82]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)217AdultsSouth AfricaMaterialism
Tang et al., 2018 [48]The Love of Money229MBA students/investorsChinaAge, gender
Tang et al., 2018 [83]The Love of Money6586Managers32 NationsGender, age, GDP
Tang et al., 2018 [84]The Love of Money6382Managers31 NationsAge, gender, education, private sector
Harnish et al., 2018 [20]Money Attitude Scale (MAS), Money Ethics Scale (MES)265AdultsUSACompulsive buying, pain of paying
Lay & Furnham, 2019 [9]New Money Attitudes Questionnaire
(NMAQ)
268AdultsUKSex, age, ideology, work success
Furnham and
Murphy, 2019 [62]
Money Mindset3285AdultsAustraliaSex, age, wealth, spend-save
Fenton-O’Creevy & Furnham, 2020 [85,86]Short Money Type Measure (SMTM)3500AdultsUKAge, income, conscientiousness
Furnham and
Grover, 2020 [43]
New Money Behaviour Quiz (NMBQ)402AdultsUKSex, age, wealth
Sleczka et al., 2020 [63]German Scale of Money Attitudes (SMAG)2584, 105AdultsGermanyGambling disorder
Rimple, 2020 [87]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)243AdultsIndiaGender
LeBaron et al., 2020 [26]New Adolescent Money Attitude Scales
(NAMAS)
437Children and their parentsUKParent’s financial education
Ong et al., 2021 [88]Money Attitude Scale (MAS)356StudentsMalaysiaCompulsive buying, materialism
Furnham and Horne, 2021 [89]Money Madness Questionnaire256AdultsUKFinancial literacy
Kim, 2021 [23]Theory of Planned Behaviour395AdultsUSAPerceived behavioural control, subjective norm, and financial self-efficacy
de Almeida et al., 2021 [16]Various questionnaires365AdultsPortugalIncome, debt, household population
Kappes et al., 2021 [27]Spending Implies Wealth (SIW)2232, 1600, 1000AdultsUKAge, sex, income
Chaudary et al., 2022 [90]The Love of Money277Active equity traders (money managers, investors)Karachi, Lahore, PakistanDemographic variables, status, current income, future inheritance
Franzen & Mader, 2022 [64]Importance of Money Scale (IMS)510, 2914Students, adultsSwitzerlandEnvironmental concern, consumption behaviour, donation behaviour
Tang et al., 2022 [91]The Love of Money227InvestorsShanghai, Beijing, Shenzhen, Chongqing, ChinaSalary, domicile
Chen et al., 2022 [92]Spending Implies Income2541AdultsUSAMultiple income sources, status-seeking motivation
Lozza et al., 2022 [25]Short Money Type Measure (SMTM)186Financial advisors and their clientsItalyEmotional associations
Tang et al., 2023 [93]The Love of Money6500Managers32 NationsGender, education, GDP, CPI
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Furnham, A. Measuring Money Attitudes and Beliefs. Encyclopedia 2025, 5, 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/encyclopedia5020078

AMA Style

Furnham A. Measuring Money Attitudes and Beliefs. Encyclopedia. 2025; 5(2):78. https://doi.org/10.3390/encyclopedia5020078

Chicago/Turabian Style

Furnham, Adrian. 2025. "Measuring Money Attitudes and Beliefs" Encyclopedia 5, no. 2: 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/encyclopedia5020078

APA Style

Furnham, A. (2025). Measuring Money Attitudes and Beliefs. Encyclopedia, 5(2), 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/encyclopedia5020078

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop