Next Article in Journal
Perceptions of Forms of Address in European Portuguese in Online Metadiscourse or What Happens When You Use você in Court
Next Article in Special Issue
Chilean Spanish Intonational Plateaus and Their Pragmatic Functions: A Case of Contact with Mapudungun
Previous Article in Journal
On the Absence of Certain Island Effects in Mende
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Comparative Analysis of Declarative Sentences in the Spontaneous Speech of Two Puerto Rican Communities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Language-Specific Prosody in Statements of Palenquero/Spanish Bilinguals

by
Wilmar Lopez-Barrios
Spanish and Portuguese Division, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
Languages 2024, 9(4), 132; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040132
Submission received: 31 October 2023 / Revised: 7 March 2024 / Accepted: 15 March 2024 / Published: 3 April 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Prosody in Shared Linguistic Spaces of the Spanish-Speaking World)

Abstract

:
This study explores the extent to which Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, a population that is said to have a residual high tone of African origin, keep their two languages temporally and intonationally distinct across statements. While creole languages that emerged from the contact of African and European languages, such as Palenquero, may develop hybrid prosodic systems with tones from substrate languages, and stress from the majority language, language-specific prosody might be expected to converge or simplify over the course of time. As prosodic convergence seems to be inescapable under Palenquero’s circumstances, which factors could support language-specific prosody in this population, if there are any? Two-hundred and thirty-four five-syllable statements were elicited through a discourse completion task, with the participation of ten Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, in two unilingual sessions. Both phrase-final lengthening and F0 contours were assessed using linear mixed-effects models testing their association with final stress, language, and generation. F0 contours were dimensionally reduced using functional principal component analysis. Despite the strong similarities between the two languages, results indicate that both groups keep their two languages intonationally distinct using plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero initial rises followed by steeper declinations in Spanish. However, elderly bilinguals implement penultimate lengthening language-specifically, being more pronounced in Palenquero. Adults, in contrast, do not show this distinction. In addition to this, elderly speakers show hyperarticulation in Spanish intonation, increasing the difference between their languages. This leads us to believe that adults exhibit a more simplified prosodic system between their languages, relative to elderly bilinguals. In spite of such differences, both generations seem to have the same underlying process (perhaps a substrate effect) driving plateau-shaped intonation in Palenquero, which enhances language differentiation.

1. Introduction

Even though bilingual speakers may acquire or develop phonological categories that are language-specific, these categories do not appear to be autonomous as languages somehow influence one another (Barlow 2014; Deuchar and Clark 1996; Flege and Eefting 1987; Paradis 2001). In a language contact situation, speakers may become bilingual (Appel and Muysken 2005; Montrul 2008), and despite that the majority language could have more effects on the minority language, both languages would—to some extent—influence each other in the mind of proficient bilinguals. Prosody, the music of language, is not immune to cross-linguistic interactions, opening a new avenue for bilingual research in language contact scenarios.
Prosody involves phenomena such as tone, stress, intonation, phonological phrasing, or temporal limits (Arvaniti 2022; Beckman and Venditti 2011; Fujisaki 1997; Ladd 2008; Shengli 2019). Given that bilingual speakers exhibit cross-linguistic interactions showing transfer, interference, simplification, and convergence of forms and meanings (Grosjean 2008; Gutiérrez 1994), bilingual prosody also entails any of these cross-linguistic interactions (see Bullock and Toribio 2004). In addition to this, we have become increasingly aware of the vulnerability of prosody to bilingualism and language contact situations. In fact, bilingual speakers may be less exposed to one language input due to the strong influence of a majority language, accelerating prosodic convergence or simplification (Bullock 2009; Stefanich and Cabrelli 2020). Last but not least, prosody is inherently variable, as it is deployed to carry information about constituent and rhythmic structure (i.e., temporal limits), semantic and pragmatic meaning, and emotional content (Dupoux et al. 2008, p. 3). For these reasons, showing how prosodic information is represented at the phonological level, and produced at the phonetic level, in bilingual speakers of contact-induced languages poses major challenges. Nevertheless, knowing to what extent these bilingual speakers maintain their languages prosodically distinct could be a good starting point in order to better understand bilingual prosody in language contact scenarios.
Bilingual speakers of creole languages that developed from the contact of African and European languages, as in the case of Papiamento (Kouwenberg and Muysken 1994), Saramaccan (Bakker et al. 1994), or Palenquero (Hualde 2006; Hualde and Schwegler 2008), have shown a tendency towards prosodic merge or converge as they may also develop hybrid prosodic systems with (residual) tones from substrate African languages, and stress from source (or lexifier) European languages (see Gooden et al. 2009). Thus, prosodic convergence appears to be inescapable when language contact with the majority language is consistent and intense, as Bullock and Gerfen (2004) have argued. We do not know however what factors may counterbalance prosodic convergence, and could therefore explain language-specific prosody in these bilinguals. In fact, it is unclear whether bilingual speakers of creole languages could really keep their two languages, i.e., the creole and European languages, prosodically distinct. What is more, if this were possible, we would not have a clear understanding of how many generations could preserve language-specific intonation, or those temporal limits (i.e., rhythmic structures) that are used by these bilinguals in a language-specific fashion. One of the main motivations for asking a question of this sort is to explore what factors and conditions contribute to maintaining language-specific prosody in bilingual speakers of creole languages.
This paper reports on the effects of final stress, language, and generation on both phrase-final lengthening and intonation, in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals. The aim of this study is to know, in the first place, whether these speakers maintain their two languages prosodically distinct when producing statements, and thus understand better which underlying processes might have contributed to language-specific prosody, and which ones could be indicative of prosodic simplification or convergence. In order to operationalize these concepts, two research questions are addressed in this paper:
  • Do bilingual Palenquero/Spanish speakers use phrase-final lengthening in statements language-specifically? If that was the case, how final stress, language, and generation condition language distinctions from phrase-final lengthening production?
  • Do these bilinguals keep their two languages intonationally distinct in statements? If language-specific intonation is attested, what is the contribution of final stress and generation to the bilingual production of statement intonation in these speakers?
Palenquero is a creole language that has developed in San Basilio de Palenque, Colombia (South America), from the contact of African people with Spanish conquerors during the times of slavery and abduction in African lands. The most salient prosody of Palenquero creole was described as a type of phrase-final “cadence” (Friedemann and Patiño-Roselli 1983; Montes 1962), correlated acoustically with a falling pitch and a lengthening effect, stretched from the penultimate syllable until the end of the statement (Hualde 2006; Hualde and Schwegler 2008). In addition to this, it has been shown that statement intonation, in Palenquero, tends to be flat or, at least, exhibits plateau-shaped contours in most cases, and also that penultimate lengthening is ostensibly more frequent in Palenquero than Spanish (Correa 2017). On the other hand, Spanish broad-focus statements are typically realized with a steeper rise whose peak is reached later in prenuclear intonation (i.e., non-final intonation). This rise tends to be followed by a declination whereby statements describe a falling intonation for the most part of the utterance (see Beckman et al. 2002; Hualde and Prieto 2015). Therefore, flat or plateau-shaped contours do not seem to be frequent throughout unmarked statements in more Spanish dialects.
The social conditions under which adult and elderly bilinguals have acquired both Palenquero and Spanish relate to some differences in their particular ecological contexts (for linguistic ecology perspectives, see Gooden 2022; Mufwene 1996, 2001; Steien and Yakpo 2020). Elderly speakers have experienced discrimination, marginalization, and exclusion more severely than adult speakers in the current ecological context. This may be evidenced by Patiño-Roselli (1983), who found that Palenquero speakers, in the early 1980s, avoided the use of the creole, and did not want their children to speak Palenquero due to the stigma associated with its use. However, most elderly bilinguals are illiterate, as they have not received formal instruction in Spanish, but nowadays are the most fluent Palenquero speakers. Even though adult bilinguals were led away from Palenquero in their childhood, they were also part of the first cohort of students in the revival program for the Palenquero creole that started in the 1990s (Morton 2005). This implies that, as well as having learned the creole at home with their parents and grandparents, adult bilinguals have also learned Palenquero in public school while being taught to read and write in Spanish.
In what follows, Section 1 will cover the prosodic characteristics of some creole languages, and how the context wherein these languages develop may determine a hybrid status for their prosody. This section also offers a brief overview of bilingual prosody, and describes prosodic patterns that have been shown so far for Caribbean Spanish varieties and Palenquero creole, especially with regard to phrase-final lengthening and intonation. Then, Section 2 will describe the background and language proficiency of the bilingual participants, and present the production task used in this study, as well as the corpus, and the statistical analyses performed. The functional principal component analysis used for F0 contours is explained and visualized in this section. On the other hand, Section 3 will illustrate results on penultimate lengthening and intonation from both adult and elderly bilinguals, while Section 4 and Section 5 will discuss and conclude how convergence, simplification, hyperarticulation, final stress, and language-specific intonation could disclose a particular perspective to understand statement prosody in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals.

1.1. Prosody in Creole Languages

Creoles are languages that arise from “intense language contact situations between groups of people with no language in common, but a common need for communication” (Holm 2000). For instance, creoles that have developed in plantations, also throughout the period of slavery in the Americas, have emerged from the contact of European and African languages. Broadly speaking, African speakers have initially borrowed lexical items from the given European language, and have inserted them into the morpho-syntactic structure of their original African language. The resultant language (i.e., the pidgin) was then the first language for subsequent generations, who have theoretically made use of their innate linguistic capacities to transform it into a full-fledged language, being at this particular point a creole language (cf. Bioprogram Theory, and the Founder Principle in Creole Genesis in Mufwene 1996; Muysken and Smith 1994, respectively). Creole speakers used the European language to communicate outside their local community, whereas the creole is solely used within the community, being unintelligible to monolingual speakers of the European language. For that reason, African descendants in the Americas were raised in bilingual contexts, where speakers use a creole language demoted to be for informal contexts, and a dominant European language promoted to be used in more formal settings (Mufwene 1996; Rickford 1980; Thomason and Kaufman 1988).
Empirical evidence seems to indicate that creole speakers with tonal substrates from West African languages reinterpreted stress from the European language with the surface form of an African H tone (e.g., Adamson and Smith 1994; Bakker et al. 1994; Hualde 2006; Hualde and Schwegler 2008; Kouwenberg and Muysken 1994). Thus, they exhibit a H tone on primary-stressed syllables in the creole even when tones do not contrast paradigmatically (i.e., when there are no minimal pairs differing solely on the basis of tone), just as speakers from Bantu languages did with words borrowed from French and Portuguese (cf. Kikongo language in Samba 1989). In Saramaccan, for instance, European-derived words exhibit a H tone in primary-stressed syllables (Bakker et al. 1994, p. 170); in Papiamento, H and L tones correspond respectively to stressed and unstressed syllables (Kouwenberg and Muysken 1994, p. 208), and in Sranan, a double accent phenomenon is correlated with double length, and a H tone is used in intensive constructions and onomatopoeia (Adamson and Smith 1994, p. 221). In addition to this, it has been shown that creoles may also associate H tones with non-stressed syllables, such as Fa d’Ambu creole, whose long vowels, being stressed or not, show a high rise tone (LH), and the following syllable is always pronounced with a H tone (Post 1994, p. 194).
Even though bilingual speakers of creole languages may develop hybrid prosodic systems (Gooden et al. 2009), telling apart those patterns that are influenced by tonal substrate languages from those that are intonational is not as straightforward as it might seem. This is because some patterns can be found in both tonal and intonational languages. The phrasal phonology of Saramaccan, for instance, exhibits high-tone plateauing, wherein two H tone syllables can flank syllables unspecified for tone, as in (1a) and (1b) (Good 2004, p. 598).
(1)a.dí   foló   bɛ́ —> dí fóló bɛ́
the flower red
‘The flower is red.’ (Taken from Good 2004)
b.dí   wajamáka=dɛ́ á  óbo —> dí wájámáká=dɛ́á óbò
the iguana=there  have egg
‘The iguana has eggs.’ (Taken from Good 2004)
However, Papago language—known as Tohono O’odham—a North American language spoken in Southern Arizona and Mexico, also exhibits high-tone plateauing, given that speakers associate a H accent to each stressed vowel, and to all vowels in between (Hale and Selkirk 1987, p. 152). This is illustrated in (2) with the words wákial ‘cowboy’, and wísilo ‘calf’. The final result would be very similar to what has been observed in Saramaccan. Despite that, Papago has been described as an intonational language, and there is no evidence for tonal remnants in this language (Hale and Selkirk 1987).
(2)na-t                              g      wákial    g    wísilo    cépos? —>
Inter-Aux.3sg.Perf     Art  cowboy  Art  calf   brand.Perf
na-t g wákíál g wísíló cépos?
  
‘Did the cowboy brand the calf?’ (Taken from Hale and Selkirk 1987)

1.2. Creole Speakers as Agents of a Minority Language, and a Majority Language

As speakers of creole languages are in close contact with a majority language, prosodic outcomes may be driven by phonological loans, transfer, interference, convergence, or simplification (see Matras 2009; Thomason and Kaufman 1988; van Coetsem 1988). This implies that either language, i.e., the African or the European language, can provide the creole with prosodic features (Yakpo 2021). This is the reason why van Coetsem (1988) used the term “agentivity” to explain how language contact determines phonological features in contact-induced language. From this perspective, there are phonological loans that entail the partial or total reproduction of phonological material from a source language into a recipient language, including its form (matter in Matras 2009) and structure (pattern in Matras 2009). This is triggered by the relative “agentivity” of speakers’ languages, given that they behave as agents of either the source language or the recipient language (van Coetsem 1988, pp. 7–12). In other words, agentivity is therefore determined by the relative activation of speakers’ languages. Two critical processes are then explained from this perspective: Impositions (previously referred to as interference, see Thomason and Kaufman 1988), and borrowings. These terms can be more readily understood when observed in succession, as with a shift in language dominance between two generations of speakers who have come into contact with a majority language. As shown below, a topical subject such as the immigration phenomenon provides the context to better understand “agentivity”, given that language contact and bilingualism are, most of the time, driven by immigration.
The following examples were adapted from van Coetsem (1988, pp. 88–89) who showed that, when the speaker’s recipient language is more active, recipient language agentivity is in effect. Therefore, speakers would borrow forms from the non-native source language, and adapt those to the native recipient language phonology. For example, a first generation of Dutch immigrants in an English-speaking country borrowed English words while speaking Dutch, because the social context required them to speak English. Dutch is then the recipient language for the borrowings coming from English, which is the source language. English borrowings are thus adapted to Dutch phonology. On the other hand, source language agentivity connotes more activation of the speaker’s source language, as they impose its grammar and phonotactics on the form being reproduced in the recipient language. To continue with the generational transition for the above example, and see the succession for the two types of agentivity, the Dutch immigrants then had children who acquired L1 proficiency in English, and have also acquired Dutch from their parents as a heritage language. They are, in practical terms, bilingual speakers. When Dutch/English children speak Dutch with their parents, they may include English functional words, for example, the English preposition ‘at’ instead of Dutch ‘naar’. In this case, these bilinguals are applying source language agentivity. Within this type of interference, it is expected that those English forms used while speaking Dutch follow English phonotactics. They may be considered, therefore, as impositions from the native source language features on Dutch, which is the recipient language. It is worth reminding ourselves that these effects could entail more issues and greater complexities when it comes to explaining prosodic loans, transfer, interference, and simplification.
Consequently, source or lexifier languages have a vast impact on creoles, given that this latter is commonly the minority language. In the main, bilingual speakers tend to “impose” the source language prosody upon the recipient language, which is a linguistic behavior that in most cases leads to intonational convergence or simplification. Thus, when prosodic patterns from a majority language are “imposed” on the minority or speakers’ non-dominant language, source language agentivity is applied. Despite that, the minority or non-dominant language may also permeate the majority or dominant language prosody, in which case recipient language agentivity would be applied. Thomason and Kaufman (1988) consider this as substratum interference, being a subtype of interference that usually results from imperfect group learning during the process of language shift (p. 38). In general, bilingual speakers show transfer, interference, convergence, and other processes such as simplification, overgeneralization, or hyperarticulation of prosodic patterns. This is why cross-linguistic interactions are a constant stream of variation, and may be hiding a more general trend towards intonational simplification or convergence (Bullock and Toribio 2004; Gutiérrez 1994).

1.3. Prosodic Convergence in Bilingualism and Language Contact

It has become evident that, at least for segmental and prosodic information, some structures of bilinguals’ languages are somehow inclined to merge. Interference, transfer, or simplification are processes that make bilinguals’ languages less distinct or structurally more alike, a characteristic that has also been used to describe convergence. In bilingual studies, convergence has been defined as a process whereby “bilinguals’ languages become uniform with respect to a property that was initially merely similar” (Bullock and Toribio 2004, p. 91). For instance, Spanish-English bilinguals have shown a degree of convergence for voice onset time (VOT) in code-switched productions. That is, when switching from Spanish to English, the English VOT values were more Spanish-like, and when switching from English to Spanish, the Spanish VOT values were more English-like (Olson 2012). In addition to convergence, code-switched tokens may show hyperarticulation correlating with longer duration and higher pitch, when communicative constraints are at a higher level (see Hyper- and Hypo-articulation Theory in Lindblom 1990, cited in (Olson 2012)). Another case of convergence was reported in bilingual French-English speakers, living in Frenchville, Pennsylvania. These bilinguals have replaced the mid-front round French vowels [ø] and [œ] with the English rhoticized schwa [ɚ]. According to Bullock and Gerfen (2004), cross-language acoustic and perceptual similarity among these vowels makes them phonetically unstable, being this the reason why phonological convergence took place. However, they do not rule out the possibility that language contact with English could, at some point, have motivated this change. In any case, as the particular direction of change is opaque, the authors ascribe the merger primarily to the phonetic instability in the vowels because of their similarity (Bullock and Gerfen 2004, p. 103).
Convergence is a technical term widely used also in language contact studies. It is defined as a process by which two languages in contact change to become structurally more alike, assuming they were different at the onset of contact (Aalberse and Muysken 2019; Silva-Corvalán 1990; Thomason 2001). Thomason (2001) gave a wide definition for convergence claiming that, in a contact situation, languages converge in ways that make them more similar, but cross-language interference can be mutual, and not necessarily unidirectional. This holds true especially when neither the source nor the recipient language has the resulting feature, or when the direction of areal features is often impossible to determine (pp. 89–90). Bullock and Toribio (2004), presenting convergence as an emergent feature in bilingual studies, do not differ much from this perspective, since their definition does not give special relevance to the direction of influence between the languages in contact (p. 91). Furthermore, Silva-Corvalán (1990)’s definition is also stark and critical given that, in addition to resulting from transfer, convergence may occur due to internally motivated changes in one language, being most likely accelerated by contact, rather than transferred from one language to the other (p. 164). This implies that convergence not only results from transfer, but also from language internal processes such as simplification or overgeneralization.
Simplification is defined as the expansion of a given form to a larger number of contexts, at the expense of a different form that was in competition with the given form. That is, simplification involves the contraction of a different form which is used less frequently (Silva-Corvalán 1990). This concept might overlap with overgeneralization as the latter also describes the extensive use of a given form. Nevertheless, this would affect contexts where no competing form exists (Silva-Corvalán 1990). Prosodic systems in some creole languages and Afro-Spanish varieties have been analyzed as simple or reduced, showing apparently a degree of structural simplification (see Correa 2017; Sessarego and Rao 2016). It seems to be more convenient when we do not know whether substrate or source language effects could provide a sense of directionality in order to explain the current (or resulting) features. However, Alvord (2006) and Colantoni (2011) warn that prosodic patterns in contact-induced languages could be the result of transfer from one language rather than a language-internal simplification. Convergence is therefore a critical concept to this analysis, and seems to be in line with simplification, since creole languages undergo regular typological change and areal convergence with European source languages. These processes are not specific to creoles, but are part of the adaptation to their linguistic ecology (Yakpo 2021).
As prosodic studies with bilingual speakers following a language contact framework are relatively recent (e.g., Bullock 2009; Colantoni and Gurlekian 2004; Elordieta 2003; Elordieta and Irurtzun 2016; O’Rourke 2005; Queen 2006; Rodríguez-Vázquez 2019; Simonet 2011, among others), both bilingual and language contact studies are starting to have meeting points, at least with reference to prosody. Simonet (2011), for example, mentioned that bilingual speakers may show two types of convergence: Symmetrical and asymmetrical convergence. The latter would explain prosodic changes in which the two languages become more similar because one language is adopting the features of the other language, accounting for one-way directionality, either from the majority language to the minority language, or vice versa. Symmetrical convergence, on the other hand, should explain prosodic changes where the two languages are more alike by developing shared features (Simonet 2011, p. 158). However, it remains unclear what the scope and impact of symmetrical convergence might be, and whether this always depends on the prosodic influence that languages at play exert against each other.
Prosodic studies with bilingual participants whose languages are in intense contact allow us to see a little closer how symmetrical convergence may occur in bilingual prosody. For instance, young Turkish/German bilinguals, being Turkish heritage speakers, use short rises typically found in monolingual Turkish speakers, as a way to prosodically highlight the main point of narratives in nuclear position (i.e., phrase-final position). They also use German steeper rises which are produced by monolingual speakers for pragmatically more salient information in nuclear position. These young girls kept both short and steeper rises as contrastive, despite that the functions were similar and that the German language was the majority language, and could have therefore exerted a stronger influence on Turkish short rises. Queen (2006) showed that the two rises were contrastive to one another, and also to nuclear contours with falling or flat intonation, but were equally used in both Turkish and German. That is to say, they were no longer used language-specifically. These patterns were thus “fused” rather than mixed, given that the two rises went from being contrastive in two separate intonational grammars to being contrastive in one single intonational grammar that has resulted from “fusion” (Queen 2006, p. 175).
Bullock (2009), on the other hand, has interviewed the last two English/French bilinguals, who were French heritage speakers living in Frenchville, Pennsylvania. At the time of the study, they were about seventy years old, and were also English-dominant. She found that these speakers use pitch accents and tonal contours that have not been reported in any other French variety. They produced a penultimate prominence that was very similar—albeit not identical—to their English intonation. However, penultimate prominence in Frenchville French is not attributed to English influence, as it also resembles the secondary or emphatic accent from regional varieties of French. It is worth noting that these bilinguals no longer had contact with French. The study suggests therefore that speakers were innovators, but the contact with English intonation has enhanced the development of penultimate prominence as well because it is also similar (i.e., congruent) with English. In other words, penultimate prominence in Frenchville French is an innovation that could have resulted from both a language-internal mechanism for prosodic change in French, and from the influence of English intonation. Unfortunately, the report did not confirm whether penultimate prominence was still language-specific, yet all of them, penultimate prominence, focus via prominence in situ, and the prosody of left dislocation seem to be convergent with English, i.e., the speakers’ dominant language (Bullock 2009). Despite that, it does not appear to be an indisputable case of asymmetrical convergence.
This type of convergence, albeit driven by Italian, the native and recipient language of one of the most representative groups of people who have migrated to Argentina, might explain why Spanish intonation in Buenos Aires is distinguished from other Spanish varieties. In this dialect, early alignment of F0 peaks occurs in prenuclear position (i.e., non-final), and a pronounced pitch drop is observed in statement-final position (Fontanella de Weinberg 1966, 1980; Kaisse 2001; Malmberg 1950; Sosa 1999). That is, the former describes intonational rises whose peaks are reached within the boundaries of the stressed syllable in prenuclear position (i.e., non-final position), which is represented as H* or L+H* within the AM model for Spanish, in contrast to late alignment (represented as L+<H*, see Section 1.4.2). The latter refers to a more pronounced fall, in phrase-final position. It has been shown that prenuclear intonation in Spanish varieties exhibits late alignment (see Face 2001; Hualde 2000; Hualde and Prieto 2015; Sosa 1991, among others), it is therefore widely accepted as a common feature in Spanish intonation. However, some Spanish varieties in a situation of language contact, on the contrary, show early alignment, as is the case for Peruvian Spanish in contact with Quechua (O’Rourke 2012), or Lekeito Spanish in the Basque Country in Northern Spain (Elordieta 2003; Elordieta and Irurtzun 2016). In Colantoni and Gurlekian (2004)’ study, Spanish speakers showed early alignment to prosodically mark narrow focus, which is why they did not ascribe early-aligned peaks in prenuclear intonation to language contact. Nevertheless, they did connect the more pronounced fall occurring in phrase-final position of statements to both direct and indirect language contact from Lunfardo slang and Italian language, as both show falling contours (HL) downstepping pitch accents in phrase-final position. In Colantoni and Gurlekian (2004)’s words, it is plausible to assume that there were both active and passive Italian/Spanish bilinguals, and it is also reasonable to consider that monolingual Spanish speakers probably imitated the speech of Italian immigrants (p. 115). Recipient language agentivity could in both prospects explain why intonation in Buenos Aires Spanish differs from other Spanish varieties, supporting the language contact hypothesis.
In addition to this, O’Rourke (2012) found an interesting trend for contrastive and broad-focus marking in Peruvian Spanish. Prosodic distinctions, in terms of pitch height and alignment, were less contrastive in the two conditions, as the experiment moved from native Spanish speakers to Quechua/Spanish bilinguals, to native Quechua speakers (p. 508). These findings indicate that alignment is not distinct, and lower peaks are used in both contrastive and broad-focus conditions mainly by native Quechua participants in Cuzco, while, at the other end, monolingual Spanish speakers, most of them living in Lima, showed focus marking patterns similar to those found in Peninsular Spanish. It is not clear, however, how intonation contributes to focus marking in Quechua, given that empirical evidence is sparse. O’Rourke (2005) studied the prosody of Quechua evidential suffixes, including the suffix used to mark focus morphologically. She argued that speakers did not use peak height to distinguish evidential meanings in Quechua. On the other hand, Cole (1982, cited in (O’Rourke 2012)) studied Quechua from Imbaura, Ecuador, and found that non-final peaks under contrastive focus were higher than peaks in broad focus (pp. 210–11). O’Rourke suggests that native speakers of Quechua, as well as some Quechua/Spanish bilinguals, do not keep the two focus conditions prosodically distinct as a consequence of the influence of Quechua prosody. Nevertheless, it remains to be explored how Quechua intonation has influenced focus marking in Peruvian Spanish for these two groups, since the process is still opaque, and ruling out symmetrical convergence could be inaccurate at this point.
In view of the above, understanding how the direction of prosodic changes occurs from the majority to the minority language, or vice versa is not that simple. There appears to be no clarity or agreement on the way we assess prosodic changes that are influenced, at the same time, by both the majority and the minority languages, as well as on those patterns or “innovations” that have not been clearly outlined in any of the component languages at play, but are equally used in the two languages. Nevertheless, prosody from bilingual speakers of creole languages, such as Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, is an understudied phenomenon that might help us understand how bilingual prosody evolves over time. Perhaps we do not have all the historical and statistical details to reconstruct the evolution of prosody, but we could start by asking whether bilingual speakers of creole languages can keep their two languages prosodically distinct, despite cross-linguistic interactions reflected through transfer, interference, and convergence. To this end, Section 1.4 will describe prosodic features found in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals thus far, and what has been discovered about the past of this population, including their connection with African prosodies, and their contact with Caribbean Spanish intonation.

1.4. The Intonation of Palenquero and Caribbean Spanish

Palenquero is an Afro-Hispanic creole language spoken in San Basilio de Palenque, Colombia, South America, which developed in the XVII century, from the contact of African slaves brought to the current territory of Colombia by Spanish colonists (Navarrete 2008; Schwegler 2011b). The vitality of Palenquero appears to have remained stable until the 1970s, when monolingual speakers of Palenquero still coexisted with both active and passive bilinguals, and monolingual speakers of Spanish (see Lewis 1970). Since the early 1990s, Palenquero has been taught in the public school following a language revitalization program (see Morton 2005). Today, there are no monolingual speakers of Palenquero, and the elderly members of the community, those who have acquired the creole as their native language, are the most fluent speakers of Palenquero. They used the creole along with the local variety of Caribbean Spanish spoken in the village. Adult Palenquero speakers belong to a different generation, and indeed not all of them speak Palenquero. Some have acquired the creole as a heritage language and, at some point in the past, have attended Palenquero classes in the local school (Smith 2014). As mentioned earlier, Palenquero is unintelligible to Spanish monolingual speakers, whereas Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals switch languages depending on specific social contexts. Caribbean Spanish is used outside and inside the community, in more formal settings, while the creole is demoted to be informal, and for everyday transactions only within the community (Morton 2005). It is worth noting that despite the fact that Palenquero speakers have been in contact with Spanish for around four centuries, Palenquero is not undergoing a process of decreolization (Dussias et al. 2016; Lipski 2016; Schwegler 2001), which suggests that the language revival program is helping children hold onto their creole language and African cultural heritage.
Spanish is the source language or lexical donor language for Palenquero; thus, most Palenquero words are cognates from Spanish. Despite that, the creole contains lexical and morphological elements from Bantu origin (Hualde and Schwegler 2008), possibly coming from two dialects of Kikongo—Kiyombe and Civili—both of which would have been spoken by Bakongo communities (Moñino 2017; Noguera et al. 2014). Kikongo melodies have been explored previously (e.g., Bentley 1887; Butaye 1909; de Clercq 1907; Laman 1922; Lumwamu 1973; Marichelle 1902; Samba 1989). Laman (1922) found, for instance, that Kikongo words display a rhythmic accent, falling always at the penultimate syllable, which is also weakened when the final syllable is accented (Samba 1989, p. 22). As a result, the intonation of words was said to be flat when the root and the following syllables had the same pitch; it fell when the following syllables were lower than the root, and it rose when the final pitch was higher than the root (Samba 1989, p. 23). It is also interesting to note that French and Portuguese lexical borrowings in varieties of Kikongo were assigned a H tone by analogy in the most prominent vowel (Samba 1989) which, following the same logic, has also been posited for Palenquero, as a H tone surfaces stressed syllables in the creole (Correa 2017; Hualde and Schwegler 2008; Lipski 2010). For that reason, Palenquero prosody is said to be in an intermediate position between tone and stress (Hualde 2006), meaning that not only Kikongo dialects have contributed to current Palenquero melodies, but also Caribbean Spanish has influenced the shape of Palenquero prosody.

1.4.1. Phrase-Final Lengthening

Phrase-final lengthening is one of the dimensions that might help understand the extent to which Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals keep their two languages prosodically distinct. Although the final “cadence” of Palenquero was impressionistically described like this by Montes (1962) and Friedemann and Patiño-Roselli (1983), thanks to the development of speech technologies, this can now be acoustically associated with a sustained H tone followed by a falling pitch occurring in tandem with a lengthening effect which stretches from the penultimate syllable until the end of the utterance (Hualde and Schwegler 2008). Clearly of relevance to this study is the claim that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals produce more penultimate lengthening in Palenquero than Spanish (Correa 2017, p. 264). While this phonological process has not been reported in Caribbean Spanish varieties, some Bantu languages exhibit a process of penultimate lengthening in phrase-final position (see Hyman 2013), which might be another pattern from African origin. Therefore, it is reasonable to consider that penultimate lengthening may potentially contribute to language-specific intonation in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals.
It is interesting however that, even though lengthening at the rightmost edge of statements could point towards a more comprehensive understanding of the intonational grammar for Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, the study of final lengthening and its contribution to language-specific prosody is still uncharted territory. This could stem from the fact that pitch contours are relatively well-known when they are implemented on iambs in statement-final position. In this specific context, Palenquero speakers have shown flat contours, which usually lead to the flattening of F0 declination, and the truncation of low boundary tones (L%) (Correa 2012; Hualde and Schwegler 2008). Lipski (2010) tested statements ending in combinations of two or more stressed syllables, and found that Palenquero speakers dissimilate consecutive H tones through upstep, having one H tone higher than the other, or downstep, having one H tone lower. He suggested that a possible locus for the Palenquero high tone (H) lies precisely in this position.
Prosodic convergence, as detailed in Section 1.3, may lead Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals to produce phrase-final lengthening equally in their two languages. However, if adult speakers used penultimate lengthening irrespective of language, and if elderly speakers made a language-specific use of it, at least we could surmise that a change in apparent time might actually be happening (see Labov 1972). Final lengthening, on the other hand, might not parallel penultimate lengthening, if this latter were language-specific. In general, both final and penultimate lengthening are analyzed separately, as has been shown in Shekgalagari (see Hyman and Monaka 2011). In that report, final lengthening was above penultimate lengthening and its subsequent implementation of L% tones. This implies that Shekgalagari speakers attach more importance to final lengthening than penultimate lengthening, despite that the latter is phonologically less marked. Furthermore, it has been attested that both penultimate and final lengthening might be blocked for use in utterances other than statements, as in Bantu languages like Tswana, Shekgalagari, Sesotho, or Kinande (Hyman 2013). However, it is unknown thus far whether the relation between penultimate and final lengthening goes beyond that of the implicational relation claiming that if final lengthening is met, then penultimate lengthening should be met as well, and not in the opposite order (see Hyman and Monaka 2011).
As a way to address these gaps in understanding the prosody of Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, it is conjectured in this present study that, as penultimate and final lengthening seem to be partly independent of each other, language mode may not condition them in the same way. In other words, if penultimate lengthening is language-specific, this would not imply that final lengthening is also language-specific, whereby language effects might differ. Additionally, age effects were not bypassed or set aside here. Recall that adult and elderly bilinguals have acquired Palenquero and Spanish at home, and within the Palenquero community. Notwithstanding this, adults took part in the Palenquero revival process that began in the early 1990s. Hence, they have also learned Palenquero in school (Morton 2005), which suggests that concern about the preservation of Palenquero creole grew among the members of the community during that time, due probably to the increasing use of the local Caribbean Spanish. It is surmised therefore that if a prosodic change were occurring in “apparent time” (see Labov 1972), because of the intense contact with Caribbean Spanish, adults should not mirror the elderly's lengthening. In consequence, the standardization process of Palenquero in school could have been permeated by the high influence of Caribbean Spanish.

1.4.2. Statement Intonation

It has been suggested that flat or plateau-shaped intonation contours in Palenquero are linked to the reinterpretation of Spanish stress with a residual high tone (H) from two dialects of Kikongo language (see Correa 2012, 2017). The main implication of this claim is that these intonational outcomes could be envisaged as a result of H tones flanking unstressed syllables (or units underspecified for the residual H tone), which is, in other words, the definition used by Good (2004) to explain high-tone plateauing in Saramaccan. As mentioned above, the realization of L% tones is oftentimes truncated in Palenquero given the lack of F0 declination because of flat contours and, interestingly enough, due to iambic stress in phrase-final position (Correa 2017; Lipski 2010). Final stress would then serve as the right flank for flat contours, hence the interest in iambs when they occur in phrase-final position.
Even though statement intonation with flat or plateau-shaped contours has not been widely mentioned across Caribbean Spanish varieties, there is no evidence as yet that they are rare in this geographic region. Statement intonation in Spanish varies across dialects, but, overall, it describes a rising F0 in utterance-initial position (i.e., in prenuclear intonation), and once the rise reaches its peak, it is followed by a declination that falls until the end of the statement (see Figure 1a). Moreover, F0 peaks do not tend to be aligned within the boundaries of the stressed syllable in prenuclear intonation (Hualde and Prieto 2015). In other words, Spanish speakers produce F0 peaks that are reached after the stressed syllable in prenuclear intonation, which is why they are said to be late aligned or delayed relative to the accented syllable. The Autosegmental Metrical Model for Spanish (Beckman et al. 2002; Hualde and Prieto 2015) describes late alignment with the bitonal pitch accent L+<H*1, as can be found in the examples from Figure 1.
The stressed syllable is the first ‘be’ in Figure 1a,b. Recall that both of these examples correspond to the answer for the question ‘What does s/he do?’, and in either case, the first F0 peak is late aligned relative to the accented syllable (i.e., the first stressed syllable, in this case). On the other hand, nuclear intonation (i.e., final intonation) could present two possibilities: The first option is a smooth falling interpolation which spreads from the first F0 peak to the low boundary tone (L%), whose nuclear intonation has been annotated as L* L%, as illustrated in Figure 1a. The second option is a nuclear intonation showing a rising contour reaching a peak associated with the accented syllable (L+H*), and just before the L% tone, as Figure 1b demonstrates (examples taken from Hualde and Prieto 2015, p. 364). While changes in the pragmatic meanings have not been attested between these two nuclear possibilities, they appear to be an example of allotonic variation across Spanish intonation so far.
An iambic stress pattern in phrase-final position may prosodically motivate the production of flat contours in statements, resulting in phonological processes such as L% tone truncation. However, the linear association of flat intonation contours with L% tone truncation has not been extensively studied in Caribbean Spanish. One exception is the study done by Armstrong (2012), who noted that final rises or final plateaus in Puerto Rican Spanish yes/no questions tend to recur with increasing frequency when stressed syllables are in final position, whereby L% is said to be truncated. Cuban speakers also produce L% tone truncation in questions when the nuclear contour H* L% is anchored to an iambic syllable (Martín and Dorta 2018). Furthermore, L% tone truncation has been shown to be driven by iambic stress in phrase-final position of Canarian Spanish (Cabrera-Abreu and Vizcaíno-Ortega 2010), a variety that is historically connected to Caribbean Spanish. Other romance varieties such as Italian, Friulian, Moldavian Romanian, and Portuguese have shown L% tone truncation (Frota and Prieto 2015, p. 416), because of which, we will not be able to say that this outcome is alien to Caribbean Spanish intonation.
As languages with hybrid prosodic systems (i.e., tone and stress), like the Saramaccan creole (Good 2004), also exhibit this phonological pattern, it is therefore difficult, at this point, to attribute the occurrence of flat and plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero to the tonal remnants of a substrate African language, or to the intense contact with Caribbean Spanish. However, to understand better how these contours are used by these speakers, it is worth asking whether flat and plateau-shaped contours are equally likely to occur in both the Palenquero and Spanish of these bilinguals. Although there is not yet any definite evidence to answer this question, the few studies done in this regard seem to present incompatible perspectives. For instance, Correa (2012, 2017) argued that there were no substantial differences between the intonation of Palenquero and Caribbean Spanish statements across Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals. That report highlights that speakers use the same inventory of pitch accents and nuclear configurations in statements from both Palenquero and Spanish (Correa 2017, p. 263). However, later in time2, Lipski (2016) noted that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals can disambiguate language in identification tasks by using the intonational grammar of Palenquero. In his study, when bilingual listeners heard Palenquero intonation in Spanish utterances, they ostensibly biased language identification in the direction of Palenquero (Lipski 2016, p. 53). If that were the case of Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals in the present study, then language-specific intonation should be expected in at least some specific contexts.
Finally, as previously conjectured for age effects on phrase-final lengthening results, the main difference in the acquisition of Palenquero between adult and elderly bilinguals is grounded in the fact that adult speakers, besides learning Palenquero at home and within the community, have learned the creole in school (Morton 2005; Schwegler 2011b). Therefore, adults have had a different experience with Palenquero given that, since the early 1980s, fluent creole speakers ceased transmitting Palenquero to children (Patiño-Roselli 1983). It is then appropriate to have control for age effects, and also test the conjecture that if an intonation change were occurring in apparent time (see Labov 1972), adult Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals should not mirror elderly bilinguals’ intonation. As a result, adults might show more intonational convergence between their two languages, probably due to the simplification of prosodic patterns.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Production Task

The materials used in production tasks were designed to elicit semi-spontaneous statements. In order to more realistically reflect intonation variance across this sentence type in both Palenquero and Spanish, twelve near-minimal pairs were created aiming to explore the intonational contrast in statements from adult and elderly bilinguals. Minimal pairs were conveniently syllabified to have five units so that they were comparable and, at the same time, had a familiar ring to the participants in the production experiment. One sample from Palenquero creole is illustrated with the sentences in bold in (3) and (5), and their counterpart pairs in Spanish are shown with the sentences in bold in (4) and (6).
(3)Pal Nuno sabé nú si andi Balá ané a ten kaló nú.
Chitiá Nuno ke andi Balá ané a ten kaló.
(Expected response: Nuno, andi Balá, ané a ten kaló.)
‘Nuno does not know if in Bala’s house they are hot.
Tell Nuno that, in Bala’s house, they are hot.’
(Expected response: ‘Nuno, in Bala’s house, they are hot.’)
(4)Spa A Nuno le gusta escuchar música de Kalé.
Dile a Nuno que donde Balá tú oíste su Kale.
(Expected response: Nuno, donde Balá, yo oí tu Kalé.)
‘Nuno likes listening to Kale’s music.
Tell Nuno that, in Balá’s house, you listened to his Kale.’
(Expected response: ‘Nuno, in Balá’s house, I listened to your Kalé.’)
(5)Pal Nuno sabé nú si andi Balá ané a ten kalo nú.
Chitiá Nuno ke andi Balá ané a ten kalo.
(Expected response: Nuno, andi Balá, ané a ten kalo.)
‘Nuno does not know if in Bala’s house they have soup.
Tell Nuno that, in Bala’s house, they have soup.’
(Expected response: ‘Nuno, in Bala’s house, they have soup.’)
(6)Spa Nuno no sabe dónde dejó su palo.
Dile a Nuno que donde Balá tú viste su palo.
(Expected response: Nuno, donde Balá, yo vi tu palo.)
‘Nuno does not know where he left his stick.
Tell Nuno that, in Balá’s house, you saw his stick.’
(Expected response: ‘Nuno, in Balá’s house, I saw your stick.’)
As might have been inferred from the target sentences in (3)–(6), minimal pairs were designed with the aim of controlling for final stress effects on phrase-final lengthening, and also on intonation contours. Hence, the items in (3) and (4) end with iambic stress, whereas the items in (5) and (6) end with trochaic stress. This approach would allow us to see more clearly final stress effects, as it comprises two-syllable words which are near-minimal pairs varying their prosody from stress position. To make the point, the final Pal word [ka.ˈlo] “heat”, in (3), contrasts within the same language (i.e., Palenquero) with Pal [ˈka.lo] “soup”, in (5). On the other hand, the final Spa word [ka.ˈle] “name of a well-known musician”, in (4), contrasts with Spa [ˈpa.lo] “stick”, in (6), within Spanish statements. Consequently, the two-syllable words in final position contrast within and between languages.
The sentences to be elicited were designed and prepared in collaboration with a community leader and a Palenquero teacher, following the grammar of Palenquero described in Morton (2005); Pérez-Tejedor (2004). Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals performed two discourse completion tasks (DCTs) adapted to the participants’ everyday life. A DCT is a questionnaire used to gather prosodic data based on semi-spontaneous speech acts while speakers complete a turn in a given dialogue Brown (2001); Kasper and Dahl (1991). In order to present short, simple, and intuitive situations, the DCT instructions have followed dialogue construction and the old versions of DCT formats (see Vanrell et al. 2018). DCT instructions were therefore initiated with a brief situational background, followed by one direction prompting the speaker to reply as in (3)–(6).
To move forward in line with the idea of providing these bilinguals with familiar contexts, the voice of a known interlocutor was recorded while giving examples and the specific instructions, similar to those shown in (3)–(6). It aims to facilitate listeners’ understanding so that they are able to complete their turn in the conversation more naturally and as spontaneously as possible. Utterances were collected in a sound-attenuated recording studio in San Basilio de Palenque using a ZOOM H4n voice recorder, at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz, with 16-bit amplitude resolution. Participants wore a head-mounted microphone. It should be noted that tasks were designed and presented on psycho.py Peirce (2007, 2009), all the speakers were then exposed to the same voice from the well-known interlocutor who is fluent in Palenquero.

2.2. Participants and Procedure

Ten participants (five of them female) were recruited for the production experiment with the help of Palenquero leaders and teachers who were familiar with their Palenquero proficiency. Speakers were divided into two different generations: Six adults (three of them female) ( μ a g e = 39.5 , S D = 1.97 ), and four elderly speakers (two of them female) ( μ a g e = 64.3 , S D = 5.44 ). Participants completed a linguistic background questionnaire in which they were asked about their place of birth, level of education, occupation, Palenquero acquisition age, and whether the creole was acquired at home, at school, or outside in the village streets. All participants were born in San Basilio de Palenque, and learned the creole at home and within the community. As expected, unlike adults, elderly speakers had a lower level of education, and did not have the need to study Palenquero at school. They are thus native speakers of Palenquero, who have acquired the creole both at home and while interacting with community members. All participants were asked about the origin of their parents and spouse, and also if they speak Palenquero with them. At the end, they provided a self-rated proficiency of Palenquero for both speaking and listening skills, which is shown in Figure 2.
Since bilingual speakers are more likely to inhibit cross-linguistic interactions in unilingual contexts (see Antoniou et al. 2011; Olson 2013; Simonet 2016), participants were recorded on two different days. Thus, the priming effects of the non-target language were controlled in this way. The first session was in Palenquero, whereas the second session was in Spanish, and took place around a week later. Participants were told that they were going to hear a short description of a situation, and they had to answer as the voice indicated. They were trained with one example at the beginning of each session. The task generally began once they had understood both the example and the expected response. In addition to this, they were able to move on at their own pace, by pressing the space bar to hear the next situation. Participants were asked to repeat the utterance only if the statement fell far short of the requirements for the expected response. In this way, the list effect on intonation driven by frequent repetitions was addressed.

2.3. Data Analysis

A total of 240 utterances were collected, but after removing outliers and influential points using Cook’s distance Cook (1977) and the car package in R Fox and Weisberg (2018), 231 statements were considered for phrase-final lengthening analysis, and 196 statements were deemed to be optimal for the intonation analyses. Phrase-final lengthening analysis, including both penultimate and final lengthening, required the automatic extraction of vowel length from penultimate and final syllables. Thus, penultimate and final lengthening effects were accounted for by the statistical analysis of vowel length. Measurements were normalized by the subject being log-transformed and scaled as z-scores in R, following Mücke et al. (2018). This is the main reason why results will be reported as units, which in practice are standard deviations (SDs). The regression equation that best described the linear association between vowel length with final stress, language, and generation, was found by conducting automatic forward and backward stepwise regressions, with the R function step Venables and Ripley (2002). In order to make regression results more readable and interpretable, predictors’ levels were dummy-coded to be zero or one. That is, final trochee, Palenquero, and adult were coded as zero (0), while final iamb, Spanish, and elderly were coded as one (1). Random effects of speakers’ variation on the intercept were accounted for by the linear mixed-effects model, for completeness.
The analysis of intonation contours was conducted using functional principal component analysis (FPCA) Ramsay and Silverman (2005); Ramsay et al. (2009) along with linear regression analyses. The former required an exhaustive data preparation and normalization of intonation contours. The entire process may be outlined in three major steps: Firstly, F0 contours were time-normalized by extracting ten F0 points per syllable, having fifty points per utterance. Then, F0 samples were automatically extracted at equidistant times on each syllable using ProsodyPro Xu (2013). Second, pitch ranges were normalized by subject, by transforming F0 points as log values and turning them into z-scores. This means that the mean for each subject’s range was zero. It is the reason why intonational differences represent, in practice, standard deviations (SDs). At this point, outliers and influential intonation points were manually transformed into NAs (not available). Third, FPCA was performed in order to reduce F0 dimensionality, and simultaneously obtain the most substantial modes of F0 variations (i.e., the functional principal components (FPCs)). The R-package fda Ramsay et al. (2009) was used to serve this purpose. The most substantial modes of F0 variation, resulting from FPCA, were analyzed incrementally from the first FPC through the one which, after being cumulatively added, covered approximately 80% of the total variance. It means that FPC2 was cumulatively added to FPC1, and then FPC3 to FPC2 and FPC1, and so on, until the cumulative addition of variances covered approximately 80% of the total variance. The linear equation to explain the mode of F0 variance contained in each FPC was found using forward and backward stepwise regressions, and model comparisons, in order to find predictors and interactions that best explain the relationship between each FPC with final stress, language, and generation. Backward and forward stepwise regressions were achieved with the R function step Venables and Ripley (2002). Random effects of speakers’ variation on the intercept of each FPC were accounted for by the linear mixed-effects models for completeness.

2.3.1. Functional Principal Component Analysis (FPCA)3

Intonation as a physical property is the modulation of vocal fold vibration; hence, intonational data are high dimensional, and involve much variation. However, FPCA helps understand how this variation occurs by revealing the most substantial modes of variation, and showing towards which intonational pattern F0 contours are trending. Variance is then partitioned into functional principal components (FPCs), and each FPC corresponds to one dimension that captures one portion of the whole variance. In a second step, linear regression analyses are performed in order to explain the variance accounted for by each FPC. That is, main, interaction, and random effects are tested to predict the variance contained in each FPC (see Asano et al. 2016; Aston et al. 2010). FPCA is a data-driven semi-automatic analysis used to understand the dominant or the most substantial modes of variation in functional data while projecting the variances as smoothed curves generated from the same data Asano et al. (2016); Ramsay et al. (2009). To the best of my knowledge, projecting FPCs onto eigenvectors as smoothed intonation contours is the only way the variance contained in one dimension may be visually understood. Each projected FPC could reveal different pitch contours, and, furthermore, when they are incrementally added, the total variance can be reconstructed.
In order to illustrate this process, a sample dataset with 301 three-syllable contours, coming from two different predictors (Factor A and Factor B), is shown in Figure 3. It should be noted that the F0 contours presented in this section are by no means the actual contours studied in this paper. The FPCA performed in this illustrative dataset is visualized in Figure 4. Intonational data from Factors A and B are shown in Figure 3A, while averaged contours are presented in Figure 3B. FPCA helps find the most substantial modes of F0 variation, as illustrated in Figure 3A, so that we can visualize the average contours towards which actual contours are trending, and infer pitch accents whenever possible. Figure 4 shows the first three most substantial variances in these contours (see Figure 4A–F), as well as the entire reconstruction of intonation contours after having cumulatively added seven FPCs (see Figure 4G,H). Each FPC is one dimension of intonation contours, and is also the dependent variable for predictors, as presented in the following section.

3. Results

This section shows the statistical analyses conducted, and the results obtained in order to answer, initially, whether Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals use phrase-final lengthening language-specifically across statements and, afterwards, whether the speakers maintain their two languages intonationally distinct in this sentence type. Given that penultimate lengthening is one of the most noticeable characteristics of Palenquero intonation (Friedemann and Patiño-Roselli 1983; Montes 1962), phrase-final lengthening analysis investigates how penultimate and final lengthening are implemented in both Palenquero and Spanish. Hence the interplay of final stress, language, and generation with each other is tested, aiming to understand its association with phrase-final lengthening. Palenquero intonation seems to differ from Spanish intonation among Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, as flat or plateau-shaped contours have reportedly been more connected to the creole than to Spanish (Correa 2017). For that reason, the contribution of final stress and generation to language-specific intonation was also tested.

3.1. Phrase-Final Lengthening

Phrase-final lengthening results are presented below. I anticipate that they supported the claim that, overall, penultimate lengthening is much longer across elderly speakers than adults, being used language-specifically by the former. On the other hand, despite that final lengthening was significantly longer than penultimate lengthening, both adults and the elderly exhibit final lengthening to the same extent in their two languages. Therefore, there were no age effects on the way speakers implement final lengthening, and is not used language-specifically. In a nutshell, while adults produced penultimate lengthening in both Palenquero and Spanish, elderly speakers showed much longer vowels in Palenquero trochees, demonstrating that they implement penultimate lengthening in a language-specific fashion. Final lengthening, on the contrary, was equally likely in both Palenquero and Spanish. Let us examine these findings in more detail.
The predicted vowel length for penultimate syllables (i.e., from final trochees) in adult bilinguals was −0.23 (95% CI [−0.44, −0.02]), as can be found in Table 1. Adults increased vowel length at a rate of 1.07 units in stressed syllables of final iambs from Palenquero, which indicates that final lengthening is longer than penultimate lengthening in this generational group (see Adults in Figure 5). Nevertheless, adult speakers did not show language-specific differences for vowel length in the stressed syllable of final trochees (B = −0.05, 95% CI [−0.33, 0.22], p = 0.70; see Figure 6). Interestingly, elderly bilinguals increased vowel length at a rate of 0.57 units in Palenquero trochees, as compared to adult bilinguals (95% CI [0.23, 0.92], p = 0.01; see Figure 5).
This implies that, while final lengthening occurs in Palenquero regardless of age, the two generations differed in the phonetic implementation of penultimate lengthening. Elderly bilinguals showed therefore a more pronounced lengthening in Palenquero than adults (B = −0.80, 95% CI [−1.32, −0.28], p = 0.002). The Palenquero lengthening found across elderly speakers has a particularly relevant consequence for language-specific prosody in this group. Results show that elderly bilinguals produced final trochees in Spanish with a shorter lengthening relative to Palenquero, whereas adult speakers have produced penultimate lengthening equally in both Palenquero and Spanish, as shown in Figure 6. In other words, despite that adults use penultimate lengthening, this is not only less pronounced with respect to the elderly (B = 0.57, 95% CI [0.23, 0.92], p = 0.01), but also equally likely in their two languages (B = −0.05, 95% CI [−0.33, 0.22], p = 0.70).

3.2. Intonation Contours

The aim of the present analysis is to resolve whether Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals exhibit language-specific intonation patterns across statements, and to determine the extent to which other prosodic factors, such as stress in phrase-final position, and sociolinguistic factors, like generation, intermediate. Figure 7A shows the intonation contours from both Palenquero and Spanish, after removing outliers and influential points, and after being normalized by the speaker (see Section 2.3). This Figure also offers a more general perspective on the actual intonation variation across statements. Figure 7B, in turn, presents the averaged contours depicting an initial high rise in Spanish statements, which is followed by a steeper declination. In Palenquero, statement intonation seems to describe a high plateau sustained throughout syllables two, three, and four (i.e., S2, S3, and S4).
As described earlier in Section 2.3.1, the most substantial modes of F0 variation arising from the F0 variance of statements, shown in Figure 7A, were obtained through FPCA. Then, each FPC was explained using a linear mixed-effects model, including final stress, language, and generation as predictors, and speaker as a random variable for the intercept. Results indicate that, despite the great intonational similarities between Palenquero and Spanish statements, speakers produced statements using language-specific intonation patterns. As shown below, the intonation declination seemed to be partly suspended in Palenquero, as suggested from Figure 7B through the averaged plateau-shaped contour observed in Palenquero. Spanish statements, on the other hand, exhibited initial rises that were as steep as the declinations that followed them. Let us then now see the extent to which these language-specific differences were driven by final stress and generation effects.

3.2.1. First Most F0 Variance, FPC1

The first most substantial F0 variation (i.e., FPC1) across statements is illustrated in Figure 8, and represents the effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (− − − line) FPC1 from the grand mean. The variance in FPC1 targets two specific trends, the first being related to flat contours preceding the boundary tone at the rightmost edge, which has resulted from subtracting FPC1 from the mean, in Figure 8, and the second one showing an initial steeper rise, followed by a pronounced declination, similar to the contour that has resulted from adding FPC1 to the mean, in Figure 8.
F0 variance captured by FPC1 was best explained by the linear model presented in Table 2. The model has yielded significant effects of language, and the two-factor interactions from final stress and language, as well as final stress and generation (F(5, 190) = 8.63, p = 2.17 × 10 7 ). As presented in Figure 9A4, the first substantial variance covers 42.2% of the total variance, and stretches across the first four syllables (i.e., S1–S4). On the whole, adult bilinguals showed two different trends upon which language-specific intonation might be based. The predicted average for FPC1 increased 1.64 units for Spanish statements ending in trochees, relative to Palenquero (B = 1.64, 95% CI [0.75, 2.54], p = 4 × 10 4 ). This implies that the intonation from Spanish statements trends towards wider pitch excursions in utterance-initial position, which results in steeper declinations, as illustrated in Figure 9B. Palenquero statements, on the other hand, exhibit shorter pitch excursions, whereby F0 peaks occur roughly at a similar height, showing a less pronounced declination across prenuclear intonation (see Figure 9B). In line with Correa (2017)’s results, the realization of plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero could be grounded in the realization of high F0 peaks that are occurring at a similar height, as effects of language in FPC1 suggest.
The linear regression model presented in Table 2 also indicates that the intonation of elderly bilinguals was not sharply distinguished from adults when producing Palenquero statements ending in trochees (B = 0.21, 95% CI [−0.81, 1.22], p = 0.70). Therefore, within FPC1, there is no evidence to reject the idea that elderly speakers use language-specific intonation in statements similarly to adults. Consequently, the elderly have also yielded to the phonetic implementation of plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero (see statements ending in trochees, in Figure 10).
Despite that, generational differences were attested from the two-factor interaction of final stress and generation (B = −1.91, 95% CI [−3.79, −0.04], p = 0.046). Thus, while both generations showed similar variability across Palenquero statements ending in trochees, they differed when these statements ended in final iambs. Palenquero statements from adults trended towards slightly wider pitch excursions in initial position, compared to the elderly (see Figure 10B). As was explained previously, wider and higher initial pitch excursions appeared to be a phonetic pattern that seems to be more closely related to the Spanish intonation of these bilinguals.
Nevertheless, language distinctions were also conditioned by final stress at the first most F0 variance (i.e., FPC1), given that adults showed a wider intonational distance between their two languages when statements ended with final iambs (B = 2.36, 95% CI [0.51, 4.20], p = 0.013). Figure 11A,B show, respectively, the variance scheme in FPC1 by final stress and language, and the predicted average contours for this interaction within FPC1. Initial pitch excursions tended to be high and steeper in Spanish statements ending in trochees, but were much higher in statements with final iambs, as shown in Figure 11B. As the model did not yield a three-factor interaction with final stress, language, and generation, differences based on the generational group do not seem to go beyond the ones presented in Figure 10, at least for the first most substantial variance. Therefore, it all leads to believe that elderly bilinguals would also exhibit a wider distance between the intonation of their two languages when statements end in final iambs, in the same way as adults did (see Figure 11).

3.2.2. Second Most F0 Variance, FPC2

The second most substantial F0 variation (i.e., FPC2) is illustrated in Figure 12, which represents the effect of adding and subtracting FPC2 from the mean. The variance in FPC2 covers 21% of the total variance, and targets two different trends whose main locus is on the fourth syllable. More specifically, F0 was found to be high on this syllable, when FPC2 was added to the grand mean, and low, when FPC2 was subtracted from the grand mean. F0 variance captured by FPC2 was explained by the linear mixed-effects model presented in Table 3. The model indicates that the intonation variance contained in FPC2 was conditioned by final stress, language, and the two-factor interaction of language and generation (F(4, 191) = 74.03, p = 8.70 × 10 38 ). In order to better understand F0 variance in FPC2, it was cumulatively added to the variance in FPC1. Therefore, effects are described incrementally, as illustrated in Figure 13.
The predicted average for FPC2 decreased −1.16 units for Spanish statements ending in trochees, compared to Palenquero (B = −1.16, 95% CI [−1.62, −0.70], p = 1.55 × 10 6 ). As adult bilinguals are the reference group, this means that, within the second most F0 variance, adults decreased FPC2 in Spanish statements trending towards the F0 contour that resulted from subtracting FPC2 from the mean, in Figure 12. F0 peaks over final trochees in Spanish statements were lower than Palenquero peaks, as illustrated in Figure 13B. The variance scheme in FPC2 by language condition is shown in Figure 13A, and it seems to be partly connected to FPC1, given that wider pitch excursions in statement-initial position also manifested when FPC2 was subtracted from the mean (see Figure 12). Since FPC2 decreased in Spanish statements, the fact that wider pitch excursions were followed by a steeper declination appears to have significantly affected F0 height in the nuclear position of Spanish statements, as presented in Figure 13B. Palenquero statements, on the contrary, showed shorter pitch excursions in initial position, and high peaks occurring at a similar height. This latter might explain why Palenquero’s high F0 peaks over final trochees were higher than Spanish (see Figure 13).
Did elderly bilinguals show the same variance exhibited by adults in FPC2? Indeed, both generations showed the same language-specific patterns. However, the intonational distance between the two languages was wider for the elderly (B = −0.76, 95% CI [−1.49, −0.02], p = 0.043). Figure 14A,B show, respectively, the variance scheme in FPC2 by language and generation, and the predicted average contours for this interaction within FPC2. Spanish statements from elderly bilinguals exhibited much wider pitch excursions in utterance-initial position, and lower F0 peaks over final trochees, maintaining their two languages slightly more intonationally distinct than adults (see Figure 14B).
Another important effect for the second most F0 variation (i.e., FPC2) came from final stress, as presented in Table 3. The predicted average for FPC2 decreased −3.04 units for Palenquero statements ending in iambs, relative to final trochees (B = −3.04, 95% CI [−3.46, −2.62], p = 2.99 × 10 31 ). Statements ending in iambs trended towards the F0 contour that resulted from subtracting FPC2 from the mean, in Figure 12. Final stress effects are illustrated in Figure 15A,B. The former presents the range of variation by final stress, while the latter shows the averaged contours by the two levels of final stress. Final iambs largely drove steeper declinations supporting L% tones in Palenquero statements, in spite of the fact that this stress pattern was expected to induce L% tone truncation in phrase-final position (see Figure 15B). On the other hand, L% tones on final trochees in Palenquero were not affected by high plateaus, that is to say, L% tones did not prove to be higher when the F0 peak on the stressed syllable kept the same height as the preceding peaks. This indicates then that, in most cases, speakers tend to implement L% tones in nuclear contours, as shown in Figure 15. In sum, final stress directly conditions F0 variance over syllable four (S4), which was the second most important variance.

3.2.3. Third Most F0 Variance, FPC3

The third most substantial F0 variation (i.e., FPC3) is illustrated in Figure 16, which shows the effect of adding and subtracting FPC3 from the mean. The variance in FPC3 covers 14.7% of the total variance, and mainly targets final intonation in statements, describing how low the pitch floor for the phonetic implementation of L% tones across statements could be.
In addition to this, FPC3 also targets—albeit to a lesser degree—wider pitch excursions, followed by steeper declinations, driving much lower L% tones, as shown in Figure 16, with the contour that results after subtracting FPC3 from the mean. On the other hand, lower pitch excursions might end up with a L% that is less low, as suggested from the addition of FPC3 to the mean in Figure 16. The F0 variance captured by FPC3 was subsequently explained by the linear mixed-effects model presented in Table 4. The model shows that the intonation variance contained in FPC3 was mediated by final stress, language, and the two-factor interactions of final stress with generation, and language with generation (F(6, 189) = 18.50, p = 7.14 × 10 17 ). As effects are presented incrementally from the cumulative addition of FPCs, this time, FPC3 was added to the effects resulting from the addition of FPC2 to FPC1, as illustrated in Figure 17.
The predicted average for FPC3 decreased −0.66 units for Spanish statements ending in trochees, relative to Palenquero (B = −0.66, 95% CI [−1.21, −0.12], p = 0.018). Adults have trended again towards the contour described after subtracting FPC3 to the mean, as shown in Figure 16. Thus, Figure 17A,B show, respectively, the variance scheme in FPC3 by language, and the predicted average contours split by language within FPC3. It can be noted that the intonation distance between adults’ languages was widened through slightly much higher initial rises in Spanish statements, as illustrated in Figure 17B which can be compared with Figure 13B. It was also found that the pitch floor for L% tones was not affected by language. Therefore, the implementation of L% tones for statements with final trochees did not yield language-specific variation across adults. However, elderly bilinguals did exhibit this distinction between Palenquero and Spanish, as detailed below.
Unlike adults, the intonational distance between the languages of elderly speakers extended into the realization of L% tones. The phonetic implementation of L% tones, across elderly bilinguals, was higher in Palenquero as compared to Spanish (B = −1.14, 95% CI [−1.94, −0.34], p = 0.006). Therefore, elderly speakers showed a higher floor for Palenquero L% tones, when statements end in trochees, as suggested from the variance scheme of FPC3 averaged by the interaction of language and generation, in Figure 18B. The F0 range for this interaction is shown in Figure 18A. As noted with the variance contained in FPC2, effects of the interaction of language and generation in FPC3 have also included wider pitch excursions in utterance-initial position, in Spanish, being much shorter in Palenquero. These trends were connected to steeper declinations in Spanish, and F0 peaks occurring at the same height in Palenquero, all of which impacts the realization of L% tones. Consequently, Figure 18B demonstrates that the pitch floor at the rightmost edge is also language-specific across elderly bilinguals.
In addition to language and generational differences, large effects on FPC3 came from final stress, as shown in Table 4. The predicted average for FPC3 increased 2.43 units for Palenquero statements ending in iambs (B = 2.43, 95% CI [1.68, 3.18], p = 1.53 × 10 9 ). These statements trended towards the F0 contour that resulted from adding FPC3 to the mean, in Figure 16. As can be seen here, the addition reveals higher L% tones that are expected when the final syllable (S5) is stressed. Final stress effects are illustrated in Figure 19A,B. The former presents the range of variation by final stress, while the latter shows the averaged contours split by the two levels of final stress. After the cumulative addition of FPC3, final iambs proved to be driving higher L% tones in Palenquero statements, compared to final trochees (see Figure 19B). This also implies that nuclear contours anchored to iambic stress tend to be flatter, as the intonation falls seem to be slightly more truncated in this context.
In this respect, results for FPC3 suggest that final stress effects held true for both Palenquero and Spanish, as no difference between adults’ languages was found (B = −0.76, 95% CI [−1.68, 0.17], p = 0.11). On the other hand, it was attested that generational effects interacted with final stress effects (B = −1.28, 95% CI [−2.22, −0.34], p = 0.008). Figure 20A,B show, respectively, the variance scheme in FPC3 by final stress and generation, and the predicted average contours for the same interaction within FPC3. Generational differences based on the pitch floor of L% tones took place in final trochees, but not in final iambs, as illustrated in Figure 20. That is, elderly bilinguals showed higher L% tones compared to adults, but only on final trochees, not with final iambs.
These first three functional principal components have encapsulated the main intonational variances across statements, and taken together can explain to a large extent (approx. 80% of the total variance) how final stress, language and generation mediate intonation in this sentence type. Summarizing these findings, we may posit that, despite the similarities between Palenquero and Spanish, both generations still use language-specific intonation across statements. Nevertheless, elderly bilinguals show a wider distance between the intonation of their two languages. In either case, the distinction was grounded in wider pitch excursions found in utterance-initial position for Spanish, whereas initial pitch excursions from Palenquero were much shorter. These outcomes were respectively followed by steeper declinations in Spanish, and F0 peaks occurring at the same height in Palenquero, yielding to the phonetic implementation of flat or plateau-shaped contours. Speakers implemented L% tones, except that when these were anchored to iambic stress, they tended to be less low, and flatter.
Although results did not provide evidence to believe that language-specific intonation on the statements of adult bilinguals is sharply distinguished from elderly speakers, the latter exhibited a wider difference between their two languages. Thus, they did not differ from adults in the shape of intonational patterns—in fact, that was not the case—, but rather because adults show language-specific patterns which appear as being simplified. This suggests that adults might be showing more intonational convergence than the elderly. Finally, but not less important, stress pattern in phrase-final position was proven to be a driving factor for the intonation of these speakers. Recall that language-specific intonation within the first most F0 variance resulted from the interaction of language and final stress, implying that language distinctions based on flat contours and steeper declinations were even greater for statements ending in iambs (see Figure 11).

4. Discussion

While prior work has considered that statement intonation in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals does not seem to be produced in a language-specific fashion, since speakers appeared to use the same pitch accents and nuclear configurations in both languages (Correa 2012, 2017), Lipski (2016) noticed that these bilinguals biased language identification in the direction of Palenquero, when Palenquero intonation was superimposed onto Spanish statements. The results of these analyses indicate that the second option emerges as the most plausible, given that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals have maintained their languages prosodically distinct in the present study. It has been shown that bilingual speakers of creole languages in the Caribbean may develop hybrid prosodic systems with (residual) tones from substrate African languages, and stress from lexifier European languages (Gooden et al. 2009). Along these lines, it has been suggested that Palenquero speakers reinterpreted Spanish stress with a H tone, being in an intermediate position between tone and stress (Hualde 2006; Hualde and Schwegler 2008). In the main, bilinguals’ languages are constantly interacting, showing interference, transfer, and convergence of prosodic patterns (Bullock and Toribio 2004; Grosjean 2008), because of which, considering that the intonations of Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals could have merged with the passing of time was therefore a reasonable prospect. For that reason, the fundamental question that this paper tried to answer was whether these bilinguals can keep their two languages temporally and intonationally distinct. Correa (2012, 2017) had already suggested that penultimate lengthening was ostensibly more frequent in Palenquero than in Spanish, and our results support this claim, given that elderly bilinguals produced penultimate lengthening language-specifically. In addition to this, even though adult speakers produced language-specific intonation, these distinctions manifested at a more considerable level across the elderly. However, it seems reasonable to think that some of these differences became greater probably due to a tendency to hyperarticulate Spanish patterns, on the part of elderly bilinguals, and due to the simplification of language-specific prosody, on the part of adult bilinguals. The implications of these findings are discussed below.

4.1. Language-Specific Lengthening in Elderly Speakers

Results provide statistical evidence suggesting that phrase-final lengthening production contributes to language-specific intonation in statements from Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, given that elderly speakers had greater chances to show penultimate lengthening when they spoke Palenquero. This finding extends those of Correa (2012, 2017), confirming that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals use penultimate lengthening language-specifically. Nevertheless, generational effects indicated that adult speakers did not mirror elderly speakers’ behavior as vowel lengthening in penultimate syllables was equally likely in adults’ languages. The main implication of this result is that adults did not keep their two languages prosodically distinct through the production of penultimate lengthening. We may also consider the idea that speech rate would slow as we grow older, but elderly speakers showed no temporal difference from adults in Spanish statements ending with trochees. This is the first reason to believe that adult bilinguals could exhibit prosodic convergence through penultimate lengthening, being both shorter, and equally used in Palenquero and Spanish.
We might think that age effects could lie with the fact that adult bilinguals had less exposure to Palenquero, as they were intentionally discouraged from using the creole, and this did not happen to the elderly. However, the adults who participated in the present study were born around 1979, a decade before the Palenquero revival process starting in the early 1990s (see Morton 2005). Thus, they have participated and are playing a pivotal role in the revitalization program of Palenquero (Morton 2005; Smith 2014).
It could be surmised then that prior to the language revitalization program, children might have started to simplify bilingual prosody given the increasing use of the local variety of Caribbean Spanish spoken in San Basilio de Palenque. This trend did not seem to be fully counterbalanced with Palenquero instruction at school, which would partly explain why adults are no longer using penultimate lengthening language-specifically. However, elderly speakers demonstrate that they use penultimate lengthening in a language-specific fashion. This implies, from a more general standpoint, that penultimate lengthening could indeed be a prosodic feature specific to Palenquero. Penultimate lengthening is not rare in Spanish varieties either, but this lengthening effect has been reported before for Palenquero prosody as a final “cadence” that is stretched from the penultimate syllable until the end of the statement (Friedemann and Patiño-Roselli 1983; Montes 1962). This pattern has also been reported in some Bantu languages, and has been called penultimate lengthening (Hyman 2013). Thus, the connection between Palenquero prosody and with Kikongo language does not seem to be arbitrary, since the distribution of penultimate lengthening in Bantu languages spread over the Eastern and Southern parts of the Bantu zone, as well as the Western part, where dialects of Kikongo were spoken (Schwegler 2017). This makes it probable the connection between Palenquero penultimate lengthening with Kikongo, considering it as an African substrate effect.
Results also indicate that stress plays a pivotal role in the language-specific use of phrase-final lengthening across elderly speakers. It is interesting that trochaic foot, being the unmarked stress pattern found in 79.5% of Spanish words (Quilis 1983), enclosed language distinctions; whereas iambic foot, being found in 17.7% of Spanish words (Quilis 1983)—and therefore being more marked—did not show final lengthening language-specifically. The implicational relation between final and penultimate lengthening across Bantu languages suggests that final lengthening might occur in a language, if penultimate lengthening is used first, meaning that this latter is unmarked, and occurs irrespective of final lengthening (see Hyman and Monaka 2011). Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals did not violate this implicational relation, as the occurrence of final lengthening implied the occurrence of penultimate lengthening, showing that the prosody of these bilinguals does not exhibit final lengthening alone, without penultimate lengthening, similarly to Bantu languages. Furthermore, evidence was insufficient to reject that penultimate and final lengthening are not independent. However, language effects found across elderly bilinguals do not condition penultimate and final lengthening in the same way, given that final lengthening was equally likely in both Palenquero and Spanish, suggesting that language effects on penultimate lengthening have not spread to final lengthening.

4.2. Flat or Plateau-Shaped Contours versus Initial Rises and Steeper Declinations

Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals kept their two languages intonationally distinct across statements, despite the prosodic similarities observed between the two languages. Plateau-shaped contours serve to describe statement intonation in Palenquero. Thus, F0 peaks were produced roughly at the same height and initial peaks were not steep, and did not have wider pitch excursions either. On the contrary, statement intonation in Spanish exhibits initial rises, which were as steep as the declinations that followed them. These findings are consistent with actual contours observed in both adult and elderly speakers, as illustrated in Figure 21 and Figure 22, both of which were produced by the same elderly participant. Results could consequently support the idea put forth by Lipski (2016), who suggested that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals can disambiguate language in identification tasks using Palenquero intonational cues. Although the present research is far from a perception study, results demonstrate that, at least with respect to statements, both adult and elderly speakers can use different contours in each one of their languages, which could be potentially used as language-specific cues in a language-identification task. It is reasonable to assume therefore that language-specific intonation is available to these speakers, if they were prompted to tell their two languages apart. However, whether this prosodic information is used for that purpose or not requires further investigation.
Although we were able to see the realization of flat and plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero, from the intonation envelope of statements, which is a wider perspective, these were also reported previously in Correa (2012, 2017). It all leads us to believe that perhaps the phonetic implementation of high peaks in accented syllables of Palenquero might help Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals maintain their languages as intonationally distinct. More work needs to be done in order to understand, first, whether Palenquero/Spanish use high-tone plateauing exclusively in Palenquero, and second, whether Spanish prosody has not contributed to this pattern. Surely these questions have a bearing on more general questions such as why high-tone plateauing is observed in Saramaccan (Good 2004), and in O’odham language, which has been typologically labeled as a language without tone (Hale 1959; Saxton 1963), and also in Chilean Spanish, whose intonation may also depict a hat pattern with a sustained high plateau (Rogers 2013, pp. 178–79). Moreover, it is still not clear whether plateau-shaped and flat contours in Palenquero might also constitute evidence of Palenquero prosody derived from the stress-to-tone mapping for a hypothetical high tone during Spanish stress reinterpretation (For stress-to-tone mapping see Steien and Yakpo 2020; Yakpo 2021).
With all this in mind, in this study, speakers’ behavior suggested that high-peak plateauing could drive language-specific intonation across Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals. Therefore, the lack of steeper declinations, in Palenquero, would result from high-peak plateauing across prenuclear intonation. The declination tends to be suspended while the final peak is reached in nuclear intonation, especially when the F0 peak is associated with trochaic stress. Moreover, F0 peaks in nuclear position tend to be higher in Palenquero as compared to Spanish, a pattern that appears to be driven also by high-peak plateauing.

4.3. Final Stress and Final Intonation in Tandem

Final stress effects were large, in particular, for the second and third modes of F0 variance, as the scope of the first most substantial variance did not cover final intonation. We learned that statement intonation then changes substantially when stress in phrase-final position is controlled. Contrary to all expectations, L% truncation did not seem to have outnumbered instances where L% tones were reached above the iambic stress pattern in nuclear position. See for example Figure 23 and Figure 24 both ending with iambic stress, and uttered by the same elderly speaker. Rather, final stress could drive flatter implementations in nuclear position.
Results reveal that L% tones were much lower when final trochees were associated with the nuclear contour. Thus, final intonation tends to be flat when anchored to final iambs, regardless of the language used, which emphasizes the importance we need to attach to stress effects on intonational variation. There may be however an intersection between language-specific intonation and final stress, as found in the first mode of F0 variance, given that the intonational difference between Palenquero and Spanish was significantly wider when statements ended with final iambs. Since this mode of F0 variance (i.e., FPC1) primarily covers prenuclear intonation, the difference could indeed be the result of higher initial rises in Spanish showing large pitch excursions, and steeper declinations when statement intonation is constrained by final iambs, as shown in Figure 24.
Palenquero statements ending with final iambs did not show wider pitch excursions in initial position and the slope of declinations was not as steep as in Spanish (cf. Figure 23). The main implication of this result is that speakers might have increased their speech effort in order to accurately produce F0 peaks in statement-initial position, in Spanish, when final iambs were used in this final position (see The effort code in Gussenhoven 2004). This is not to say that final stress is the cause for this behavior, but it is associated with Spanish initial rises. In either case, most Spanish speakers typically produce steeper rises in initial position, with delay peaks (see Beckman et al. 2002; Hualde and Prieto 2015), as presented earlier in Section 1.4.2.

4.4. Final Remarks on Generational Effects

Was the speech effort expended on Spanish statements similar between adult and elderly speakers? Generational effects indicate that elderly bilinguals show a wider distance between the intonation of their two languages as compared to adults. It is also worth noting that results did not suggest that language-specific intonation on the statements of adult bilinguals was sharply distinguished from elderly speakers. Both generations therefore use language-specific intonation, but elderly speakers exhibit much steeper declinations in Spanish which has also resulted in L% tones that are produced at a lower pitch floor compared to Palenquero. Initial rises are also slightly higher, showing wider pitch excursions that reflect a greater speech effort on the part of elderly speakers, which is interpreted here as a case of hyperarticulation. As explained in Section 1.3, bilingual speakers may show hyperarticulation when communicative constraints are at a higher level (Lindblom 1990), leading to greater articulatory precision. In other words, due to environment or processing demands, elderly bilinguals could have increased pitch in Spanish, widening prosodic differences between their two languages.
Recall that bilingual speakers show transfer, interference, or simplification of prosodic patterns that might lead to convergence (Bullock and Toribio 2004; Colantoni and Gurlekian 2004; Simonet 2011), especially because languages in the bilingual mind are not fully autonomous (Paradis 2001). For this reason, we could understand why some creole languages have developed hybrid prosodic systems (Gooden et al. 2009). Therefore, it could also be expected that bilingual intonation finds contexts to merge over the long term. Along these lines, there would also be a reasonable prospect that adult bilinguals are gradually simplifying language-specific intonational patterns, as the intonational distance between their languages was slightly narrower, compared with elderly bilinguals. The fact that adults did not use penultimate lengthening language-specifically could also lead us to believe that there is one sign of intonational convergence across adult bilinguals, as this feature has been typically used by previous generations (Friedemann and Patiño-Roselli 1983; Montes 1962). Thus, age effects could not be set aside, first of all, because, unlike adults, the elderly lived with Palenquero monolingual speakers, and did not have the need to learn Palenquero at school (Morton 2005; Schwegler 2011b).
In view of this, it has been hypothesized here that adult speakers should exhibit more intonational simplification or convergence than elderly bilinguals. Schwegler (2011a, p. 158) argued that prior to the mid-1990s, the Palenquero creole did not have external projection, that is, it was not part of the common knowledge in Cartagena de Indias, nor was throughout the rest of Colombia. In fact, during this period the use of Palenquero was on a steady decline. Then, it is likely that, at the end of the 1980s, the intergenerational transmission of Palenquero creole had been dramatically reduced. In 1983, Patiño-Roselli (1983, pp. 187–88) reported that conversations, taking place while children and adolescents were playing games, were in the local Spanish variety, and not in Palenquero. Despite the decline of Palenquero throughout the decade of the 1980s, an extraordinary revival of the creole has placed Palenquero in the common knowledge of Colombian people, being a reason to be proud, pooling efforts to reverse the decline, and preserving the creole (Schwegler 2011a). As a result of the revitalization program of Palenquero, and despite that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals have been in contact with Spanish for around four centuries, scholars have reached the consensus that this creole has not undergone a significant process of decreolization (Dussias et al. 2016; Lipski 2016; Schwegler 2000, 2011b). Because of this, the fact that both adult and elderly speakers were showing language-specific intonation across statements could prove that if the language revival program has not recovered all the distinctive features of Palenquero prosody, it has certainly served well to preserve at least some of them in the bilingual production of adult speakers. Therefore, it is likely that age effects on language-specific intonation have been found because of both hyperarticulation in Spanish intonation, on the part of elderly speakers, and prosodic simplification, on the part of adult speakers.

5. Conclusions

The main conclusion to be drawn from this study is that Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals can keep their languages temporally and intonationally distinct to a varying extent, depending on stress and age effects. It has been shown that intonation is also gradient (Gussenhoven 1999), and this nature has manifested through Palenquero/Spanish statements. Despite the intense contact of Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals with Caribbean Spanish, the present study proves that these bilinguals may still use prosodic patterns language-specifically. Since creoles may exhibit hybrid prosodic systems, as has been previously reported for Papiamento (Kouwenberg and Muysken 1994), Saramaccan (Bakker et al. 1994; Good 2004), Sranan (Adamson and Smith 1994) or Fa d’Ambu (Post 1994), on the understanding that these creole speakers use (residual) tones from their African ancestors, and stress from the European language that was imposed on them, it seems fair to believe that a prosodic reinterpretation of stress using tonal features would have inevitably taken place in these populations. As a result, language-specific prosody should also be expected to merge. What is more, if we see how prosodic outcomes evolve across bilingual speakers as in the case of bilingual English–French speakers (Bullock 2009), Turkish/German bilinguals (Queen 2006), Dutch/Greek bilinguals (Mennen 2004), or Quechua/Spanish bilinguals (O’Rourke 2012), we could also think that intonational convergence in bilingual speakers should be inescapable over the course of time. Nevertheless, statement prosody in Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals suggests that the prosodic distinction between their two languages is still preserved in spite of the course of time. The fact that adult bilinguals do not use penultimate lengthening language-specifically, as elderly speakers do, suggests however that language-specific prosody could have been adversely diminished among new generations. This may help explain why within the context of gradient prosody, adults show convergence in the duration dimension, and not in the F0 dimension, where they rather exhibit a gradient simplification of intonation patterns.
In line with the gradient nature of these results, we cannot reject the idea that adults’ intonation is not substantially different from the elderly’s. That is, both adult and elderly speakers produced plateau-shaped contours in Palenquero, and steeper rises followed by steeper declinations in Spanish. Therefore, generational differences may be observed at the phonetic level, resulting from hyperarticulation, as was put forth here for statement intonation across elderly speakers. These findings are important, since they reflect how Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals use language-specific intonation, which is deemed necessary before we proceed with the comparison of these bilinguals with monolingual speakers of Caribbean Spanish, and before we test prosodic patterns in perceptual experiments.

Funding

This research was funded by the UMASS GRADUATE SCHOOL, and the CENTER FOR LATIN AMERICAN, CARIBBEAN AND LATINO STUDIES, at the University of Massachusetts Amherst; in Amherst, MA, United States.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The present study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST. This study results from the protocol entitled “The intonation of questions and declarative utterances in Palenquero” (IRB Number: 559; date of approval: 22 September 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Acknowledgments

I express my gratitude to Ana Joaquina Cásseres and Bernardino Pérez Miranda, as well as all the Palenquero speakers who participated in this study, and to the whole community for having shared their language and culture in this research. I also gratefully acknowledge the support and insightful comments of Meghan Armstrong, John Lipski, and Kristine Yu. They have made a large contribution to this study which is part of my Ph.D. dissertation. Furthermore, I am deeply grateful to the two anonymous reviewers for their comments and suggestions to improve both the form and the substance of this manuscript. I am solely responsible for all the remaining shortcomings.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Notes

1
Initially, this pattern was labeled as L*+H (Beckman et al. 2002).
2
Because the original document is in Spanish, and was published in 2012.
3
The F0 contours presented in this section do not represent, and are not related to the statements collected and analyzed for this study. Although they were uttered by Palenquero/Spanish bilinguals, are presented here with the aim of illustrating how FPCA can be reversed to see the contribution to variation from dimensions in each FPC.
4
Figures’ labels referring to final trochees and final iambs are meant for the reader to find stress patterns in syllables four and five (i.e., S4 and S5, or between time slices 30 to 50).

References

  1. Aalberse, Suzanne, and Pieter Muysken. 2019. Bilingualism and Language Contact. In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism. Edited by Annick De Houwer and Lourdes Ortega. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 524–43. [Google Scholar]
  2. Adamson, Lilian, and Norval Smith. 1994. Sranan. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction. Edited by Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken and Norval Smith. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 15, pp. 219–32. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alvord, Scott Mark. 2006. Spanish Intonation in Contact: The Case of Miami Cuban Bilinguals. Ph.D. thesis, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA. [Google Scholar]
  4. Antoniou, Mark, Catherine Best, Michael Tyler, and Christian Kroos. 2011. Inter-language interference in VOT production by L2-dominant bilinguals: Asymmetries in phonetic code-switching. Journal of Phonetics 39: 558–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Appel, René, and Pieter Muysken. 2005. Language Contact and Bilingualism. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Armstrong, Meghan E. 2012. The Development of Yes-No Question Intonation in Puerto Rican Spanish. Ph.D. thesis, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. [Google Scholar]
  7. Arvaniti, Amalia. 2022. The Autosegmental-Metrical Model of Intonational Phonology. In Prosodic Theory and Practice. Edited by Jonathan Barnes and Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 25–83. [Google Scholar]
  8. Asano, Yuki, Michele Gubian, and Dominik Sacha. 2016. Cutting down on manual pitch contour annotation using data modeling. Paper presented at the Speech Prosody 2016, Boston, MA, USA, May 31–June 3; pp. 282–86. [Google Scholar]
  9. Aston, John, Jeng-Min Chiou, and Jonathan Evans. 2010. Linguistic pitch analysis using functional principal component mixed effect models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C (Applied Statistics) 59: 297–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bakker, Peter, Norval Smith, and Tonjes Veenstra. 1994. Saramaccan. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction. Edited by Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken and Norval Smith. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 15, pp. 165–78. [Google Scholar]
  11. Barlow, Jessica A. 2014. Age of acquisition and allophony in Spanish-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Beckman, Mary E., and Jennifer J. Venditti. 2011. Intonation. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed. Edited by John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle and Yu Alan. Medford: Wiley Online Library, pp. 485–532. [Google Scholar]
  13. Beckman, Mary E., Manuel Díaz-Campos, Julia Tevis McGory, and Terrell A. Morgan. 2002. Intonation across Spanish, in the Tones and Break Indices framework. Probus 14: 9–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bentley, William Holman. 1887. Dictionary and Grammar of the Kongo Language: As Spoken at San Salvador, the Ancient Capital of the Old Kongo Empire, West Africa. London: Baptist Missionary Society, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown, James Dean. 2001. Pragmatics tests: Different purposes, different tests. In Pragmatics in Language Teaching. Edited by Kenneth R. Rose and Gabriele Kasper. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 301–26. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bullock, Barbara E. 2009. Prosody in contact in French: A case study from a heritage variety in the USA. International Journal of Bilingualism 13: 165–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bullock, Barbara E., and Almeida Jacqueline Toribio. 2004. Introduction: Convergence as an emergent property in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 91–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bullock, Barbara E., and Chip Gerfen. 2004. Phonological convergence in a contracting language variety. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 95–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Butaye, René. 1909. Dictionnaire kikongo-français, français-kikongo. Edited by J. de Meester. Roulers: imprimeur-éditeur. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cabrera-Abreu, Mercedes, and Francisco Vizcaíno-Ortega. 2010. Canarian Spanish intonation. In Transcription of Intonation of the Spanish Language. Edited by Pilar Prieto and Paolo Roseano. Munich: Lincom Europa, pp. 87–121. [Google Scholar]
  21. Colantoni, Laura. 2011. Broad-focus declaratives in Argentine Spanish contact and non-contact varieties. In Intonational Phrasing in Romance and Germanic: Cross-Linguistic and Bilingual Studies. Edited by Christoph Gabriel and Conxita Lleó. Hamburg studies on multilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 183–212. [Google Scholar]
  22. Colantoni, Laura, and Jorge Gurlekian. 2004. Convergence and intonation: Historical evidence from Buenos Aires Spanish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 107–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cole, Peter. 1982. Imbabura Quechua. In Lingua Descriptive Studies. Edited by Bernard Comrie, Norval Smith and Anna de Hass. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company, vol. 5. [Google Scholar]
  24. Cook, R. Dennis. 1977. Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics 19: 15–18. [Google Scholar]
  25. Correa, José Alejandro. 2012. La entonación del palenquero y del kateyano hablado en Palenque, Colombia. In Palenque Colombia. Oralidad, identidad y Resistencia. Edited by Graciela Maglia and Armin Schwegler. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo, pp. 31–56. [Google Scholar]
  26. Correa, José Alejandro. 2017. Intonation in Palenquero Creole and Palenquero Spanish, Colombia. Orality, Identity, and Resistance in Palenque, Colombia: An Interdisciplinary Approach 54: 245. [Google Scholar]
  27. de Clercq, Aug. 1907. Grammaire du Kiyombe. (Suite et fin). Anthropos II: 761–94. [Google Scholar]
  28. Deuchar, Margaret, and Angeles Clark. 1996. Early bilingual acquisition of the voicing contrast in English and Spanish. Journal of Phonetics 24: 351–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dupoux, Emmanuel, Núria Sebastián-Gallés, Eduardo Navarrete, and Sharon Peperkamp. 2008. Persistent stress ‘deafness’: The case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition 106: 682–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dussias, Paola E., Jason W. Gullifer, and Timothy J. Poepsel. 2016. How psycholinguistics can inform contact linguistics: Converging evidence against a decreolization view of Palenquero. In The Iberian Challenge: Creole Languages beyond the Plantation Setting. Edited by Armin Schwegler, John McWhorter and Liane Ströbel. Frankfurt: Iberoamericana Vervuert, pp. 181–204. [Google Scholar]
  31. Elordieta, Gorka. 2003. The Spanish intonation of speakers of a Basque pitch-accent dialect. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 2: 67–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Elordieta, Gorka, and Aritz Irurtzun. 2016. Pitch accent tonal alignment in declarative sentences in the Spanish of the Basque Country: A study of language contact. In Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Subfields. Edited by Meghan E. Armstrong, lNicholas Henriksen and Maria del Mar Vanrell. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 25–44. [Google Scholar]
  33. Face, Timothy L. 2001. Focus and early peak alignment in Spanish intonation. Probus 13: 223–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Flege, James Emil, and Wieke Eefting. 1987. Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers. Journal of Phonetics 15: 67–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fontanella de Weinberg, María Beatriz. 1966. Comparación de dos entonaciones regionales argentinas. Thesavrvs 21: 17–29. [Google Scholar]
  36. Fontanella de Weinberg, María Beatriz. 1980. Three intonational systems of Argentinean Spanish. In The Melody of Language. Edited by Linda R. Waugh and Cornelius H. van Schooneveld. Baltimore: University Park Press, pp. 115–26. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fox, John, and Sanford Weisberg. 2018. An R Companion to Applied Regression. Los Angeles: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  38. Friedemann, Nina, and Carlos Patiño-Roselli. 1983. Lengua y Sociedad en el Palenque de San Basilio. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  39. Frota, Sónia, and Pilar Prieto. 2015. Intonation in Romance: Systemic similarities and differences. In Intonation in Romance. Edited by Sónia Frota and Pilar Prieto. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 392–418. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fujisaki, Hiroya. 1997. Prosody, models, and spontaneous speech. In Computing Prosody: Computational Models for Processing Spontaneous Speech. Edited by Yoshinori Sagisaka, Nick Campbell and Norio Higuchi. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 27–42. [Google Scholar]
  41. Good, Jeff. 2004. Tone and accent in Saramaccan: Charting a deep split in the phonology of a language. Lingua 114: 575–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Gooden, Shelome. 2022. Intonation and Prosody in Creole Languages: An Evolving Ecology. Annual Review of Linguistics 8: 343–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gooden, Shelome, Kathy-Ann Drayton, and Mary E. Beckman. 2009. Tone inventories and tune-text alignments: Prosodic variation in hybrid prosodic systems. Studies in Language 33: 396–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Grosjean, François. 2008. Studying Bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gussenhoven, Carlos. 1999. Discreteness and gradience in intonational contrasts. Language and Speech 42: 283–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2004. The Phonology of Tone and Intonation. New York: Research Series in Linguistics, Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  47. Gutiérrez, Manuel J. 1994. Simplification, transfer, and convergence in Chicano Spanish. Bilingual Review/La Revista Bilingüe 19: 111–21. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hale, Kenneth. 1959. A Papago Grammar. Ph.D. thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA. [Google Scholar]
  49. Hale, Kenneth, and Elisabeth Selkirk. 1987. Government and tonal phrasing in Papago. Phonology 4: 151–83. [Google Scholar]
  50. Holm, John. 2000. An Introduction to Pidgins and Creoles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hualde, José Ignacio. 2000. Intonation in Spanish and the other Ibero-Romance languages: Overview and status quaestionis. In Proceedings of the Romance Phonology and Variation (Selected Papers from the 30th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages). Edited by Caroline R. Wiltshire and Joaquim Camps. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 101–15. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hualde, José Ignacio. 2006. Remarks on word-prosodic typology. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, CA, USA, February 10–12; vol. 32, pp. 157–74. [Google Scholar]
  53. Hualde, José Ignacio, and Armin Schwegler. 2008. Intonation in Palenquero. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 23: 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hualde, José Ignacio, and Pilar Prieto. 2015. Intonational variation in Spanish: European and American varieties. In Intonation in Romance. Edited by Sonia Frota and Pilar Prieto. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 350–91. [Google Scholar]
  55. Hyman, Larry M. 2013. Penultimate lengthening in Bantu: Analysis and Spread. In Language Typology and Historical Contingency: In Honor of Johanna Nichols. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 104, pp. 309–30. [Google Scholar]
  56. Hyman, Larry M., and Kemmonye C. Monaka. 2011. Tonal and Non-Tonal Intonation in Shekgalagari. In Prosodic Categories: Production, Perception and Comprehension. Edited by Sonia Frota, Gorka Elordieta and Pilar Prieto. Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 267–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kaisse, Ellen M. 2001. The Long Fall: An intonational melody of Argentinian Spanish. In Features and Interfaces in Romance: Essays in Honor of Heles Contreras. Edited by Julia Herschensohn, Enrique Mallén and Karen Zagona. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 148–60. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kasper, Gabriele, and Merete Dahl. 1991. Research methods in interlanguage pragmatics. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13: 215–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kouwenberg, Silvia, and Pieter Muysken. 1994. Papiamento. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction. Edited by Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken and Norval Smith. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 15, pp. 205–18. [Google Scholar]
  60. Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Ladd, Robert D. 2008. Intonational Phonology, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  62. Laman, Karl Edvard. 1922. The Musical Accent or Intonation of the Kongo Languages. Stockholm: Forgotten Books. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lewis, Anthony. 1970. A Descriptive Analysis of the Palenquero Dialect: A Spanish-Based Creole of Northern Colombia, South America. Master’s thesis, University of the West Indies, Mona, Jamaica. [Google Scholar]
  64. Lindblom, Björn. 1990. Speech Production and Speech Modelling. Edited by William J. Hardcastle and Alain Marchal. Dordrecht: Springer, vol. 55, pp. 403–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lipski, John M. 2010. Pitch polarity in Palenquero: A possible locus of H tone. In Linguistic Studies in Romance Languages. Edited by Olarrea Colina and Carvalho. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 111–27. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lipski, John M. 2016. Palenquero and Spanish. Journal of Pidgin & Creole Languages 31: 42–81. [Google Scholar]
  67. Lumwamu, François. 1973. Essai de morphosyntaxe systématique des parlers kongo. Paris: Editions Klincksieck, vol. 10. [Google Scholar]
  68. Malmberg, Bertil. 1950. Études sur la phonétique de l'espagnol parlé en Argentine. Lund, Copenhague and Paris: CWK Gleerup, vol. 45. [Google Scholar]
  69. Marichelle, Christophe. 1902. Dictionnaire Vili-Français. Loango: La Mission. [Google Scholar]
  70. Martín, José, and Josefa Dorta. 2018. La entonación de Cuba. In La entonación declarativa e interrogativa en cinco zonas fronterizas del español: Canarias, Cuba, Venezuela, Colombia y San Antonio de Texas. Edited by Josefa Dorta. Lausanne: Peter Lang Publishing Group, pp. 109–30. [Google Scholar]
  71. Matras, Yaron. 2009. Language Contact. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  72. Mennen, Ineke. 2004. Bi-directional interference in the intonation of Dutch speakers of Greek. Journal of Phonetics 32: 543–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Montes, José J. 1962. Sobre el habla de San Basilio de Palenque. Thesaurus 17: 446–50. [Google Scholar]
  74. Montrul, Silvina. 2008. Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-Examining the Age Factor. Studies in Bilingualism. Philadelphia: Cambridge University Press, vol. 39. [Google Scholar]
  75. Moñino, Yves. 2017. Past, present, and future of Palenquero creole. In Orality, Identity, and Resistance in Palenque, Colombia. Edited by Armin Schwegler, Bryan Kirschen and Graciela Maglia. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
  76. Morton, Thomas. 2005. Sociolinguistic Variation and Language Change in El Palenque de San Basilio, Colombia. Ph.D. thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  77. Mufwene, Salikoko S. 1996. The Founder Principle in Creole Genesis. Diachronica 13: 83–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Mufwene, Salikoko S. 2001. The Ecology of Language Evolution. Cambridge Approaches to Language Contact. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  79. Muysken, Pieter, and Norval Smith. 1994. The study of pidgin and creole languages. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction. Edited by Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken and Norval Smith. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 15, pp. 3–14. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mücke, Doris, Anne Hermes, Timo B. Roettger, Johannes Becker, Henrik Niemann, Till A. Dembek, Lars Timmermann, Veerle Visser-Vandewalle, Gereon R. Fink, Martine Grice, and et al. 2018. The effects of thalamic deep brain stimulation on speech dynamics in patients with essential tremor: An articulographic study. PLoS ONE 13: e0191359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Navarrete, María Cristina. 2008. San Basilio de Palenque: Memoria y tradición. Surgimiento y avatares de las gestas cimarronas en el Caribe colombiano. Cali: Programa Editorial, Universidad del Valle. [Google Scholar]
  82. Noguera, María Claudia, Armin Schwegler, Verónica Gomes, Ignacio Briceño, Luis Alvarez, Daniel Uricoechea, Antonio Amorim, Escilda Benavides, Carlos Silvera, Marlon Charris, and et al. 2014. Colombia’s racial crucible: Y chromosome evidence from six admixed communities in the Department of Bolivar. Annals of Human Biology 41: 453–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Olson, Daniel. 2012. The phonetics of insertional code-switching: Suprasegmental analysis and a case for hyper-articulation. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2: 439–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Olson, Daniel. 2013. Bilingual language switching and selection at the phonetic level: Asymmetrical transfer in VOT production. Journal of Phonetics 41: 407–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. O’Rourke, Erin. 2005. Intonation and Language Contact: A Case Study of Two Varieties of Peruvian Spanish. Ph.D. thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
