Next Article in Journal
Performance Evaluation of Balcony Designs for Mitigating Ground Level Noise
Next Article in Special Issue
Enhancing Speaker Recognition Models with Noise-Resilient Feature Optimization Strategies
Previous Article in Journal
Influence of the Gain–Bandwidth of the Front-End Amplifier on the Performance of a QEPAS Sensor
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Effect of the Frequency and Energetic Content of Broadband Noise on the Lombard Effect and Speech Intelligibility
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels Produced by Adult Heritage Speakers of Albanian

by
Georgios P. Georgiou
*,†,‡ and
Aretousa Giannakou
Department of Languages and Literature, University of Nicosia, Makedonitissis 46, 2417 Nicosia, Cyprus
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Director of the Phonetic Lab.
Acoustics 2024, 6(1), 257-271; https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics6010014
Submission received: 20 December 2023 / Revised: 26 February 2024 / Accepted: 8 March 2024 / Published: 10 March 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Developments in Acoustic Phonetic Research)

Abstract

:
Investigating heritage language (HL)-contact effects on the dominant language has received limited attention despite its importance in understanding the dynamic interplay between linguistic systems in situations of bilingualism. This study compares the acoustic characteristics of Greek vowels produced by heritage speakers (HSs) of Albanian and monolingual Greek speakers, aiming to identify potential differences and explain them. The participants were adult second-generation HSs of Albanian with Greek as their dominant language, born and raised in Greece. A control group of age-matched monolingual Greek speakers was included for comparison purposes. All participants engaged in a controlled speech production task, with the data segmented to extract acoustic values pertaining to the first three formants and the duration of Greek vowels. Bayesian regression models were employed for the subsequent statistical analysis. The results demonstrated differences in the first three formants of certain vowels and the duration of all vowels. These differences can be attributed to the crosslinguistic effect of HL on the dominant language, as well as the interplay between the dynamic and internalized language system of the speakers and the complex effect of the sociophonetic context. These outcomes contribute to the hypothesis positing the emergence of deflected phonetic categories among a distinctive group of bilinguals, namely HSs. Furthermore, this study underscores the significance of a comprehensive exploration of the sociophonetic context of HSs for a nuanced understanding of their phonetic patterns.

1. Introduction

Heritage speakers (HSs) are simultaneous or early sequential bilinguals raised in homes where a language other than the dominant societal language is spoken (see, e.g., [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]). A language naturalistically acquired at home within a broader sociolinguistic context where “it is not a dominant language of the larger (national) society” is a heritage language (HL) [4] (p. 156). Although this term may refer to cases of languages spoken in various sociopolitical contexts by different ethnolinguistic communities (see [8,9,10]), the definition adopted here is that given by Montrul [11] (p. 399): a HL “is a sociopolitically minority and/or minoritized language acquired as the first or one of the first languages in a bilingual or multilingual context”.
Mobility of people in the context of migration gives rise to immigrant HLs. HSs who are simultaneous bilinguals acquire the HL as a minority language alongside the majority language of the society, while in sequential bilingualism, the HL is acquired first and the dominant societal language subsequently in early childhood [7,9]. Under each scenario, by the time they reach adulthood, typical HSs are dominant in the language of their broader speech (national) community, such that heritage bilingualism is commonly “heavily imbalanced in favor of the dominant language” [7] (p. 5). While HSs qualify as native speakers of both their languages [12,13], the ultimate attainment of many adult HSs in the HL is regarded as the result of incomplete [3,9] differential [2] or divergent acquisition [6,7]. Thus, HSs comprise a special subset of bilinguals with individual variability standing as the hallmark of these populations [9].
Bilingualism, including heritage bilingualism, can be viewed as a continuum, where monolingual speakers of each language represent the two extremes (e.g., [5,6,7,13,14,15,16]). Research on HLs has consistently focused on comparing the linguistic outcomes of HSs in their HL against monolingual norms. Most of these studies consider only the HL under the assumption that the majority language, as HSs’ dominant language, is stronger and hence unaffected by language-contact effects [9]. Indeed, studies on adult HSs have demonstrated that linguistic patterns of HSs’ dominant (societal) language are similar to those of monolinguals, while differences are observed when heritage performance is compared to the monolingual baseline of the said HL. In the domain of phonology, there is evidence substantiating that HSs can be (mis)identified as monolingual native speakers of their dominant language (e.g., [17,18]). However, bilingualism involves crosslinguistic influence, which in turn implies a bidirectional (rather than unidirectional) influence between the languages (see, e.g., [19], and references therein). Such an interaction may result in mutually restructuring of the two languages across linguistic development. With regard to phonological competence, in particular, crosslinguistic influence may give rise to an approximation of the two phonological systems (e.g., [20,21,22]).
The current study investigates how heritage bilinguals (re)organize their phonetic systems by examining the acoustic characteristics of vowels produced by adult HSs of Albanian in their dominant language, namely Standard Modern Greek (henceforth Greek). The aim is to identify potential differences from monolingual Greek productions and to account for the findings.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Phonology and Heritage Language

According to the premises of several speech models, such as the Speech Learning Model [23], both the first language (L1) and the second language (L2) are found in a common phonological/phonetic space creating bidirectional effects from L1 to L2 [23,24,25] and from L2 to L1 [16,26,27]. Similarly, the speech of HSs presents crosslinguistic influence, with evidence of the dominant majority language affecting the HL and vice versa [28]. HL pronunciation within ethnic minority settings demonstrates notable distinctions when compared to that of monolingual individuals, with discernible differences emerging not only between the patterns of first-generation immigrants and monolinguals but also between first-generation immigrants and HSs of second or third-generations (e.g., [29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36]). A dominance shift usually happens in second-generation immigrants (HSs), when formal education in the majority language starts around the age of 6 (e.g., [9]).
In the context of heritage bilingualism, research on dominant language pronunciation is more limited. When a weaker language (i.e., a HL) comes into contact with a dominant language, speakers’ pronunciation and accent may influence their dominant (societal) language to some extent. This phenomenon is often referred to as substrate influence. Substrate characteristics have been demonstrated to strengthen and expand existing patterns in the dominant language, frequently becoming notable regional traits [37]. For example, Natvig [38] evaluated claims regarding the impact of Norwegian as an HL substrate on the development of American English dialects in the Upper Midwest region of the USA. The author emphasized the production of monophthongal English /o/ in words such as “goat” and “boat”, which is found in the dialects spoken in Minnesota and North Dakota. It has been suggested that Scandinavian long [oː] vowel is the source of English /o/ due to the high level of emigration of Scandinavian people into these areas. The results seem to verify that the productions of Norwegian HSs for this vowel were primarily monophthongal. McCarthy et al. [39] examined the production of Sylheti and English stops and vowels by London-Bengali speakers. The participants were early and late arrivals from Bangladesh to the UK with English and Sylheti as their dominant languages, respectively, second-generation HSs with English as their dominant language, and monolingual Sylheti and English speakers. The authors used a picture forced-choice task to elicit productions targeting the L1 and L2 vowels. The results showed that late arrivals produced the Sylheti stops and vowels in a native-like manner, while their English productions were influenced by their dominant language. Early arrivals and second-generation speakers produced Sylheti vowels in a native-like manner but not stops. The English productions of both groups were similar to those of English L1 speakers. However, there were some subtle differences between the English productions of Sylheti-speaking groups and English L1 speakers. Specifically, a lower F2 for vowel /i/ was observed, as well as a different duration for vowel /ɒ/.
A crucial question in bilingual studies, including the study of HLs, is how similar the phonetic categories of bilinguals are to those of monolinguals of the dominant societal language. To address this question, Mack [40] examined the perception and production of English consonants and vowels by early English–French bilinguals and English monolinguals in the USA. The author found that bilinguals did not differ from English monolinguals in the discrimination and production of /d/ and /t/ and the production of /ɪ/, but differed in the identification of the /d–t/ and /i–ɪ/ contrasts and the production of /i/. Mack concluded that only certain aspects of the phonetic system are similar between early bilinguals and monolinguals in the dominant language. Flege [23] proposes that even if L2 phonetic categories are established in bilinguals, they may not be able to produce the sounds in the same way as L1 speakers. This is because as long as both vowel systems are found in a common phonological space, bilinguals strive to maintain phonetic distance between the sounds, thereby creating deflected phonetic categories to accommodate the L2 sounds. Another explanation for such a divergence is the possibility that bilinguals use features not exploited in their L1 to distinguish the L2 sounds. MacLeod et al. [41] compared early Canadian French–Canadian English bilinguals and English and French monolinguals in the production of high vowels. They reported that bilinguals retained distinct categories across their two languages for similar vowels and they produced vowels in a monolingual-like manner. However, a more careful examination of the findings indicated divergences between bilinguals and monolinguals in some productions.
Baker and Trofimovich [20] examined the role of the age of onset of bilingualism in the organization of phonetic systems in early and late Korean–English bilinguals. This study compared these bilingual groups to English and Korean monolinguals, focusing on the phonetic systems in both English and Korean vowels. It was found that early bilinguals manifested a bidirectional influence in their two languages, producing distinct acoustic realizations of vowels in both languages compared to monolingual productions. This was not the case for late bilinguals, whose performance indicated a unidirectional influence of L1 on L2 as their L2 vowel productions were found to be affected by the acoustic properties of their L1. According to the authors, the extent and directionality of crosslinguistic influence in early and late bilinguals depends on the degree of acoustic similarity between L1 and L2 vowels and the length of their exposure to each language. Guion [21] reported similar results in investigating the production of vowels by simultaneous, early, mid, and late Quichua–Spanish bilinguals dominant in Spanish. More specifically, simultaneous bilinguals could divide their vowel space in a more refined manner to accommodate the vowels of their two languages compared to early bilinguals. Additionally, among bilinguals who acquired knowledge of Spanish vowels (that is, simultaneous, early, and some mid bilinguals), Quichua vowels demonstrated a higher position compared to late bilinguals, suggesting a discernible influence of Spanish on their native Quichua vowel productions. The author concluded that the particular sociolinguistic context in Ecuador, and specifically the city of Otavalo where local people speak both Quichua and Spanish, gives rise to contact-induced change through the intergenerational transmission of bilinguals’ reorganized phonetic systems.
The current study aims to investigate the acoustic characteristics of Greek vowels, namely the first three formant frequencies (F1, F2, F3), and duration, produced by second-generation adult HSs of Albanian who live in Athens, Greece. The Greek vowel production of Albanian HSs was compared to that of age-matched monolingual Greek speakers. The objective was to explore whether and to what extent differences can be discerned in the Greek performance of Albanian–Greek bilinguals (i.e., in their dominant language) when compared to the monolingual Greek baseline and to account for the findings. It was expected that there might be differences at least for some acoustic aspects pertaining to some vowels, as expounded below, based on previous evidence on language contact and consequent crosslinguistic influence. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the acoustic patterns of second-generation Albanian HSs in Greece in their dominant language.

2.2. Albanian as a HL in Greece

Following the fall of communism in the early 1990s, a substantial migration wave started from Albania to Greece, given the adjacency of the two countries. Such a mobility persists to this day, albeit with diminished intensity. Although precise figures are hard to acquire due to ongoing fluctuations, more than 50% of immigrants residing in Greece today can trace their origin to Albania (480,804 individuals in the 2011 census) [42,43]. Consequently, Albanians constitute the largest ethnic minority and immigrant community in Greece, making up approximately 5% of the total population [44,45]. Children born to Albanian parents grow up in bilingual environments with Albanian (and Greek) spoken at home and Greek used at school and most other social settings. These speakers are thus HSs of Albanian, who eventually become dominant bilinguals in Greek. The racist character of Greek society [46,47], entailing the stigmatization of Albanians in Greece [44], interrelates with the high level of assimilation of this ethnolinguistic group into the Greek social fabric. Such conditions have led to a gradual language shift towards Greek, despite the significant demographic presence of Albanians in this social context [44,48]. Ndoci [45], for instance, reported that the production of Albanian ethnolectal phonological features in Greek is negatively stigmatized by both Greeks and Albanians. The tendency towards language shift is particularly evident in adult second-generation immigrants, i.e., HSs of Albanian, who use predominantly Greek in everyday life [44,49,50]. However, Albanian HSs seem to acknowledge their dual identity and construct complex identities (see [46,47,49]).

2.3. Greek and Albanian Vowel Systems

The Standard Modern Greek vowel system includes the five vowel qualities, /i e a o u/ [51,52]. Greek does not differentiate between long and short vowels [53], although stress alters vowel duration, with stressed vowels being longer than unstressed vowels [54]. Greek vowels /i/ and /u/ are described as close front unrounded and close back rounded, respectively [55]. Greek /a/ is between open and open-mid central unrounded vowels, while Greek unrounded /e/ and rounded /o/ are between close-mid and open-mid vowels [56].
The Standard Albanian vowel system based on the Tosk variety typically consists of the seven vowel qualities, /i y e ə a o u/ [57]. Albanian /i/, /y/, and /u/ are described as close front unrounded, close front rounded, and close back rounded vowels, respectively. Albanian /e/, /ə/, and /o/ are (close-)mid front unrounded, mid central unrounded, and (close-)mid back rounded vowels, respectively. However, /ə/ presents great variation across different speakers and contexts. It has been suggested that it comprises an open-mid unrounded vowel, phonologically transcribed as /ɜ/ [58]. In addition, vowels /e/ and /o/ exhibit free variation, occasionally manifesting as more open-mid sounds [ɛ] and [ɔ] [59]. Lastly, Albanian /a/ is described as an open central-to-back unrounded vowel [57].
To investigate the acoustic features of L1 Albanian and L1 Greek vowels, five Albanian speakers (nfemales = 3) and 10 Greek speakers (nfemales = 7) participated in a production task in which they produced their L1 vowels. The Albanian speakers lived permanently in central and central-southern Albania and were in the age range of 18–29 (Mage = 23, SD = 5.05). All of them were native speakers of Standard Albanian. They were asked to produce carrier phrases including words targeting their L1 vowels. The words were in the context of /fVf/ (V = vowel) as part of the phrase “E the /fVf/ prap” (“You said <word> again”) (7 vowels × 2 repetitions each). The Greek speakers lived permanently in Athens, Greece and were in the age range of 21–33 (Mage = 26.3, SD = 4.57); their native language was Standard Modern Greek. They produced the phrase “Ípes /fVf/ páli” (“You said <word> again”), which also included words in the /fVf/ context (5 vowels × 2 repetitions each). For more information about the experimental procedure, see the Methodology section below. Figure 1 illustrates F1 and F2 for Albanian and Greek vowels as produced by native speakers of these languages. Table 1 shows the average F1, F2, F3, and duration values of Albanian and Greek vowels.
The crosslinguistic acoustic analysis of Albanian and Greek vowels demonstrates that the similar vowels between the two languages, namely /i e a o u/, occupy close spaces in the vowel space in terms of F1 and F2. Nevertheless, there are some acoustic differences. Specifically, Albanian /i/ is closer and further front than Greek /i/, while Albanian /a/ is further back than its Greek counterpart. Albanian /u/ is closer and further back than the corresponding Greek vowel. In terms of F3, Albanian vowel /e/ shares close values with the similar Greek vowel /e/. However, Albanian /o/ and /u/ have lower values compared to their Greek counterparts. The same applies to Albanian /a/ compared to Greek /a/. In addition, the durations of Albanian vowels appear to be considerably lower compared to the durations of Greek vowels. If crosslinguistic influence takes place between heritage Albanian and majority/societal Greek, the HSs will produce Greek vowels such as /i u/ with lower F1 values, /a u/ with lower F2 values, and /a o u/ with lower F3 values. Furthermore, the Greek vowels will be produced with remarkably shorter duration values.

3. Methodology

3.1. Participants

Twenty individuals participated in this study. The control group consisted of 10 Greek-speaking individuals of Greek origin, the same who participated in the production task (see Section 2.3). They were born and raised in Greece and lived permanently in Athens. The native variety of these speakers was Standard Modern Greek. The experimental group included another 10 Greek-speaking individuals of Albanian origin, living in Athens (nfemales = 3), with an age range of 20–34 (Mage = 27.3, SD = 4.59). They were all identified as simultaneous or early bilinguals of Standard Albanian and Standard Modern Greek and, more precisely, HSs of Albanian, dominant in Greek. The HSs were born to both Albanian parents, i.e., they were 100% minority in terms of ethnic/racial heritage, in line with Kupisch [60]. Their place of birth was either Greece (n = 5) or Albania (n = 5). In the latter case, they had moved to Greece before the age of 4. They had all completed at least 12 years of formal education in Greece and reported low or no literacy in Albanian, as well as no systematic use of other languages apart from Greek and Albanian in their everyday life. All participants had healthy vision and hearing and had never suffered from any cognitive or language disorders. Prior to their participation in the study, they were informed about the goal of the study and their rights, and they gave their written consent, according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

3.2. Materials

The materials consisted of five monosyllabic nonsense words in an /fVf/ context targeting each of the five Greek vowels. They were part of the carrier phrase “Ípes /fVf/ páli” (“You said /fVf/ again”). The chosen specific context is particularly advantageous because it centers on a vowel flanked by identical fricative consonants, /f/. This arrangement simplifies the segmentation process, allowing for a more focused examination of the vowels’ properties, free from the potential influence of different adjacent consonants.

3.3. Procedure

3.3.1. Production Test

Each participant completed the test individually in a quiet room. Phrases were shown on a piece of paper and participants were asked by the researcher to read them as if speaking to a friend. The phrases were written using the Standard Modern Greek orthography. Their productions were recorded using Tascam DR05 V2 at a 44.1 kHz sampling rate and saved as .wav files with a resolution of 24 bits. Each individual produced 10 phrases (5 vowels × 2 repetitions), with a total number of 200 phrases for all individuals. The phrases were randomized for each participant. Before the test, we ensured that participants could read the sentences properly.

3.3.2. Feature Extraction

The target words spoken by the speakers were extracted and processed in Praat [61] (for similar descriptions, see [62,63]). Spectrograms and waveforms were visually examined to identify key acoustic features, enabling the measurement of vowel boundaries, including formant frequencies and vocalic duration. To generate these analyses, we employed the following specific settings: a 0.025 positive window length, a 50 Hz pre-emphasis, and a spectrogram view range up to 5500 Hz. Frequencies were measured starting from the end of the noise of the preceding fricative consonant /f/ and the onset of the vowel (V) and concluding at the end of the vocalic periodicity and the onset of the second consonant /f/. Frequencies were measured at their midpoint, a position within the vowel segment where they were least influenced by adjacent sounds. Vowel durations were manually labeled by the first author, involving the determination of each vowel token’s start and end points. F1, F2, F3, and duration values were normalized using the z-score method due to their broad distribution across a wide range of values for all speakers [64].

3.4. Statistical Analysis

Bayesian regression models were employed for data analysis using the brms package [65] in R [66]. Bayesian models were favored over conventional frequentist approaches because of their capability to handle small sample sizes [67,68]. F1, F2, F3 and duration were the dependent variables, including the extracted normalized values (expect duration) from speech outputs. Vowel (i, e, a, o, u) (reference level: a) and group (heritage, monolingual) (reference level: heritage) served as fixed factors, while participant served as a random factor. Another notable advantage of Bayesian statistics lies in its ability to incorporate prior knowledge gleaned from previous research. This pre-existing information is integrated into the models using prior distributions. Due to the exploratory nature of this study and the absence of preconceived assumptions about the behavior of data parameters, weakly informative priors were employed. These priors were specified as a Student’s t-distribution with 3 degrees of freedom, a mean of 0, and a standard deviation of 2.5 [69,70,71]; Figure 2 shows the density of the selected priors. After fitting the model with these priors, posterior predictive checks were used to evaluate the fit of the models to the observed data. As seen in Figure 3, the analyses demonstrated that the replicated data (yrep) closely resembled the observed data (y) in terms of their distribution, patterns, and variability, suggesting that all models provided good fits to the data. To assess the probability of the test hypotheses relative to their alternatives, we utilized the evidence ratio (ER) as a quantified measure. An ER of >10 is deemed strong evidence in favor of a hypothesis, while an ER of <0.1 is considered strong evidence against a hypothesis [72]. For those familiar with frequentist statistics, an ER < 19 is approximately equivalent to α = 0.05 [73]. We also mention the posterior probabilities (PP).

4. Results

As shown in Figure 4 and Figure 5, Greek vowels were produced in a similar manner by both HSs and monolinguals. However, some differences occurred. The most considerable one pertained to /a/, which had a lower F1 (i.e., occupying a closer position) and a lower F2 (i.e., occupying a further back position) in HSs. In addition, vowel /u/ appeared to have lower F2 values in HSs. With respect to F3, /a o u/ seemed to have lower values in HSs. Furthermore, as seen in Figure 6, the duration of Greek vowels was lower in HSs compared to monolinguals. Hypothesis testing assessed whether the acoustic values of Greek vowels produced by HSs were lower compared to corresponding values produced by monolingual speakers. The results indicated strong evidence that Greek /e a o/ (ER = 15.74—inf, PP = 0.94–1) exhibited lower values for HSs compared to monolinguals in terms of F1. Furthermore, there was strong evidence that Greek /i a u/ were produced with lower F2 values by HSs (ER = 12.61–75.92, PP = 0.93–0.94). In terms of F3, strong evidence was found that Greek /o u/ demonstrated lower values in HSs (ER = 21.86–55.34, PP = 0.96–0.98). Moreover, there was strong evidence that the duration of all Greek vowels produced by HSs was lower compared to monolinguals (ER = 15.33–570.43, PP = 0.94–1.00). The results of the main analysis and the hypothesis testing are presented in Table 2 and Table 3, respectively.

5. Discussion

This study examined the acoustic characteristics of Greek vowels (F1, F2, F3, and duration) produced by second-generation HSs of Albanian(Albanian-Greek bilinguals), and monolingual Greek speakers. We were interested in investigating the phonetic categories in the vowel space of bilinguals and monolinguals and explaining the source of potential differences between the two populations. The statistical analysis was conducted using Bayesian regression models. We employed for the first time an acoustic framework to study Albanian and Greek.
A crucial question was addressed regarding potential acoustic differences in the production of Greek vowels between HSs and monolingual speakers. Although HSs maintained clear boundaries between the different Greek vowel categories as seen in their productions, some acoustic aspects differed from those of monolingual Greek speakers. Specifically, vowels /e a o/ were produced with lower F1 values by HSs. Similarly, vowels /i a u/ were produced with lower F2 values, and vowels /o u/ were produced with lower F3 values. All vowels produced by HSs exhibited shorter durations compared to those of monolingual speakers. This shows that each Greek vowel was produced with at least two different acoustic features.
Another aim of this study was to identify the source of acoustic differences between HSs and monolingual speakers in their Greek performance. The lower F2 values of Greek /a u/ in HSs compared to monolingual speakers can be regarded as a crosslinguistic influence since Albanian /a u/ are further back than their Greek counterparts. In terms of F3, HSs appeared to have transferred phonetic patterns from their HL, resulting in lower values for Greek /o u/. Notably, this is consistent with the lower F3 values found in Albanian /o u/ compared to their Greek counterparts. Furthermore, the production of Greek vowels with a shorter duration by HSs can be explained as a crosslinguistic influence from Albanian, as Albanian vowels do have considerably shorter durations than Greek vowels.
These findings not only validate most of our initial predictions but also align with the notion that crosslinguistic influence operates from the nondominant language to the dominant language (see also [26,27]). In our study, the speakers’ HL, which is not the dominant language of the society in which they reside and not the primary language in their everyday lives (albeit one of their L1s), was found to influence certain acoustic features of their dominant language based on the acoustic analysis of vowels produced by Albanian native speakers. Similar effects have been observed in previous studies. For instance, Natvik [38] concluded that English /o/ found in the speech of Norwegian HSs, who reside in the Upper Midwest region of the USA, originates from the Scandinavian long [oː]. According to Flege [23], crosslinguistic effects arise from the coexistence of the two languages within a common phonological space. Bilingual speakers strive to maintain auditory contrast between their L1 and L2 vowels and, consequently, their productions may differ from those of monolinguals of a given language. In our study, despite Albanian HSs also being L1 speakers of Greek, their vowel categories did not completely overlap with those of monolingual Greek speakers in some acoustic features. This aligns with Flege’s [23] hypothesis regarding deflected phonetic categories for bilinguals, which therefore extends to the context of simultaneous/early (heritage) bilingualism.
Nevertheless, not all observed phonetic productions can be attributed to crosslinguistic influence. In particular, the lower F1 values of Greek /e a o/ and the lower F2 values of Greek /i/ in HSs do not seem to simply result from crosslinguistic effects. This is because the acoustically similar Albanian /e a o/ exhibit comparable values to Greek /e a o/. In addition, Albanian /i/ demonstrates higher F2 values than its Greek counterpart. These findings align with Flege’s [23] hypothesis that bilinguals develop deflected categories that do not solely reflect crosslinguistic influence since bilinguals’ representations are based on different features or feature weights as compared to those of monolinguals. This divergence may stem from the fact that HSs exploit phonetic features not present in their nondominant HL. These features may possibly originate from the speakers’ sociophonetic context, encompassing phonetic features common in their social environment. One possibility is that HSs acquire both their languages through input provided to a large extent by their parents and relatives who are first-generation immigrants with L1 Albanian and L2 Greek. The parental/environmental input may exhibit divergence from monolingual norms in both Greek and Albanian: in Greek due to late L2 acquisition and in Albanian due to L1 attrition. Thus, the abovementioned differences attested in the phonetic productions of HSs may potentially result from differential input (see, e.g., [74]). Such a possibility requires additional investigation examining the performance of the parental generation, a research direction that we aim to address in future work. An alternative explanation may be related to language-internal evolution phenomena of variation and variability, an approach which points to distinguishing variation from deviation [75]. Regarding the latter account, the adoption of these features by HSs may arise from the potential for language change. This phenomenon can impact not only vocabulary and grammar but also phonetic patterns, as speakers adjust to the linguistic features of their contact languages, as suggested by Natvik [38]. Considering the relatively long-term contact between Albanian and Greek speakers in a bilingual community, it is plausible that language contact over time is leading to the emergence of new phonetic features among Albanian HSs in Greece. While challenging to test within the scope of this study, this hypothesis presents an intriguing avenue for future research.

6. Conclusions

This study compared the acoustic characteristics of Greek vowels as produced by groups of adult Greek-speaking HSs of Albanian and monolingual Greek speakers, both residing in Greece. Some acoustic aspects were found to be different between the two populations as a result of crosslinguistic influence and the potential effect of sociophonetic factors. Future studies can investigate to a greater extent the sociolinguistic background of the speakers to gain deeper insights into the source(s) of phonetic differences and compare vowel production between Greek speakers and other HL populations such as first-generation immigrants. They can also assess the role of individual variation among participants in their productions. The findings of this study shed light on the complex interplay between HL acquisition and dominant language proficiency among Albanian-Greek bilinguals, contributing to our understanding of bilingual speakers’ phonetic patterns in situations of intense language contact.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.P.G. and A.G.; methodology, G.P.G.; software, G.P.G.; validation, G.P.G.; formal analysis, G.P.G.; investigation, G.P.G. and A.G.; resources, G.P.G. and A.G.; data collection, A.G.; data curation, G.P.G. and A.G.; writing—original draft preparation, G.P.G.; writing—review and editing, G.P.G. and A.G.; visualization, G.P.G.; supervision, G.P.G. and A.G.; project administration, G.P.G. and A.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study’s protocol was approved by the Department of Languages and Literature, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Nicosia.

Informed Consent Statement

All individuals gave their written consent to participate, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Phonetic Lab of the University of Nicosia. We would like to thank the speakers for their participation in this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aalberse, S.; Backus, A. Pieter Muysken. In Heritage Languages; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  2. Kupisch, T.; Rothman, J. Terminology Matters! Why Difference Is Not Incompleteness and How Early Child Bilinguals Are Heritage Speakers. Int. J. Bil. 2016, 22, 564–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Montrul, S. Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-Examining the Age Factor; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  4. Rothman, J. Understanding the Nature and Outcomes of Early Bilingualism: Romance Languages as Heritage Languages. Int. J. Bil. 2009, 13, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rothman, J.; Bayram, F.; DeLuca, V.; Di Pisa, G.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Gharibi, K.; Hao, J.; Kolb, N.; Kubota, M.; Kupisch, T.; et al. Monolingual Comparative Normativity in Bilingualism Research Is out of “Control”: Arguments and Alternatives. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2023, 44, 316–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Polinsky, M. Heritage Languages and Their Speakers; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  7. Polinsky, M.; Scontras, G. Understanding Heritage Languages. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 23, 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kupisch, T. Heritage Languages in Europe. In The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages and Linguistics; Montrul, S., Polinsky, M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021; pp. 45–68. [Google Scholar]
  9. Montrul, S.A. The Acquisition of Heritage Languages; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  10. Montrul, S.; Polinsky, M. Introduction: Heritage languages, heritage speakers, heritage linguistics. In The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages and Linguistics; Montrul, S., Polinsky, M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  11. Montrul, S. Heritage Languages: Language Acquired, Language Lost, Language Regained. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2023, 9, 399–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Rothman, J.; Treffers-Daller, J. A Prolegomenon to the Construct of the Native Speaker: Heritage Speaker Bilinguals Are Natives Too! Appl. Linguist. 2014, 35, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wiese, H.; Alexiadou, A.; Allen, S.; Bunk, O.; Gagarina, N.; Iefremenko, K.; Martynova, M.; Pashkova, T.; Rizou, V.; Schroeder, C.; et al. Heritage Speakers as Part of the Native Language Continuum. Front. Psychol. 2022, 12, 717973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Polinsky, M.; Kagan, O. Heritage Languages: In the ?Wild? And in the Classroom. Lang. Ling. Comp. 2007, 1, 368–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Zyzik, E. Toward a prototype model of the heritage language learner. In Innovative Strategies for Heritage Language Teaching: A Practical Guide for the Classroom; Fairclough, M., Beaudrie, S.M., Eds.; Georgetown University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 19–38. [Google Scholar]
  16. Grosjean, F. Bilingual and Monolingual Language Modes. In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics; Chapelle, C.A., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  17. Kupisch, T.; Barton, D.; Hailer, K.; Klaschik, E.; Stangen, I.; Lein, T.; van de Weijer, J. Foreign Accent in Adult Simultaneous Bilinguals. Her. Lang. J. 2014, 11, 123–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lloyd-Smith, A.; Einfeldt, M.; Kupisch, T. Italian-German Bilinguals: The Effects of Heritage Language Use on Accent in Early-Acquired Languages. Int. J. Biling. 2020, 24, 289–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kupisch, T. Introduction: Recent Developments in Early Bilingualism. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 653–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Baker, W.; Trofimovich, P. Interaction of Native- and Second-Language Vowel System(S) in Early and Late Bilinguals. Lang. Spee. 2005, 48, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Guion, S.G. The Vowel Systems of Quichua-Spanish Bilinguals. Phonetica 2003, 60, 98–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lleó, C. Aspects of the Phonology of Spanish as a Heritage Language: From Incomplete Acquisition to Transfer. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2017, 21, 732–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Flege, J.E. Second language speech learning: Theory, findings and problems. In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Theoretical and Methodological Issues; Strange, W., Ed.; York Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1995; pp. 233–277. [Google Scholar]
  24. Flege, J.E. Age of learning and second language speech. In Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis; Birdsong, D., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1999; pp. 111–142. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kuhl, P.K.; Tsao, F.-M.; Liu, H.-M. Foreign-Language Experience in Infancy: Effects of Short-Term Exposure and Social Interaction on Phonetic Learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 9096–9101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Grosjean, F. Neurolinguists, Beware! The Bilingual Is Not Two Monolinguals in One Person. Br. Lang. 1989, 36, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Major, R.C. Losing English as a First Language. Mod. Lang. J. 1992, 76, 190–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Shea, C. Dominance, Proficiency, and Spanish Heritage Speakers’ Production of English and Spanish Vowels. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 2017, 41, 123–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Amengual, M. Acoustic Correlates of the Spanish Tap-Trill Contrast: Heritage and L2 Spanish Speakers. Herit. Lang. J. 2016, 13, 88–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Au, T.K.; Knightly, L.M.; Jun, S.-A.; Oh, J.S. Overhearing a Language during Childhood. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 238–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Asherov, D.; Fishman, A.; Cohen, E.-G. Vowel Reduction in Israeli Heritage Russian. Herit. Lang. J. 2016, 13, 113–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Chang, C.B.; Yao, Y.; Haynes, E.F.; Rhodes, R. Production of Phonetic and Phonological Contrast by Heritage Speakers of Mandarin. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 3964–3980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Evans, B.; Mistry, A.; Moreiras, C. An acoustic study of first- and second generation Gujurati immigrants in Wembley: Evidence for accent convergence? In Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XVI), Saarbrücken, Germany, 6–10 August 2007; pp. 1741–1744. [Google Scholar]
  34. Flores, C.; Rato, A. Global Accent in the Portuguese Speech of Heritage Returnees. Herit. Lang. J. 2016, 13, 161–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Flores, C.; Rinke, E.; Rato, A. Comparing the Outcomes of Early and Late Acquisition of European Portuguese: An Analysis of Morpho-Syntactic and Phonetic Performance. Herit. Lang. J. 2017, 14, 124–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Mayr, R.; Siddika, A. Inter-Generational Transmission in a Minority Language Setting: Stop Consonant Production by Bangladeshi Heritage Children and Adults. Int. J. Biling. 2016, 22, 255–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Salmons, J.; Purnell, T. Contact and the development of American English. In The Handbook of Language Contact; Hickey, R., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010; pp. 454–477. [Google Scholar]
  38. Natvig, D. Heritage Norwegian Vowel Phonology and English Dialect Formation. Herit. Lang. J. 2016, 13, 245–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. McCarthy, K.M.; Evans, B.G.; Mahon, M. Acquiring a Second Language in an Immigrant Community: The Production of Sylheti and English Stops and Vowels by London-Bengali Speakers. J. Phon. 2013, 41, 344–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mack, M. Consonant and Vowel Perception and Production: Early English-French Bilinguals and English Monolinguals. Percept. Psychophys. 1989, 46, 187–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. MacLeod, A.A.N.; Stoel-Gammon, C.; Wassink, A.B. Production of High Vowels in Canadian English and Canadian French: A Comparison of Early Bilingual and Monolingual Speakers. J. Phon. 2009, 37, 374–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Magos, K. “To send a letter to my grandparents”: Learning Albanian as a heritage language in the Albanian complementary school of Volos. In Heritage Language Education in Greece and Cyprus; Gogonas, N., Ed.; Cambridge Scholars Publishing: Newcastle upon Tyne, UK, 2023; pp. 36–56. [Google Scholar]
  43. Mattheoudakis, M.; Chatzidaki, A.; Maligkoudi, C. Heritage Language Classes and Bilingual Competence: The Case of Albanian Immigrant Children in Greece. Int. J. Biln. Ed. Biling. 2017, 23, 1019–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gogonas, N.; Michail, D. Ethnolinguistic Vitality, Language Use and Social Integration amongst Albanian Immigrants in Greece. J. Mult. Mult. Dev. 2014, 36, 198–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ndoci, R. Albanians in Greece and the social meaning of ethnolectal features in L2 Greek. In Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 6, USA, Virtual, 7–10 January 2021; pp. 906–912. [Google Scholar]
  46. Archakis, A. The Continuum of Identities in Immigrant Students’ Narratives in Greece. Narrat. Inq. 2022, 32, 393–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Archakis, A. Immigrant Voices in Students’ Essay Texts: Between Assimilation and Pride. Discourse Soc. 2014, 25, 297–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gogonas, N. Language Shift in Second Generation Albanian Immigrants in Greece. J. Mult. Mult. Dev. 2009, 30, 95–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Androulakis, G.; Gkaintartzi, A.; Kitsiou, R.; Tsioli, S. Parents-Schools’ communication and Albanian as a heritage language in Greece. In Handbook of Research and Practice in Heritage Language Education; Trifonas, P.P., Aravossitas, T., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 521–538. [Google Scholar]
  50. Iliopoulou, A. Heritage Language Phonotactics: Word-Medial Cluster Syllabification in Albanian Heritage Speakers in Greece. Master’s Thesis, University of Crete, Crete, Greece, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  51. Georgiou, G.P. Speakers of Different L1 Dialects with Acoustically Proximal Vowel Systems Present with Similar Nonnative Speech Perception Abilities: Data from Greek Listeners of Dutch. Speech Commun. 2023, 150, 32–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Topintzi, N. Phonetics and Phonology: An Introduction, with Special Focus on English and Greek; Kallipos, Open Academic Editions; NTUA: Zografou, Greece, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  53. Georgiou, G.P.; Themistocleous, C. Vowel Learning in Diglossic Settings: Evidence from Arabic-Greek Learners. Int. J. Biling. 2021, 25, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Botinis, A.; Fourakis, M.; Panagiotopoulou, N.; Pouli, K. Greek vowel durations and prosodic interactions. Glossologia 2001, 13, 105–127. [Google Scholar]
  55. Arvaniti, A. Greek phonetics: The state of the art. J. Greek Linguist. 2007, 8, 97–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fourakis, M.; Botinis, A.; Katsaiti, M. Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels. Phonetica 1999, 56, 28–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Moosmüller, S.; Granser, T. The Vowels of Standard Albanian. In Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS2003), Barcelona, Spain, 3–9 August 2003; Solé, M.J., Recasens, D., Romero, J., Eds.; pp. 659–662. [Google Scholar]
  58. Coretta, S.; Riverin-Coutlée, J.; Kapia, E.; Nichols, S. Northern Tosk Albanian. J. Int. Phon. Ass. 2023, 53, 1122–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Buchholz, O.; Fiedler, W. Albanische Grammatik; Verlag Enzyklopädie: Leipzig, Germany, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kupisch, T. A new term for a better distinction? A view from the higher end of the proficiency scale. Theor. Ling. 2013, 39, 203–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. 2023. Available online: http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ (accessed on 1 December 2023).
  62. Georgiou, G.P. Comparison of the prediction accuracy of machine learning algorithms in crosslinguistic vowel classification. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 15594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Georgiou, G.P.; Dimitriou, D. Perception of Dutch Vowels by Cypriot Greek Listeners: To What Extent Can Listeners’ Patterns Be Predicted by Acoustic and Perceptual Similarity? Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2023, 85, 2459–2474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Hussain, Q.; Mielke, J. An acoustic and articulatory study of rhotic and rhotic-nasal vowels of Kalasha. J. Phon. 2021, 87, 101028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Bürkner, P.-C.; Gabry, J.; Weber, S.; Johnson, A.; Modrak, M.; Badr, H.S.; Weber, F.; Ben-Shachar, M.S.; Rabel, H.; Mills, S.C.; et al. brms: Bayesian Regression Models Using ‘Stan’; R Package Vignette: Madison, WI, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  66. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2023; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 December 2023).
  67. van de Schoot, R.; Depaoli, S. Bayesian Analyses: Where to Start and What to Report. Eur. Health Psychol. 2014, 16, 75–84. [Google Scholar]
  68. Georgiou, G.P. Bayesian models are better than frequentist models in identifying differences in small datasets comprising phonetic data. arXiv 2023, arXiv:2312.01146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Gelman, A.; Lee, D.; Guo, J. Stan: A Probabilistic Programming Language for Bayesian Inference and Optimization. J. Ed. Beh. Stat. 2015, 40, 530–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Georgiou, G.P. The Acquisition of /ɪ/–/iː/ is Challenging: Perceptual and Production Evidence from Cypriot Greek Speakers of English. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Georgiou, G.P.; Giannakou, A. Discrimination of L2 vowel contrasts and the role of phonological short-term memory and nonverbal intelligence. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2024, 53, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Jeffreys, H. Theory of Probability, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1961. [Google Scholar]
  73. Milne, A.J.; Herff, S.A. The Perceptual Relevance of Balance, Evenness, and Entropy in Musical Rhythms. Cognition 2020, 203, 104233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Sorace, A. L1 Attrition in a Wider Perspective. Second. Lang. Res. 2019, 36, 203–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Flores, C.; Rinke, E. The Relevance of Language-Internal Variation in Predicting Heritage Language Grammars. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2020, 23, 25–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. F1 × F2 (normalized) of Albanian and Greek vowels produced by L1 speakers.
Figure 1. F1 × F2 (normalized) of Albanian and Greek vowels produced by L1 speakers.
Acoustics 06 00014 g001
Figure 2. Density of prior distributions for the fixed effects: β~t (3, 0, 2.5).
Figure 2. Density of prior distributions for the fixed effects: β~t (3, 0, 2.5).
Acoustics 06 00014 g002
Figure 3. Posterior predictive checks comparing the observed (y) and the replicated data (yrep) in each of the models.
Figure 3. Posterior predictive checks comparing the observed (y) and the replicated data (yrep) in each of the models.
Acoustics 06 00014 g003
Figure 4. F1 × F2 (normalized) of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Figure 4. F1 × F2 (normalized) of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Acoustics 06 00014 g004
Figure 5. Formant frequencies (normalized) and SE of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Figure 5. Formant frequencies (normalized) and SE of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Acoustics 06 00014 g005
Figure 6. Duration (in ms) and SE of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Figure 6. Duration (in ms) and SE of Greek vowels produced by HSs and monolingual speakers.
Acoustics 06 00014 g006
Table 1. Average normalized F1, F2, and F3 values (z-scores) and duration values (ms) of Albanian and Greek vowels. Parentheses show the SDs.
Table 1. Average normalized F1, F2, and F3 values (z-scores) and duration values (ms) of Albanian and Greek vowels. Parentheses show the SDs.
VowelsF1F2F3Duration
Albanian
i1.02 (0.38)1.54 (0.37)0.90 (0.93)137(33)
y−0.95 (0.39)0.49 (0.17)−0.66 (0.65)129(29)
e0.65 (0.60)0.73 (0.32)0.05 (1.16)136(29)
ə0.08 (0.45)−0.45 (0.26)−0.05 (0.60)136(27)
a1.61 (0.68)−0.36 (0.24)−0.55 (0.74)151(31)
o0.30 (0.43)−0.64 (1.12)0.35 (1.19)141(33)
u−0.72 (0.18)−1.27 (0.11)0.00 (0.96)119(31)
Greek
i−0.89 (0.29)1.48 (0.86)0.36 (1.17)149(34)
e0.53 (0.60)0.63 (0.80)−0.09 (1.15)171(36)
a1.88 (0.77)−0.07 (0.22)0.01 (1.33)186(36)
o0.23 (0.56)−0.79 (0.19)0.62 (1.06)174(32)
u−0.67 (0.35)−0.87 (0.31)0.51 (1.10)143(41)
Table 2. Results of the Bayesian regression models.
Table 2. Results of the Bayesian regression models.
βSEl—95% CI *u—95% CI *RhatBulk
ESS
Tail
ESS
F1Intercept0.920.140.641.181.008731530
vowele−0.910.11−1.13−0.711.0014652042
voweli−2.030.11−2.25−1.821.0015872219
vowelo−1.000.11−1.21−0.781.0015262300
vowelu−1.750.11−1.96−1.541.0016012208
groupmonolingual0.950.200.561.331.008601680
vowele:groupmonolingual−0.420.15−0.71−0.111.0014792280
voweli:groupmonolingual−0.730.16−1.03−0.421.0016352171
vowelo:groupmonolingual−0.640.16−0.95−0.341.0015672337
vowelu:groupmonolingual−0.790.16−1.10−0.481.0015321915
F2Intercept−0.40.11−0.60−0.191.0014162002
vowele1.090.130.831.351.0019402536
voweli1.670.131.421.931.0018342403
vowelo−0.430.13−0.68−0.171.0017672618
vowelu−0.700.13−0.96−0.451.0018062316
groupmonolingual0.330.150.040.621.0014502021
vowele:groupmonolingual−0.390.19−0.76−0.031.0021652599
voweli:groupmonolingual−0.120.18−0.490.241.0018392378
vowelo:groupmonolingual−0.290.19−0.650.081.0019002667
vowelu:groupmonolingual−0.10.19−0.460.261.0019792525
F3Intercept−0.500.27−1.020.051.0010001611
vowele0.190.21−0.230.601.0021442590
voweli0.440.210.030.841.0020072723
vowelo0.240.22−0.180.671.0020512695
vowelu0.320.22−0.100.751.0021132524
groupmonolingual0.500.39−0.261.241.009911443
vowele:groupmonolingual−0.280.31−0.890.321.0019742449
voweli:groupmonolingual−0.090.30−0.670.501.0019542526
vowelo:groupmonolingual0.370.31−0.230.971.0019602788
vowelu:groupmonolingual0.170.30−0.410.781.0020282874
DurationIntercept0.150.010.130.171.01556962
vowele−0.010.01−0.020.001.0015562062
voweli−0.030.01−0.04−0.021.0013772061
vowelo−0.010.01−0.030.001.0014061870
vowelu−0.020.01−0.03−0.011.0015562181
groupmonolingual0.040.010.010.071.016791158
vowele:groupmonolingual0.000.01−0.020.011.0014571792
voweli:groupmonolingual−0.010.01−0.020.011.0013182132
vowelo:groupmonolingual0.000.01−0.010.021.0014331953
vowelu:groupmonolingual−0.020.01−0.030.001.0016042443
* CIs refer to Credibility Intervals.
Table 3. Results of Bayesian hypothesis testing. The hypothesis tested whether heritage < monolingual in terms of acoustic values for each Greek vowel.
Table 3. Results of Bayesian hypothesis testing. The hypothesis tested whether heritage < monolingual in terms of acoustic values for each Greek vowel.
FeatureVowelβSEl—95% CIu—95% CIERPP
F1i0.220.19−0.100.546.890.87
e0.520.200.210.84189.480.99
a0.950.200.621.27Inf1.00
o0.300.20−0.020.6215.740.94
u0.150.20−0.180.483.630.78
F2i0.210.15−0.030.4612.610.93
e−0.060.15−0.310.180.480.33
a0.330.150.080.5775.920.99
o0.030.15−0.210.281.460.59
u0.230.15−0.010.4817.100.94
F3i0.410.40−0.251.045.640.85
e0.220.40−0.450.872.390.70
a0.500.39−0.151.138.780.90
o0.870.400.211.5255.340.98
u0.670.400.021.3321.860.96
Durationi0.030.010.010.05101.560.99
e0.040.010.020.06332.331.00
a0.040.010.020.06570.431.00
o0.040.010.020.06570.431.00
u0.020.010.000.0415.330.94
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Georgiou, G.P.; Giannakou, A. Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels Produced by Adult Heritage Speakers of Albanian. Acoustics 2024, 6, 257-271. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics6010014

AMA Style

Georgiou GP, Giannakou A. Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels Produced by Adult Heritage Speakers of Albanian. Acoustics. 2024; 6(1):257-271. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics6010014

Chicago/Turabian Style

Georgiou, Georgios P., and Aretousa Giannakou. 2024. "Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels Produced by Adult Heritage Speakers of Albanian" Acoustics 6, no. 1: 257-271. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics6010014

APA Style

Georgiou, G. P., & Giannakou, A. (2024). Acoustic Characteristics of Greek Vowels Produced by Adult Heritage Speakers of Albanian. Acoustics, 6(1), 257-271. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics6010014

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop