Exploring Whether Making Second-Language Vocabulary Learning Difficult Enhances Retention and Transfer
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Subjects
2.1.2. Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.1.4. Design
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Learning
2.2.2. Test
2.3. Summary and Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Subjects
3.1.2. Materials
3.1.3. Procedure
3.1.4. Design
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Learning
3.2.2. Test
3.3. Summary and Discussion
4. General Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Items Used in Experiment 1
Category | French | English |
body parts | jambe | leg |
dos | back | |
bouche | mouth | |
figure | face | |
yeux | eyes | |
doigt | finger | |
clothing | manteau | coat |
chemise | shirt | |
chaussures | shoes | |
cravate | tie | |
jupe | skirt | |
lunettes | glasses | |
dining | fourchette | fork |
cuiller | spoon | |
couteau | knife | |
assiette | plate | |
verre | glass | |
serviette | napkin | |
food | pain | bread |
beurre | butter | |
lait | milk | |
oeufs | eggs | |
jambon | ham | |
fromage | cheese | |
school | cahier | notebook |
livre | book | |
stylo | pen | |
ordinateur | computer | |
devoir | assignment | |
note | grade | |
transportation | voiture | car |
velo | bicycle | |
avion | airplane | |
moto | motorcycle | |
camion | truck | |
bateau | boat |
Appendix B. Items Used in Experiment 2
Category | French | English |
body parts | bouche | mouth |
bras | arm | |
coeur | heart | |
cou | neck | |
doigt | finger | |
dos | back | |
genou | knee | |
jambe | leg | |
main | hand | |
yeux | eyes | |
oreille | ear | |
figure | face | |
clothing | calecon | shorts |
chapeau | hat | |
chaussures | shoes | |
chemise | shirt | |
gant | glove | |
lunettes | glasses | |
manteau | coat | |
robe | dress | |
cravate | tie | |
veston | jacket | |
jupe | skirt | |
tailleur | suit | |
dining | jatte | bowl |
assiette | plate | |
couteau | knife | |
nappe | tablecloth | |
tasse | cup | |
fourchette | fork | |
cuiller | spoon | |
verre | glass | |
louche | ladle | |
serviette | napkin | |
timbale | mug | |
cruche | pitcher | |
food | aubergine | eggplant |
beurre | butter | |
epices | spices | |
farine | flour | |
fromage | cheese | |
gateau | cake | |
haricots | beans | |
legume | vegetable | |
nouille | noodle | |
oeufs | eggs | |
pain | bread | |
cerise | cherry | |
people | epoux | spouse |
fille | girl | |
fils | son | |
garcon | boy | |
homme | man | |
soeur | sister | |
boulanger | baker | |
ecrivain | writer | |
femme | women | |
pretre | priest | |
reine | queen | |
roi | king | |
places | bibliotheque | library |
ecole | school | |
eglise | church | |
epicerie | grocery | |
magasin | store | |
maison | house | |
piscine | pool | |
plage | beach | |
gare | station | |
ferme | farm | |
immeuble | apartment | |
salle | room | |
school | valise | bag |
cahier | notebook | |
livre | book | |
note | grade | |
ordinateur | computer | |
stylo | pen | |
craie | chalk | |
crayon | pencil | |
devoir | assignment | |
pupitre | desk | |
chaise | chair | |
grattoir | eraser | |
transportation | bateau | boat |
camion | truck | |
chaloupe | canoe | |
fusee | rocket | |
velo | bicycle | |
velomoteur | moped | |
voiture | car | |
cheval | horse | |
planeur | glider | |
avion | plane | |
charrette | wagon | |
patin | skate |
References
- Bach, N. T. A., & Barclay, S. (2025). The effect of semantic and physical similarity on vocabulary learning. The Language Learning Journal, 53(1), 40–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Battig, W. F. (1972). Intratask interference as a source of facilitation in transfer and retention. In R. F. Thompson, & J. F. Voss (Eds.), Topics in learning and performance (pp. 131–159). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Battig, W. F. (1979). The flexibility of human memory. In L. S. Cermak, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing and human memory (pp. 23–44). Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 229–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. (2021). Bilingualism: Pathway to cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(5), 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56–64). Worth Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Bjork, E. L., Little, J. L., & Storm, B. C. (2014). Multiple-choice testing as a desirable difficulty in the classroom. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 165–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe, & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2020). Desirable difficulties in theory and practice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9(4), 475–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bjork, R. A., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Desirable difficulties in vocabulary learning. American Journal of Psychology, 128, 241–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bolger, P. A., & Zapata, G. C. (2011). Semantic categories and context in L2 vocabulary learning. Language Learning, 61(2), 614–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brunmair, M., & Richter, T. (2019). Similarity matters: A meta-analysis of interleaved learning and its moderators. Psychological Bulletin, 145(11), 1029–1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, S. K. (2017). Spacing effects on learning and memory. In J. T. Wixted (Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (2nd ed., pp. 465–485). Academic Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, S. K., & Mueller, F. E. (2013). The effects of interleaving versus blocking on foreign language pronunciation learning. Memory & Cognition, 41(5), 671–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Effects of interleaved and blocked study on delayed test of category learning generalization. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, J. C. K., Davis, S. D., Yurtsever, A., & Myers, S. J. (2024). The magnitude of the testing effect is independent of retrieval practice performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 153(7), 1816–1836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Currie, N. K., & Muijselaar, M. M. L. (2019). Inference making in young children: The concurrent and longitudinal contributions of verbal working memory and vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(8), 1416–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Czyż, S. H., Wójcik, A. M., Solarská, P., & Kiper, P. (2024). High contextual interference improves retention in motor learning: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Scientific Reports, 14, 15974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Bruin, A. B. H. (2023). Dealing with desirable difficulties: Supporting students to accept, reduce, or silence effort. Medical Science Educator, 33(Suppl. 1), 5–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eglington, L. G., & Kang, S. H. K. (2017). Interleaved presentation benefits science category learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 475–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, G. F., & Harley, T. A. (1996). List learning of second language vocabulary. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17(4), 443–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hashemi, M. R., & Gowdasiaei, F. (2005). An attribute-treatment interaction study: Lexical-set versus semantically-unrelated vocabulary instruction. RELC Journal, 36(3), 341–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Healy, A. F., Fendrich, D. W., Crutcher, R. J., Wittman, W. T., Gesi, A. T., Ericsson, K. A., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (1992). The long-term retention of skills. In A. F. Healy, S. M. Kosslyn, & R. M. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 87–118). Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Hoshino, Y. (2010). The categorical facilitation effects on L2 vocabulary learning in a classroom setting. RELC Journal, 41(3), 301–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jongman, S. R., Khoe, Y. H., & Hintz, F. (2021). Vocabulary size influences spontaneous speech in native language users: Validating the use of automatic speech recognition in individual differences research. Language and Speech, 64(1), 35–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Learning painting styles: Spacing is advantageous when it promotes discriminative contrast. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(1), 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kievit, R. A., Hofman, A. D., & Nation, K. (2019). Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning in young children: A replication and extension of the study by Kievit et al. (2017). Psychological Science, 30(8), 1245–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kievit, R. A., Lindenberger, U., Goodyer, I. M., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P., Bullmore, E. T., & Dolan, R. J. (2017). Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning supports cognitive development during late adolescence and early adulthood. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1419–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kole, J. A., & Healy, A. F. (2007). Using prior knowledge to minimize interference when learning large amounts of information. Memory & Cognition, 35(1), 124–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornell, N. (2009). Optimising learning using flashcards: Spacing is more effective than cramming. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(9), 1297–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science, 19(6), 585–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCurdy, M. P., Viechtbauer, W., Sklenar, A. M., Frankenstein, A. N., & Leshikar, E. D. (2020). Theories of the generation effect and the impact of generation constraint: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(6), 1139–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNamara, D. S., & Healy, A. F. (2000). A procedural explanation of the generation effect for simple and difficult multiplication problems and answers. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(4), 652–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mielicki, M. K., & Wiley, J. (2022). Exploring the necessary conditions for observing interleaved practice benefits in math learning. Learning and Instruction, 80(3), 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mousavi, S. H., Kakhki, A. S., Fazeli, D., Vogel, L., Horst, F., & Schöllhorn, W. I. (2024). Effects of contextual interference and differential learning on performance and mental representations in a golf putting task. European Journal of Sport Science, 24(3), 289–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakata, T., Suzuki, Y., & He, X. (2023). Costs and benefits of spacing for second language vocabulary learning: Does relearning override the positive and negative effects of spacing? Language Learning, 73, 799–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakata, T., & Webb, S. A. (2016). Does studying vocabulary in smaller sets increase learning? The effects of part and whole learning on second language vocabulary acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 523–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olinghouse, N. G., & Leaird, J. T. (2009). The relationship between measures of vocabulary and narrative writing quality in second-and fourth-grade students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(5), 545–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olinghouse, N. G., & Wilson, J. (2013). The relationship between vocabulary and writing quality in three genres. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26(1), 45–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raviv, L., Lupyan, G., & Green, S. C. (2022). How variability shapes learning and generalization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(6), 462–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning. Instructional Science, 35(6), 481–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sana, F., Yan, V. X., Kim, J. A., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2018). Does working memory capacity moderate the interleaving benefit? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, V. I., Healy, A. F., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (1998). Contextual interference effects in foreign language vocabulary acquisition and retention. In A. F. Healy, & L. E. Bourne Jr. (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention (pp. 77–90). Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, V. I., Healy, A. F., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (2002). What is learned under difficult conditions is hard to forget: Contextual interference effects in foreign vocabulary acquisition, retention, and transfer. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 419–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, V. I., Healy, A. F., Ericsson, K. A., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (1995). The effects of contextual interference on the acquisition and retention of logical rules. In A. F. Healy, & L. E. Bourne Jr. (Eds.), Learning and memory of knowledge and skills: Durability and specificity (pp. 95–131). Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Shao, Z., Janse, E., Visser, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shea, J. B., & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5(2), 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorensen, L. J., & Woltz, D. J. (2016). Blocking as a friend of induction in verbal category learning. Memory & Cognition, 44(7), 1000–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Terai, M., Yamashita, J., & Pasich, K. E. (2021). Effects of learning direction in retrieval practice on EFL vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(5), 1116–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tinkham, T. (1993). The effect of semantic clustering on the learning of second language vocabulary. System, 21, 371–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tinkham, T. (1997). The effects of semantic and thematic clustering on the learning of second language vocabulary. Second Language Research, 13, 138–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2010). Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 447–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Overschelde, J. P., & Healy, A. F. (2001). Learning of nondomain facts in high-and low-knowledge domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(5), 1160–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, R. K., & Meros, D. (2010). Vocabulary and reading comprehension: Direct, indirect, and reciprocal influences. Focus on Exceptional Children, 2010(1), 1G1-245473138. [Google Scholar]
- Webb, S. (2009). The effects of receptive and productive learning of word pairs on vocabulary knowledge. RELC Journal, 40(3), 360–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xie, H., Wang, F., Hao, Y., Chen, J., An, J., Wang, Y., & Liu, H. (2017). The more total cognitive load is reduced by cues, the better retention and transfer of multimedia learning: A meta-analysis and two meta-regression analyses. PLoS ONE, 12(8), e0183884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, C., Luo, L., Vadillo, M. A., Yu, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2021). Testing (quizzing) boosts classroom learning: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 399–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Healy, A.F.; Schneider, V.I.; Kole, J.A. Exploring Whether Making Second-Language Vocabulary Learning Difficult Enhances Retention and Transfer. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 692. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050692
Healy AF, Schneider VI, Kole JA. Exploring Whether Making Second-Language Vocabulary Learning Difficult Enhances Retention and Transfer. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(5):692. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050692
Chicago/Turabian StyleHealy, Alice F., Vivian I. Schneider, and James A. Kole. 2025. "Exploring Whether Making Second-Language Vocabulary Learning Difficult Enhances Retention and Transfer" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 5: 692. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050692
APA StyleHealy, A. F., Schneider, V. I., & Kole, J. A. (2025). Exploring Whether Making Second-Language Vocabulary Learning Difficult Enhances Retention and Transfer. Behavioral Sciences, 15(5), 692. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050692