  86. O’Rourke, Erin. 2012. The realization of contrastive focus in Peruvian Spanish intonation. Lingua 122: 494–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Paradis, Johanne. 2001. Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of Bilingualism 5: 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Patiño-Roselli, Carlos. 1983. El habla en El Palenque de San Basilio. In Lengua y Sociedad en el Palenque de San Basilio. Edited by Nina S. de Friedemann and Carlos Patiño-Roselli. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo, pp. 83–287. [Google Scholar]
  89. Peirce, Jonathan W. 2007. PsychoPy, psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 162: 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Peirce, Jonathan W. 2009. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 2: 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Pérez-Tejedor, Juana Pabla. 2004. El Criollo de Palenque de San Basilio: Una visión estructural de su lengua. In Lenguas Aborígenes de Colombia (Descripciones 14). Number 14. Bogotá: Ediciones Uniandes. [Google Scholar]
  92. Post, Marike. 1994. Fa d’Ambu. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction. Edited by Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken and Norval Smith. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 15, pp. 191–204. [Google Scholar]
  93. Queen, Robin M. 2006. Phrase-final intonation in narratives told by Turkish-German bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 10: 153–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Quilis, Antonio. 1983. Frecuencia de los esquemas acentuales en español. In Estudios ofrecidos a Emilio Alarcos Llorach (con motivo de sus XXV años de docencia en la Universidad de Oviedo). Oviedo: Universidad de Oviedo, vol. 5, pp. 113–26. [Google Scholar]
  95. Ramsay, James, and Bernhard Silverman. 2005. Functional Data Analysis, 2nd ed. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Ramsay, James, Giles Hooker, and Spencer Graves. 2009. Functional Data Analysis with R and MATLAB, 1st ed. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 2197–5736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Rickford, John. 1980. Language Attitudes in a Creole Continuum. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society, of America, San Antonio, TX, USA, December 29. [Google Scholar]
  98. Rodríguez-Vázquez, Rosalía. 2019. The intonation of wh-questions in a language contact situation: The case of Galician and Galician Spanish bilingual speakers. Estudios de Fonética Experimental 28: 81–124. [Google Scholar]
  99. Rogers, Brandon. 2013. The extent of tonal events: Intonational hat patterns in Chilean Spanish. Estudios de Fonética Experimental 22: 171–92. [Google Scholar]
  100. Samba, Philippe. 1989. The System of Tone in Kikongo. Ph.D. thesis, University of Lancaster, Lancaster, UK. [Google Scholar]
  101. Saxton, Dean. 1963. Papago Phonemes. International Journal of American Linguistics 29: 29–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Schwegler, Armin. 2000. The myth of decreolization: The anomalous case of Palenquero. In Degrees of Restructuring in Creole Languages. Edited by Ingrid Neumann-Holzschuh and Edgar W. Schneider. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 409–36. [Google Scholar]
  103. Schwegler, Armin. 2001. Degrees of Restructuring in Creole Languages. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 409–36. [Google Scholar]
  104. Schwegler, Armin. 2011a. On the extraordinary revival of a creole: Palenquero (Colombia). In Paper presented at the Endangered Languages–Voices and Images, Proceedings of the Conference FEL XV—PUCE I, Quito, Ecuador, September 9; pp. 153–65. [Google Scholar]
  105. Schwegler, Armin. 2011b. Palenque (Colombia): Multilingualism in an Extraordinary Social and Historical Context. In The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics. Edited by M. Díaz-Campos. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 446–72. [Google Scholar]
  106. Schwegler, Armin. 2017. On the African origin(s) of Palenquero. In Orality, Identity, and Resistance in Palenque, Colombia. Edited by Armin Schwegler, Bryan Kirschen and Graciela Maglia. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 51–119. [Google Scholar]
  107. Sessarego, Sandro, and Rajiv Rao. 2016. On the simplification of a prosodic inventory: The Afro-Bolivian Spanish case. Inquiries in Hispanic Linguistics: From Theory to Empirical Evidence 12: 171–90. [Google Scholar]
  108. Shengli, Feng. 2019. Prosodic Syntax in Chinese: Theory and Facts. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  109. Silva-Corvalán, Carmen. 1990. Current Issues in Studies of Language Contact. Hispania 73: 162–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Simonet, Miquel. 2011. Intonational convergence in language contact: Utterance-final F0 contours in Catalan-Spanish early bilinguals. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 41: 157–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Simonet, Miquel. 2016. The Phonetics and Phonology of Bilingualism. In Oxford Handbook Topics in Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Smith, Hiram. 2014. Patterns of Variable Tense-Aspect Marking in Palenquero Creole. Ph.D. thesis, The Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  113. Sosa, Juan Manuel. 1991. Fonética y fonología de la entonación del español hispanoamericano. Ph.D. thesis, University of Massachussets Amherst, Amherst, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  114. Sosa, Juan Manuel. 1999. La entonación del español: Su estructura fónica, variabilidad y dialectología. Anaya: Cátedra. [Google Scholar]
  115. Stefanich, Sara, and Jennifer Cabrelli. 2020. Shared or separate representations? The Spanish palatal nasal in early Spanish/English bilinguals. Languages 5: 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Steien, Guri Bordal, and Kofi Yakpo. 2020. Romancing with tone: On the outcomes of prosodic contact. Language 96: 1–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Thomason, Sarah Grey. 2001. Language Contact: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. [Google Scholar]
  118. Thomason, Sarah Grey, and Terrence Kaufman. 1988. Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  119. van Coetsem, Frans. 1988. Loan Phonology and the Two Transfer Types in Language Contact. Number 27 in Publications in Language Sciences. Dordrecht: Foris Publications. [Google Scholar]
  120. Vanrell, Maria del Mar, Ingo Feldhausen, and Lluïsa Astruc. 2018. The Discourse Completion Task in Romance prosody research: Status quo and outlook. In Methods in Prosody: A Romance Language Perspective. Edited by Ingo Feldhausen, Jan Fliessbach and Maria del Mar Vanrell. Berlin: Language Science Press, pp. 191–227. [Google Scholar]
  121. Venables, William N., and William N. Ripley. 2002. Random and mixed effects. In Modern Applied Statistics with S. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 271–300. [Google Scholar]
  122. Xu, Yu. 2013. ProsodyPro: A tool for Large-scale Systematic Prosody Analysis. In Proceedings of the Proceedings of Tools and Resources for the Analysis of Speech Prosody (TRASP). Aix-en-Provence: Laboratoire Parole et Langage, pp. 7–10. [Google Scholar]
  123. Yakpo, Kofi. 2021. Creole prosodic systems are areal, not simple. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 690593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Tunes found in Spanish statements (adapted from Hualde and Prieto 2015).
Figure 1. Tunes found in Spanish statements (adapted from Hualde and Prieto 2015).
Languages 09 00132 g001
Figure 2. (A) Self-rated speaking proficiency in Palenquero. (B) Self-rated listening proficiency in Palenquero (N = 10).
Figure 2. (A) Self-rated speaking proficiency in Palenquero. (B) Self-rated listening proficiency in Palenquero (N = 10).
Languages 09 00132 g002
Figure 3. (A) Sample dataset with normalized three-syllable intonation contours from two groups. (B) Sample dataset averaged by Factor A and Factor B (n = 301).
Figure 3. (A) Sample dataset with normalized three-syllable intonation contours from two groups. (B) Sample dataset averaged by Factor A and Factor B (n = 301).
Languages 09 00132 g003
Figure 4. Reconstruction of the F0 contours, shown in Figure 3, by the incremental addition of FPCs, from FPC1 to FPC7. (AD) represent the first two most substantial modes of F0 variation (i.e., FPC1 and FPC2) and, taken together, account for 80.4% of the total variance.
Figure 4. Reconstruction of the F0 contours, shown in Figure 3, by the incremental addition of FPCs, from FPC1 to FPC7. (AD) represent the first two most substantial modes of F0 variation (i.e., FPC1 and FPC2) and, taken together, account for 80.4% of the total variance.
Languages 09 00132 g004
Figure 5. Predicted vowel length for both final trochees and iambs by generation, in Palenquero statements.
Figure 5. Predicted vowel length for both final trochees and iambs by generation, in Palenquero statements.
Languages 09 00132 g005
Figure 6. Predicted vowel length for both Palenquero and Spanish by generation, in final trochees.
Figure 6. Predicted vowel length for both Palenquero and Spanish by generation, in final trochees.
Languages 09 00132 g006
Figure 7. (A) All the intonation contours from statements normalized by subject, from Palenquero and Spanish. (B) All the intonation contours from statements averaged by language.
Figure 7. (A) All the intonation contours from statements normalized by subject, from Palenquero and Spanish. (B) All the intonation contours from statements averaged by language.
Languages 09 00132 g007
Figure 8. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC1 from the grand mean (solid line).
Figure 8. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC1 from the grand mean (solid line).
Languages 09 00132 g008
Figure 9. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by language.
Figure 9. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by language.
Languages 09 00132 g009
Figure 10. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by final stress and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by final stress and generation.
Figure 10. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by final stress and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by final stress and generation.
Languages 09 00132 g010
Figure 11. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by final stress and language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by final stress and language.
Figure 11. (A) Variance scheme in FPC1 by final stress and language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC1 averaged by final stress and language.
Languages 09 00132 g011
Figure 12. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC2 from the grand mean (solid line).
Figure 12. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC2 from the grand mean (solid line).
Languages 09 00132 g012
Figure 13. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by language.
Figure 13. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by language.
Languages 09 00132 g013
Figure 14. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by language and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by language and generation.
Figure 14. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by language and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by language and generation.
Languages 09 00132 g014
Figure 15. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by final stress. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by final stress.
Figure 15. (A) Variance scheme in FPC2 by final stress. (B) Variance scheme in FPC2 averaged by final stress.
Languages 09 00132 g015
Figure 16. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC3 from the grand mean (solid line).
Figure 16. Effect of adding (+++ line) and subtracting (−−− line) FPC3 from the grand mean (solid line).
Languages 09 00132 g016
Figure 17. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by language.
Figure 17. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by language. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by language.
Languages 09 00132 g017
Figure 18. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by language and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by language and generation.
Figure 18. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by language and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by language and generation.
Languages 09 00132 g018
Figure 19. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by final stress. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by final stress.
Figure 19. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by final stress. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by final stress.
Languages 09 00132 g019
Figure 20. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by final stress and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by final stress and generation.
Figure 20. (A) Variance scheme in FPC3 by final stress and generation. (B) Variance scheme in FPC3 averaged by final stress and generation.
Languages 09 00132 g020
Figure 21. Intonation on the Palenquero statement ané a ten loro ‘They have a parrot’, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Figure 21. Intonation on the Palenquero statement ané a ten loro ‘They have a parrot’, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Languages 09 00132 g021
Figure 22. Intonation on the Spanish statement yo vi tu loro “I saw your parrot”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Figure 22. Intonation on the Spanish statement yo vi tu loro “I saw your parrot”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Languages 09 00132 g022
Figure 23. Intonation on the Palenquero statement ané tan bombá “They are groveling (for food)”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Figure 23. Intonation on the Palenquero statement ané tan bombá “They are groveling (for food)”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Languages 09 00132 g023
Figure 24. Intonation on the Spanish statement yo vi tu bambú “I saw your bamboo”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Figure 24. Intonation on the Spanish statement yo vi tu bambú “I saw your bamboo”, uttered by an elderly speaker.
Languages 09 00132 g024
Table 1. Linear mixed-effects model for vowel duration in phrase-final position from statements (n = 231).
Table 1. Linear mixed-effects model for vowel duration in phrase-final position from statements (n = 231).
EstimateStd. Errort ValuePr(>F)CI[2.5%]CI[97.5%]VIFTolerance
Intercept−0.230.11−2.130.03 *−0.44−0.02
Final stress1.070.176.485.96 × 10 10 ***0.751.401.660.60
Language−0.050.14−0.380.70−0.330.221.640.61
Generation0.570.173.290.01 *0.230.922.360.42
Final stress:−0.800.26−3.060.002 **−1.32−0.282.010.50
Generation
Language:−0.750.23−3.310.001 **−1.20−0.302.680.37
Generation
Random effectsSpeakerStandard Deviation
Intercept0.00002
Residual0.84
F-test12.90 on 5 and 225 DFp-value4.844 × 10 11 ***
Residual standard error0.84 on 225 DF
AIC599.06BIC626.39logLik−291.53
R20.22Adjusted R20.21
n231
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC1 in statements (n = 196).
Table 2. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC1 in statements (n = 196).
EstimateStd. Errort ValuePr(>F)CI[2.5%]CI[97.5%]VIFTolerance
Intercept−1.070.40−2.660.009 **−1.87−0.28
Final stress0.210.760.270.78−1.291.712.660.38
Language1.640.453.634 × 10 4 ***0.752.541.310.76
Generation0.210.520.400.70−0.811.221.240.81
Final stress:2.360.932.520.013 *0.514.202.220.45
Language
Final stress:−1.910.95−2.010.046 *−3.79−0.041.940.52
Generation
Random effectsSpeakerStandard Deviation
Intercept0.35
Residual2.77
F-test8.63 on 5 and 190 DFp-value2.17 × 10 7 ***
Residual standard error2.77 on 190 DF
AIC964.98BIC990.95logLik−474.49
R20.19Adjusted R20.16
n196
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC2 in statements (n = 196).
Table 3. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC2 in statements (n = 196).
EstimateStd. Errort ValuePr(>F)CI[2.5%]CI[97.5%]VIFTolerance
Intercept1.680.394.263.31 × 10 5 **0.902.46
Final stress−3.040.21−14.22.99 × 10 31 ***−3.46−2.621.000.99
Language−1.160.23−4.971.55 × 10 6 ***−1.62−0.701.660.60
Generation−0.470.62−0.770.46−1.690.741.110.90
Language:−0.760.37−2.030.043 *−1.49−0.021.760.57
Generation
Random effectsSpeakerStandard Deviation
Intercept0.86
Residual1.27
F-test74.03 on 4 and 191 DFp-value8.70 × 10 38 ***
Residual standard error1.27 on 191 DF
AIC684.58BIC707.35logLik−335.29
R20.61Adjusted R20.60
n196
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC3 in statements (n = 196).
Table 4. Linear mixed-effects model for FPC3 in statements (n = 196).
EstimateStd. Errort ValuePr(>F)CI[2.5%]CI[97.5%]VIFTolerance
Intercept−0.120.33−0.360.72−0.770.53
Final stress2.430.386.371.53 × 10 9 ***1.683.182.660.38
Language−0.660.28−2.400.018 *−1.21−0.121.940.52
Generation0.960.511.860.10−0.061.971.280.78
Final stress:−0.760.47−1.620.11−1.680.172.220.45
Language
Final stress:−1.280.48−2.680.008 **−2.22−0.341.760.57
Generation
Language:−1.140.41−2.810.006 **−1.94−0.341.860.54
Generation
Random effectsSpeakerStandard Deviation
Intercept0.63
Residual1.39
F-test18.50 on 6 and 189 DFp-value7.14 × 10 17 ***
Residual standard error1.39 on 189 DF
AIC713.94BIC743.12logLik−347.97
R20.37Adjusted R20.35
n196
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lopez-Barrios, W. Language-Specific Prosody in Statements of Palenquero/Spanish Bilinguals. Languages 2024, 9, 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040132

AMA Style

Lopez-Barrios W. Language-Specific Prosody in Statements of Palenquero/Spanish Bilinguals. Languages. 2024; 9(4):132. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040132

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lopez-Barrios, Wilmar. 2024. "Language-Specific Prosody in Statements of Palenquero/Spanish Bilinguals" Languages 9, no. 4: 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040132

APA Style

Lopez-Barrios, W. (2024). Language-Specific Prosody in Statements of Palenquero/Spanish Bilinguals. Languages, 9(4), 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040132

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop