Next Article in Journal
Current Expertise, Opinions, and Attitude toward TNF-⍺ Antagonist Biosimilars among Physicians: A Self-Administered Online Survey in Western Switzerland
Next Article in Special Issue
Telephone-Delivered Interventions for Suicide Prevention in Schizophrenia and Related Disorders: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Viral Response among Early Treated HIV Perinatally Infected Infants: Description of a Cohort in Southern Mozambique
Previous Article in Special Issue
Biological and Clinical Aspects on the Treatment of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders: New Challenges
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Heterogeneity in Response to MCT and Psychoeducation: A Feasibility Study Using Latent Class Mixed Models in First-Episode Psychosis

by
Marta Ferrer-Quintero
1,2,3,
Daniel Fernández
3,4,5,
Raquel López-Carrilero
1,3,6,
Luciana Díaz-Cutraro
1,6,7,
Marina Verdaguer-Rodríguez
1,6,8,
Helena García-Mieres
9,10,11,
Elena Huerta-Ramos
1,3,
Juana Gómez-Benito
2,12,
Trini Peláez
1,3,
Irene Birulés
1,2,
Ana Barajas
13,14,
Esther Pousa
3,15,
Marisa Barrigón
16,
Alfonso Gutiérrez-Zotes
3,17,
Eva Grasa
3,15,
Isabel Ruiz-Delgado
18,
Esther Lorente-Rovira
3,19,
Jordi Cid
20,
on behalf of the Spanish Metacognition Study Group
and
Susana Ochoa
1,3,*
1
Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Sant Boi de Llobregat, 08830 Barcelona, Spain
2
Department of Social and Quantitative Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain
3
Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, 28029 Madrid, Spain
4
Serra Húnter Fellow, Department of Statistics and Operations Research (DEIO), Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, 08028 Barcelona, Spain
5
Institute of Mathematics of UPC—BarcelonaTech (IMTech), 08028 Barcelona, Spain
6
Sant Joan de Déu Research Foundation, 08950 Esplugues de Llobregat, Spain
7
COMSAL Research Group, Psychology Department, FPCEE Blanquerna, Ramon Llull University, 08022 Barcelona, Spain
8
Departament de Psicologia Clínica i de la Salut, Facultat de Psicologia, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Spain
9
Health Services Research Group, Instituto Hospital del Mar de Investigaciones Médicas, 08003 Barcelona, Spain
10
Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Epidemiología y Salud Pública, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, 28029 Madrid, Spain
11
Departamento de Medicina y Ciencias de la Vida, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 08003 Barcelona, Spain
12
Grupo de Estudios de Invariancia de la Medida y Análisis del Cambio (GEIMAC), Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain
13
Serra Húnter Fellow Departament de Psicologia Clínica i de la Salut, Facultat de Psicologia, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Spain
14
Department of Research, Centre d’Higiene Mental Les Corts, 08029 Barcelona, Spain
15
Department of Psychiatry, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Institut d’Investigació Biomèdica-Sant Pau (IIB-Sant Pau), 08025, Barcelona, Spain
16
Departament of Psychiatry, University Hospital Virgen del Rocio, 41013 Seville, Spain
17
Hospital Universitari Institut Pere Mata, Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili, CERCA, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Reus, Spain, 43206 Reus, Spain
18
Unidad de Salud Mental Comunitaria Malaga Norte, 29014 Málaga, Spain
19
Psychiatry Service, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain
20
Mental Health & Addiction Research Group, IdiBGi, Institut d’Assistencia Sanitària, 17190 Girona, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Collaborators of the Spanish Metacognition Study Group (SMSG) are indicated in the Acknowledgment section.
Healthcare 2022, 10(11), 2155; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10112155
Submission received: 9 September 2022 / Revised: 21 October 2022 / Accepted: 22 October 2022 / Published: 28 October 2022

Abstract

:
Metacognitive training (MCT) is an effective treatment for psychosis. Longitudinal trajectories of treatment response are unknown but could point to strategies to maximize treatment efficacy during the first episodes. This work aims to explore the possible benefit of using latent class mixed models (LCMMs) to understand how treatment response differs between metacognitive training and psychoeducation. We conducted LCMMs in 28 patients that received MCT and 34 patients that received psychoeducation. We found that MCT is effective in improving cognitive insight in all patients but that these effects wane at follow-up. In contrast, psychoeducation does not improve cognitive insight, and may increase self-certainty in a group of patients. These results suggest that LCMMs are valuable tools that can aid in treatment prescription and in predicting response to specific treatments.

1. Introduction

People that experience a first episode of psychosis (FEP) have highly variable outcomes, which range from sustained remission to treatment resistance from onset [1]. Despite efforts in identifying early treatment strategies [2], it is still a clinical challenge to deliver optimized treatment to prevent relapse and functional decline [3].
Cognitive behavioral models of psychosis have fostered the development of psychological interventions that target the cognitive biases involved in the genesis and maintenance of psychosis [4]. These have been studied as promising treatments for psychosis because in the past decades, despite considerable advances in pharmacological treatment, the outcomes of psychosis have not improved significantly [5].
Metacognitive training (MCT) [6] has emerged as one of the most effective psychological treatments for positive symptoms of psychosis [7]. MCT combines psychoeducation, cognitive bias modification, and strategy teaching to correct data-gathering biases. This approach aims to sow the seeds of doubt [8,9] by considering cognitive biases as a deviation from normality. Systematic-review findings have supported its efficacy in improving positive and negative symptoms, self-esteem, and functioning in schizophrenia [7,10], and recent studies have proven that it is also a valid intervention for people with FEP [11].
These broad beneficial effects may be rooted in that MCT intervenes over most of the cognitive biases and cognitive constructs that have established evidence of their importance in the genesis of psychosis, its maintenance, and outcomes. MCT includes sessions that work on domains of social cognition such as facial emotion recognition and theory of mind, both of which are strong predictors of outcome [12]. Likewise, it targets the jumping-to-conclusions bias (JTC), which has been repeatedly associated with delusions, poorer neurocognition, and measures of outcome [13,14,15,16,17].
A consistent finding in the literature is that MCT improves cognitive insight by increasing self-reflectivity and reducing self-certainty [18], the two domains that compose cognitive insight. Cognitive insight refers to the set of cognitive processes that permit questioning one’s beliefs and appraisals and reevaluating anomalous experiences or misinterpretations [19]. Self-reflectivity refers to a person’s ability for introspection and willingness to admit fallibility. Conversely, self-certainty refers to the confidence a person has in their beliefs and judgments [19]. It is suggested that the formula for good cognitive insight is high self-reflectivity and low self-certainty [20]. This is because self-reflectivity has usually been associated with better outcomes and treatment response [21], while self-certainty is associated with more delusions and worse cognitive function [22,23].
Although meta-analytic findings have supported the efficacy of MCT in psychosis [7,24], this is usually at a medium effect size [24]. However, this seems to be the case for most effective psychological interventions for psychosis [25]. One reason for it may be that most clinical trials report averaged results, which blurs the vast heterogeneity in psychosis [26] and does not permit detecting the patients that indeed benefit from an intervention and their clinical characteristics. Furthermore, some of the MCT effects are apparent after a sleeper effect [27], but other patients need extended treatment to consolidate the effects [28]. Understanding the heterogeneity in response to MCT could point to better treatment strategies addressed to the specific characteristics of each patient. In particular, this is a vexing issue in first-episode psychosis because early targeted treatment may help promote recovery and prevent relapse [29].
Unsupervised learning methods such as latent class mixed models (LCMMs, also known as growth mixture models) are a useful approach for studying longitudinal trajectories of latent variables. LCMMs have the advantage of being able to capture inter-individual differences in intra-individual change over time while preserving the heterogeneity of the population [30]. Another characteristic of the LCMM method is that it is a data-driven method, which allows studying longitudinal changes of a variable as it occurs naturally.
Previous studies have used LCMMs to understand trajectories of illness or outcome [31,32], but [31,32] this method has not been used to explore trajectories of cognitive biases in response to different psychological interventions.
We sought to study the feasibility of using LCMMs to understand patterns of change in cognitive insight in individuals with FEP who have received MCT. To test whether this is a specific effect of MCT, we compared latent trajectories between patients that received MCT and a group of patients who received psychoeducation. Psychoeducation is a psychological intervention that has proven to be effective in promoting better social and functional outcomes [33], but according to the literature, psychoeducation does not seem to improve cognitive insight [34,35]. Thus, the psychoeducation group will help compare the effects of MCT as opposed to how cognitive insight changes naturally.

2. Materials and Methods

This study aims to provide proof-of-concept evidence of using LCMMs to understand patterns of change in response to psychotherapy. We conducted a secondary analysis of data from a blind, multicentric clinical trial that has been published elsewhere [11]. Briefly, the original study recruited 126 patients with FEP from nine participating mental health centers: Servicio Andaluz de Salud of Jaén, Málaga and Motril (Granada), Salut Mental Parc Taulí (Sabadell), Hospital de Santa Creu i Sant Pau (Barcelona), Centro de Higiene Mental Les Corts (Barcelona), Institut d’Assistència Sanitària Girona, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valencia, and Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu (coordinating center). Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of schizophrenia, psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, delusional disorder, schizoaffective disorder, brief psychotic disorder, or schizophreniform disorder (according to DSM-IV-TR); (2) <5 years from the onset of symptoms; (3) a score of ≥3 in item delusions, grandiosity, or suspicions of PANSS in the previous year. Exclusion criteria were: traumatic brain injury, dementia, or intellectual disability (premorbid IQ ≤ 70); (2) substance dependence; and (3) PANSS ≥5 in hostile and uncooperativeness, or ≥6 in suspiciousness. This was chosen to avoid altering the dynamics of the group interventions. Participants were randomized to receive either MCT or psychoeducation. Psychoeducation included modules on healthy habits, risk behaviors, prevention of relapse, video forums, resources for work, leisure activities, and community resources. The original study randomized participants using blocks of four from a list of random numbers, and 55 patients were allocated to psychoeducation while 67 received MCT. The remaining participants were either excluded or declined to participate in the study. All participants were assessed at three points: baseline, post-treatment, and six-month follow-up.
The assessment included the following:
Sociodemographic questionnaire: Data on sociodemographic variables were collected on-site. Diagnosis and treatment were collected from the clinical history of the participants. We transformed the antipsychotic treatment to olanzapine defined daily dose (DDD) [36].
Clinical measures: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [37,38] was used to measure symptom severity. The Spanish version of the Scale Unawareness of Mental Disorders (SUMD) [39,40] was used to measure unawareness of the mental disorder. Higher scores represent more unawareness of the mental disorder. We used the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale [41], where higher scores indicate better self-esteem.
Metacognition: The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) [20,42] was used to measure cognitive insight. The BCIS is composed of two subscales: self-certainty and self-reflectivity, which are analyzed separately. Higher scores in self-reflectivity represent more ability to question one’s beliefs. Higher scores in self-certainty represent more certainty in one’s interpretations and misinterpretations. The beads task [43] was used to measure the JTC. Participants were shown a picture of two containers filled with 100 colored beads in reciprocal proportions. We used three trials with different conditions: a probabilistic trial with an 85/15 ratio, a second probabilistic trial with a 60/40 ratio, and a final trial with an affective condition in a 60/40 ratio. Participants were told that the computer had selected a container and that the goal of the task was to determine which container. To this aim, participants were shown one bead at a time. The participant was instructed to see as many beads as they needed to guess what container the beads came from. Our outcome variable was the draws to decision in the three probabilistic conditions. Less than 3 draws to decision is considered indicative of presenting the JTC bias.
Social cognition: The Internal, Personal and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ) [44] was used to assess attributional style. We used two indexes: personalizing bias and externalizing bias. Personalizing bias refers to a tendency to blame others rather than circumstances for negative events. Externalizing bias refers to a tendency to attribute the causes of negative events to others or circumstances rather than to oneself [45]. The faces test [46,47] was used to measure emotion recognition. A reduced version of the hinting task [48,49] was used to measure theory of mind.
Global functioning: The Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) [50] was used to measure global functioning on a scale of 0–100. Higher scores represent better functioning.
The Ethics Committee of each participating center approved this project.
LCMMs were conducted using the R package lcmm [51] from the statistical software R version 4.0.2 [52]. This unsupervised learning technique classifies individuals into groupings with similar trajectory patterns, called latent classes. Following the strategy of Nagin et al. [53], we fit the respective models for the outcomes of interest (self-reflectivity and self-certainty) at the three points of assessment, for which the time metric was the time at the assessment (baseline, post-treatment, and follow-up). The number of group trajectories was determined by analyzing 2–6 group models without covariates. Model selection to determine the optimal number of latent trajectories was performed according to the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), where a lower value indicates a better fit [54]. Average posterior probabilities above 70% were checked as well [55]. Individuals were allocated to classes according to maximum a posteriori criterium (MAP). To ensure adequate results, in this work we only included patients with complete data on self-reflectivity and self-certainty at the three points of assessment.
At baseline, the MCT and the psychoeducation groups were compared with chi-square tests for categorical variables and Student t-tests for continuous variables.
To compare trajectories, we used Mann–Whitney U tests, for which the effect size was reported with ranked biserial correlations.

3. Results

The final sample included 62 participants with complete data on self-reflectivity and self-certainty at the three points of assessment.
At baseline, the two groups only differed in that the MCT group had higher baseline global functioning than the psychoeducation group (t(60) = −2.857, p = 0.006). Table 1 presents baseline data of the two groups.

3.1. Cognitive Insight Trajectories in the MCT Group

We found two trajectories for self-reflectivity and two trajectories for self-certainty. The four trajectories are summarized in Figure 1 and Figure 2. Differences between each pair of trajectories at baseline, post-test, and follow-up can be found in Supplementary Table S1.

3.1.1. Self-Reflectivity

We found a trajectory (in red), henceforth “High SR” (n = 20), that grouped patients with high baseline self-reflectivity. The “High SR” trajectory included patients whose self-reflectivity was stable during the baseline and post-treatment assessments but declined at follow-up. The second trajectory (in blue), named “Improving SR” (n = 8), grouped patients with low baseline self-reflectivity. The “Improving SR” trajectory presented an improvement in self-reflectivity at follow-up, which declined to a level below baseline at the 6-month follow-up. Figure 1 shows the graphical representations of the two trajectories of self-reflectivity in the MCT group.
At baseline, the two self-reflectivity trajectories differed in negative (U = 20.00, p = 0.002) and general (U = 22.00, p = 0.003) symptoms, global functioning (U = 23.00, p = 0.004), and self-esteem (U = 29.50, p = 0.011). Patients in the “High SR” trajectory presented better scores in all the variables, indicating a better clinical state at the moment of the assessment.
At post-test, the two trajectories still differed in negative symptoms (U = 12.00, p = 0.004), general symptoms (U = 21.50, p = 0.026), global functioning (U = 20.50, p = 0.021), and self-esteem (U = 27.00, p = 0.05). However, at this point of assessment, the “Improving SR” trajectory had significantly worse clinical insight (U = 24.00, p = 0.040).
Finally, at the 6-month follow-up, the two trajectories only differed in negative (U = 22.00, p = 0.03) and general symptoms (U = 10.00, p = 0.010) and clinical insight (U = 20.00, p = 0.020).

3.1.2. Self-Certainty

For self-certainty, we also found two trajectories in the MCT group. The “Initial Decline” (in red, n = 22) trajectory grouped patients with moderate levels of self-certainty at baseline. At post-test, the scores in self-certainty declined, but they increased again to baseline levels at the 6-month follow-up.
The second trajectory (in blue, n = 6), henceforth “Responsive SC”, comprised patients with very high self-certainty at baseline. The scores in self-certainty presented a steep decline at post-test but increased again at follow-up. However, at follow-up, the scores in self-reflectivity did not reach baseline levels.
The two trajectories of self-certainty in the MCT group are depicted in Figure 2.
At baseline, the two trajectories of self-certainty did not differ in any variable. However, at post-test, the “Responsive SC” trajectory had worse positive symptoms (U = 18.50, p = 0.015). The difference in positive symptoms was maintained at follow-up (U = 20.50, p = 0.025), but patients in the “Initial Decline” trajectory also displayed worse general symptoms (U = 23.50, p = 0.05) at this point of assessment.

3.2. Cognitive Insight Trajectories in the Psychoeducation Group

3.2.1. Self-Reflectivity

In the psychoeducation group, we also found two trajectories for self-reflectivity. These are depicted in Figure 3.
Concerning self-reflectivity, the first trajectory (in red), henceforth “Low-SR” (n = 18), included patients that had a low baseline level of self-reflectivity. In this trajectory, self-reflectivity consistently declined over time. The second trajectory (in blue), henceforth “Worsening-SR” (n = 16), comprised patients exhibiting self-reflectivity that was high at baseline but declined steeply over time.
At baseline, the two trajectories of self-reflectivity differed in diagnosis (χ2 = 9.86, p = 0.04). Participants in the “Low-SR” trajectory had a bigger proportion of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia as opposed to other diagnoses in the spectrum of psychosis.
At post-test, participants in the “Low-SR” trajectory reported more personalizing bias (U = 47.00, p = 0.05). However, this difference was not maintained at follow-up. At the follow-up point of assessment, the two trajectories of self-reflectivity in the psychoeducation group did not differ in any variable.

3.2.2. Self-Certainty

Two trajectories of self-certainty were apparent in the psychoeducation group. The first trajectory (in red), named “Low and Stable SC” (n = 23), included patients with low baseline self-certainty, which remained stable throughout the three assessments. The second trajectory (in blue), named “Worsening SC” (n = 11), included patients with high baseline self-certainty, which increased significantly after intervention and then decreased to baseline levels at follow-up. The graphical representation of these trajectories can be found in Figure 4.
At baseline, the two trajectories of self-certainty differed in academic background (χ2 = 11.98, p = 0.035), positive symptoms (U = 70.50, p = 0.040), clinical insight (U = 31.50, p < 0.001), the 60–40 condition of the beads task (U= 57.50, p = 0.011), and facial emotion recognition (U = 64.00, p = 0.019).
At post-test, the Worsening SC trajectory presented more positive (U = 27.50, p = 0.014) and negative (U = 34.50, p = 0.029) symptoms, worse global functioning (U = 37.50, p = 0.042), worse clinical insight (U = 33.00, p = 0.003), and less draws to decision in the three conditions of the beads task (U = 34.00, p = 0.023; U = 32.50, p = 0.021; U = 38.50, p = 0.043).
However, at follow up, the two trajectories only differed in positive (U = 25.00, p = 0.009) and negative symptoms (U = 30.00, p = 0.020), clinical insight (U = 30.50, p = 0.005), and theory of mind (U = 38.50, p = 0.05).

4. Discussion

As predicted by previous literature [18], we found that patients that received MCT improved cognitive insight by maintaining good baseline levels of self-reflectivity or improving low baseline levels and reducing self-certainty. This was an expected result since the original study [11] found solid evidence of the improvement in cognitive insight in patients who received MCT compared to those who received psychoeducation. By using LCMMs, we could obtain a deeper view of how patients respond to each intervention.
In absence of a specific intervention on cognitive insight, it seems that self-reflectivity tends to decrease steadily independent of its baseline level, which was evidenced by the two trajectories in the psychoeducation group. This finding is consistent with a previous study comparing psychoeducation and metacognitive training that found that psychoeducation is not an effective treatment to improve self-reflectivity [35].
Our results showed that metacognitive training is useful in maintaining high baseline self-reflectivity or improving low baseline self-reflectivity. However, trajectory analysis suggests that self-reflectivity tends to decrease after the intervention. In this sense, LCMMs allowed us to detect that MCT may be beneficial in improving cognitive insight for all patients, regardless of their baseline levels. Furthermore, we found that both self-reflectivity and self-certainty experienced a steep decline at follow-up, which suggests the need for maintained booster sessions of MCT to stabilize the effects. These results are consistent with recent evidence showing that extended therapy could strengthen its positive effects [9,28].
A previous study has suggested sustained and sleeper effects of MCT in people with psychosis [27]. In this work, the authors found that improvements in delusions that were not significant at the post-test became significant at a three-year follow-up. Because our follow-up was at six months, we were not able to detect whether any trajectory experienced more sustained effects, or whether the trajectories may change over time as the sleeper effect becomes apparent.
There was a surprising finding. Psychoeducation seemed to be detrimental to patients with high self-certainty at baseline. At baseline, participants in this trajectory experienced more positive symptoms, less clinical insight, a bigger tendency to make hasty decisions (jumping to conclusions), and worse facial emotion recognition than their counterparts. These differences were not apparent in the self-certainty trajectories of the MCT group. Moreover, at post-test, patients in the Worsening SC group presented fewer draws to decision in the three conditions of the beads task, which suggests a more pronounced tendency to present the jumping-to-conclusions bias.
Furthermore, both self-certainty and the jumping-to-conclusions bias are strongly related to delusions [14,23], and clinical insight and facial emotion recognition are strong predictors of outcome and functioning in patients with psychosis [12,56]. Because this trajectory had worse scores in these domains, we speculate that it represents a group with a higher risk of relapse, although this should be tested in future studies.
Although the scope of this study precludes us from drawing conclusions on the possible mechanisms of worsening self-certainty in response to psychoeducation, our interpretation is that there may be an interaction between unawareness of the disease, positive symptoms, and poor facial emotion recognition. These patients also had poor clinical insight and thus may interpret information on the disease as threatening and react by jumping to the conclusion that their thoughts and experiences are certain. These results suggest that patients that have social cognitive and metacognitive difficulties may benefit from starting psychological treatment with a normalizing approach that reduces data-gathering biases and improves insight, such as MCT.
The findings of this study must be interpreted considering several limitations: First, the sample size in the two groups was small, rendering the study underpowered and limiting our ability to compare longitudinal outcomes. Similarly, the sample size of each trajectory precluded us from obtaining predictors for each trajectory. Finally, our follow-up data only extended to six months after the intervention, and we could not detect sleeper effects.
These limitations notwithstanding, our findings support the use of LCMMs to study cognitive biases in response to psychotherapy and highlight the heterogeneous nature of psychosis. Future studies including broader samples comparing more interventions will help detect the chances of responding to a specific intervention and detect precision treatment strategies to prescribe psychological treatment based on the individual characteristics of each person.
This study was not designed to identify predictors of outcomes after MCT. Rather, the present study aimed to offer proof-of-concept evidence of the added value of using LCMMs to improve our current knowledge of psychological interventions for psychosis. However, using LCMMs with larger samples may help detect what specific trajectories have better chances of responding to an intervention and what their predictors are and may identify potential moderators.
Furthermore, this approach is not only valid for people with psychosis, as most current psychological interventions for mental illness have sound theoretical foundations and strong evidence supporting their efficacy.

5. Conclusions

This work supports the added value of using LCMMs to understand how specific psychological interventions exert differential effects on cognitive biases while considering the variability of the patients’ responses. Specifically, we found that MCT improves cognitive insight in all participants with first-episode psychosis regardless of their baseline level. Conversely, psychoeducation does not affect cognitive insight and may trigger adverse effects in some patients. Finally, LCMMs could be a useful approach to detect predictors of response to different psychological treatments and to develop early targeted treatment for people with psychosis.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/healthcare10112155/s1, Table S1: Baseline, post-test, and follow-up characteristics of each trajectory in the MCT group; Table S2: Baseline, post-test, and follow-up characteristics of each trajectory in the psychoeducation group.

Author Contributions

M.F.-Q., S.O. were involved in the conception and drafting of the manuscript. D.F. conducted statistical analysis. R.L.-C., I.B., E.L.-R., E.G., T.P., M.B., H.G.-M., J.G.-B., A.B., E.P., A.G.-Z. and I.R.-D. collected data and revised the manuscript. L.D.-C., M.V.-R., E.H.-R. and J.C. reviewed and revised the manuscript. Members of the Spanish Metacognition Study Group provided resources and referred participants to the study. S.O. obtained funding for the project. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Government, (PI11/01347, PI14/00044 and PI18/00212); the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER), Health Department of Catalonia, PERIS call (SLT006/17/00231); Progress and Health Foundation of the Andalusian Regional Ministry of Health (PI-0634/2011 and PI-0193/2014); Obra Social La Caixa (RecerCaixa call 2013), Obra Social Sant Joan de Déu, BML (RTI2018-100927-J-I00) administrated by Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (MCI, Spain), by the Agencia Estatal de Investigación (AEI, Spain), and by the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER, UE); Daniel Fernández has been supported by grant 2017 SGR 622 (GRBIO) administrated by the Departament d’Economia i Coneixement de la Generalitat de Catalunya (Spain) and by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (Spain) [PID2019-104830RB-I00/ DOI (AEI): 10.13039/501100011033]; and CIBER, Consorcio Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the clinicians that referred patients to this study. Likewise, we thank all the participants for their time, effort, and altruism. We thank all the members of the Spanish Metacognition Study Group for their valuable insights and comments. # Spanish Metacognition Study Group (SMSG): Acevedo A., Anglès J., Argany M.A., Barajas A., Barrigón M.L., Beltrán M., Birulés I., Bogas J.L., Cabezas A., Camprubí N., Carbonero M., Carrasco E., Casañas R., Cid J., Conesa E., Corripio I., Cortes P., Crosas J.M., de Apraiz A., Delgado M., Domínguez L., Escartí M.J., Escudero A., Esteban Pinos I., Franco C., García C., Gil V., Gonzalez-Casares R., González Higueras F., González-Montoro Mª.L., González E., Grasa E., Guasp A., Gutierrez-Zotes A., Huerta-Ramos Mª.E., Huertas P., Jiménez-Díaz A., Lalucat L.L., LLacer B., López-Carrilero R., Lorente E., Luengo A., Mantecón N., Mas-Expósito L., Montes M., Moritz S., Murgui E., Nuñez M., Ochoa S., Palomer E., Paniego E., Peláez T., Pérez V., Planell K., Planellas C., Pleguezuelo-Garrote P., Pousa E., Rabella M., Renovell M., Rubio R., Ruiz-Delgado I., San Emeterio M., Sánchez E., Sanjuán J., Sans B., Schilling L., Sió H., Teixidó M., Torres P., Vila M.A., Vila-Badia R., Villegas F., Villellas R.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Hall, M.-H.; Holton, K.M.; Öngür, D.; Montrose, D.; Keshavan, M.S. Longitudinal Trajectory of Early Functional Recovery in Patients with First Episode Psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2019, 209, 234–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Liemburg, E.J.; Castelein, S.; van Es, F.; Scholte-Stalenhoef, A.N.; van de Willige, G.; Smid, H.; Visser, E.; Knegtering, H.; Bruggeman, R. The Psychosis Recent Onset GRoningen Survey (PROGR-S): Defining Dimensions and Improving Outcomes in Early Psychosis. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e113521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Suvisaari, J.; Mantere, O.; Keinänen, J.; Mäntylä, T.; Rikandi, E.; Lindgren, M.; Kieseppä, T.; Raij, T.T. Is It Possible to Predict the Future in First-Episode Psychosis? Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Gawęda, Ł.; Moritz, S.; Ochoa, S.; So, S.H. Editorial: The Relationship Between Cognitive Biases and Psychosis: Searching for Mechanisms. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 753317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Saha, S.; Chant, D.; Welham, J.; McGrath, J. A Systematic Review of the Prevalence of Schizophrenia. PLoS Med. 2005, 2, e141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Moritz, S.; Woodward, T.S. Metacognitive Training in Schizophrenia: From Basic Research to Knowledge Translation and Intervention. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2007, 20, 619–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Penney, D.; Sauvé, G.; Mendelson, D.; Thibaudeau, É.; Moritz, S.; Lepage, M.; Mental, D. Immediate and Sustained Outcomes and Moderators Associated With Metacognitive Training for Psychosis: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA 2022, 79, 417–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Moritz, S.; Andreou, C.; Schneider, B.C.; Wittekind, C.E.; Menon, M.; Balzan, R.P.; Woodward, T.S. Sowing the Seeds of Doubt: A Narrative Review on Metacognitive Training in Schizophrenia. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2014, 34, 358–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Moritz, S.; Klein, J.P.; Lysaker, P.H.; Mehl, S. Metacognitive and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions for Psychosis: New developments. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 21, 309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Jiang, J.; Zhang, L.; Zhu, Z.; Li, W.; Li, C. Metacognitive Training for Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2015, 27, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ochoa, S.; López-Carrilero, R.; Barrigón, M.L.; Pousa, E.; Barajas, A.; Lorente-Rovira, E.; González-Higueras, F.; Grasa, E.; Ruiz-Delgado, I.; Cid, J.; et al. Randomized Control Trial to Assess the Efficacy of Metacognitive Training Compared with a Psycho-Educational Group in People with a Recent-Onset Psychosis. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 1573–1584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Fett, A.K.J.; Viechtbauer, W.; Dominguez, M.d.G.; Penn, D.L.; van Os, J.; Krabbendam, L. The Relationship between Neurocognition and Social Cognition with Functional Outcomes in Schizophrenia: A Meta-Analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 573–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Falcone, M.A.; Murray, R.M.; O’Connor, J.A.; Hockey, L.N.; Gardner-Sood, P.; Di Forti, M.; Freeman, D.; Jolley, S. Jumping to Conclusions and the Persistence of Delusional Beliefs in First Episode Psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2015, 165, 243–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Falcone, M.A.; Murray, R.M.; Wiffen, B.D.R.; O’Connor, J.A.; Russo, M.; Kolliakou, A.; Stilo, S.; Taylor, H.; Gardner-Sood, P.; Paparelli, A.; et al. Jumping to Conclusions, Neuropsychological Functioning, and Delusional Beliefs in First Episode Psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2015, 41, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Tripoli, G.; Quattrone, D.; Ferraro, L.; Gayer-Anderson, C.; Rodriguez, V.; La Cascia, C.; La Barbera, D.; Sartorio, C.; Seminerio, F.; Tarricone, I.; et al. Jumping to Conclusions, General Intelligence, and Psychosis Liability: Findings from the Multi-Centre EU-GEI Case-Control Study. Psychol. Med. 2021, 51, 623–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Díaz-Cutraro, L.; López-Carrilero, R.; García-Mieres, H.; Ferrer-Quintero, M.; Verdaguer-Rodriguez, M.; Barajas, A.; Grasa, E.; Pousa, E.; Lorente, E.; Barrigón, M.L.; et al. The Relationship between Jumping to Conclusions and Social Cognition in First-Episode Psychosis. Schizophrenia 2022, 8, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Díaz-Cutraro, L.; García-Mieres, H.; López-Carrilero, R.; Ferrer, M.; Verdaguer-Rodriguez, M.; Barrigón, M.L.; Barajas, A.; Grasa, E.; Pousa, E.; Lorente, E.; et al. Jumping to Conclusions Is Differently Associated with Specific Subtypes of Delusional Experiences: An Exploratory Study in First-Episode Psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2021, 228, 357–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lopez-Morinigo, J.-D.; Ajnakina, O.; Martínez, A.S.-E.; Escobedo-Aedo, P.-J.; Ruiz-Ruano, V.G.; Sánchez-Alonso, S.; Mata-Iturralde, L.; Muñoz-Lorenzo, L.; Ochoa, S.; Baca-García, E.; et al. Can Metacognitive Interventions Improve Insight in Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Med. 2020, 50, 2289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lysaker, P.H.; Pattison, M.L.; Leonhardt, B.L.; Phelps, S.; Vohs, J.L. Insight in Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders: Relationship with Behavior, Mood and Perceived Quality of Life, Underlying Causes and Emerging Treatments. World Psychiatry 2018, 17, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Beck, A.T.; Baruch, E.; Balter, J.M.; Steer, R.A.; Warman, D.M. A New Instrument for Measuring Insight: The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 319–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. O’Connor, J.A.; Ellett, L.; Ajnakina, O.; Schoeler, T.; Kollliakou, A.; Trotta, A.; Wiffen, B.D.; Falcone, A.M.; Di Forti, M.; Murray, R.M.; et al. Can Cognitive Insight Predict Symptom Remission in a First Episode Psychosis Cohort? BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Cooke, M.A.; Peters, E.R.; Fannon, D.; Aasen, I.; Kuipers, E.; Kumari, V. Cognitive Insight in Psychosis: The Relationship between Self-Certainty and Self-Reflection Dimensions and Neuropsychological Measures. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 178, 284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Engh, J.A.; Friis, S.; Birkenaes, A.B.; Jónsdóttir, H.; Klungsøyr, O.; Ringen, P.A.; Simonsen, C.; Vaskinn, A.; Opjordsmoen, S.; Andreassen, O.A. Delusions Are Associated with Poor Cognitive Insight in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2010, 36, 830–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Liu, Y.C.; Tang, C.C.; Hung, T.T.; Tsai, P.C.; Lin, M.F. The Efficacy of Metacognitive Training for Delusions in Patients With Schizophrenia: A Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials Informs Evidence-Based Practice. Worldviews Evidence-Based Nurs. 2018, 15, 130–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Sauvé, G.; Lavigne, K.M.; Pochiet, G.; Brodeur, M.B.; Lepage, M. Efficacy of Psychological Interventions Targeting Cognitive Biases in Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 78, 101854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Moritz, S.; Mahlke, C.I.; Westermann, S.; Ruppelt, F.; Lysaker, P.H.; Bock, T.; Andreou, C. Embracing Psychosis: A Cognitive Insight Intervention Improves Personal Narratives and Meaning-Making in Patients With Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2018, 44, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Moritz, S.; Veckenstedt, R.; Andreou, C.; Bohn, F.; Hottenrott, B.; Leighton, L.; Köther, U.; Woodward, T.S.; Treszl, A.; Menon, M.; et al. Sustained and “Sleeper” Effects of Group Metacognitive Training for Schizophrenia: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2014, 71, 1103–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ishikawa, R.; Ishigaki, T.; Shimada, T.; Tanoue, H.; Yoshinaga, N.; Oribe, N.; Morimoto, T.; Matsumoto, T.; Hosono, M. The Efficacy of Extended Metacognitive Training for Psychosis: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Schizophr. Res. 2020, 215, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fusar-Poli, P.; McGorry, P.D.; Kane, J.M. Improving Outcomes of First-Episode Psychosis: An Overview. World Psychiatry 2017, 16, 251–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Jung, T.; Wickrama, K.A.S. An Introduction to Latent Class Growth Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 302–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Chang, W.C.; Ho, R.W.H.; Tang, J.Y.M.; Wong, C.S.M.; Hui, C.L.M.; Chan, S.K.W.; Lee, E.M.H.; Suen, Y.N.; Chen, E.Y.H. Early-Stage Negative Symptom Trajectories and Relationships With 13-Year Outcomes in First-Episode Nonaffective Psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2019, 45, 610–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Salagre, E.; Grande, I.; Solé, B.; Mezquida, G.; Cuesta, M.J.; Díaz-Caneja, C.M.; Amoretti, S.; Lobo, A.; González-Pinto, A.; Moreno, C.; et al. Exploring Risk and Resilient Profiles for Functional Impairment and Baseline Predictors in a 2-Year Follow-Up First-Episode Psychosis Cohort Using Latent Class Growth Analysis. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 10, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Xia, J.; Merinder, L.B.; Belgamwar, M.R. Psychoeducation for Schizophrenia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2011, 6, 21–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Dondé, C.; Senn, M.; Eche, J.; Kallel, L.; Saoud, M.; Brunelin, J. Well-Informed but Not Aware: The P.A.C.T.® Psychoeducation Program for Schizophrenia Improves Knowledge about, but Not Insight into, the Illness. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2019, 46, 15–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ahuir, M.; Cabezas, Á.; Miñano, M.J.; Algora, M.J.; Estrada, F.; Solé, M.; Gutiérrez-Zotes, A.; Tost, M.; Barbero, J.D.; Montalvo, I.; et al. Improvement in Cognitive Biases after Group Psychoeducation and Metacognitive Training in Recent-Onset Psychosis: A Randomized Crossover Clinical Trial. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 270, 720–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Leucht, S.; Samara, M.; Heres, S.; Davis, J.M. Dose Equivalents for Antipsychotic Drugs: The DDD Method. Schizophr. Bull. 2016, 42, 90–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Kay, S.R.; Fiszbein, A.; Opler, L.; Kay, S.R.; Fiszbein, A.; Opler, L.A.; Kay, S.R.; Fiszbein, A.; Opler, L. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987, 13, 261–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Peralta, V.; Cuesta, M.J. Psychometric Properties of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) in Schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1994, 53, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Amador, X.F.; Strauss, D.H.; Yale, S.A.; Flaum, M.M.; Endicott, J.; Gorman, J.M. Assessment of Insight in Psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 1993, 150, 873–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Ruiz Ripoll, A.I.; Pousa, E.; Duñó, R.; Crosas, J.M.; Cuppa, S.; García-Ribera, C. Adaptación Al Español de La Escala de Valoración de La No Conciencia de Trastorno Mental (SUMD). Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2008, 36, 111–119. [Google Scholar]
  41. Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  42. Gutiérrez-Zotes, J.A.; Valero, J.; Cortés, M.J.; Labad, A.; Ochoa, S.; Ahuir, M.; Carlson, J.; Bernardo, M.; Cañizares, S.; Escartin, G.; et al. Spanish Adaptation of the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) for Schizophrenia. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2012, 40, 2–9. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  43. Brett-Jones, J.; Garety, P.; Hemsley, D. Measuring Delusional Experiences: A Method and Its Application. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1987, 26, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kinderman, P.; Bentall, R.P. The Development of a Novel Measure of Causal Attributions: The Internal Personal and Situational Attributions Questionnaire. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1996, 20, 261–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Langdon, R.; Corner, T.; McLaren, J.; Ward, P.B.; Coltheart, M. Externalizing and Personalizing Biases in Persecutory Delusions: The Relationship with Poor Insight and Theory-of-Mind. Behav. Res. Ther. 2006, 44, 699–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Baron-Cohen, S.; Wheelwright, S.; Jolliffe, T. Is There a “Language of the Eyes”? Evidence from Normal Adults, and Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome. Vis. Cogn. 1997, 4, 311–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Huerta-Ramos, E.; Ferrer-Quintero, M.; Gómez-Benito, J.; González-Higueras, F.; Cuadras, D.; del Rey-Mejías, Á.L.; Usall, J.; Ochoa, S. Translation and Validation of Baron Cohen’s Face Test in a General Population from Spain. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2020; in press. [Google Scholar]
  48. Corcoran, R.; Mercer, G.; Frith, C.D. Schizophrenia, Symptomatology and Social Inference: Investigating “Theory of Mind” in People with Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 1995, 17, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Gil, D.; Fernández-Modamio, M.; Bengochea, R.; Arrieta, M. Adaptation of the Hinting Task Theory of the Mind Test to Spanish. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. (Engl. Ed.) 2012, 5, 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Endicott, J. The Global Assessment Scale: A Procedure for Measuring Overall Severity of Psychiatric Disturbance. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1976, 33, 766–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Proust-Lima, C.; Philipps, V.; Liquet, B. Estimation of Extended Mixed Models Using Latent Classes and Latent Processes: The R Package Lcmm. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 78, 1–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  53. Nagin, D.S.; Tremblay, R.E.; Sampson, R.J.; Laub, J.H. Developmental Trajectory Groups: Fact or a Useful Statistical Fiction? Criminology 2005, 43, 873–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Tein, J.Y.; Coxe, S.; Cham, H. Statistical Power to Detect the Correct Number of Classes in Latent Profile Analysis. Struct. Equ. Model. 2013, 20, 640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Therneau, T.M.; Atkinson, E.J. An Introduction to Recursive Partitioning Using the RPART Routines. 2019. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rpart/vignettes/longintro.pdf (accessed on 10 October 2022).
  56. Wright, A.C.; Lysaker, P.H.; Fowler, D.; Greenwood, K. Clinical Insight in First Episode Psychosis: The Role of Metacognition. J. Ment. Health 2021, 17, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Trajectories of self-reflectivity in the MCT group. The red trajectory depicts the “High SR” trajectory (n = 20). The second trajectory (in blue), refers to “Improving SR” (n = 8).
Figure 1. Trajectories of self-reflectivity in the MCT group. The red trajectory depicts the “High SR” trajectory (n = 20). The second trajectory (in blue), refers to “Improving SR” (n = 8).
Healthcare 10 02155 g001
Figure 2. Trajectories of self-certainty in the MCT group: “Initial Decline” (in red, n = 22) and “Responsive SC” (in blue, n = 6).
Figure 2. Trajectories of self-certainty in the MCT group: “Initial Decline” (in red, n = 22) and “Responsive SC” (in blue, n = 6).
Healthcare 10 02155 g002
Figure 3. Trajectories of self-reflectivity in the psychoeducation group. The red line refers to the “Low-SR” trajectory (n = 18). The second trajectory (in blue), refers to the “Worsening-SR” (n = 16) trajectory.
Figure 3. Trajectories of self-reflectivity in the psychoeducation group. The red line refers to the “Low-SR” trajectory (n = 18). The second trajectory (in blue), refers to the “Worsening-SR” (n = 16) trajectory.
Healthcare 10 02155 g003
Figure 4. Trajectories of self-certainty in the psychoeducation group. In red is depicted the “Low and Stable SC” (n = 23) trajectory. The second trajectory, in blue, refers to the “Worsening SC” (n = 11) trajectory.
Figure 4. Trajectories of self-certainty in the psychoeducation group. In red is depicted the “Low and Stable SC” (n = 23) trajectory. The second trajectory, in blue, refers to the “Worsening SC” (n = 11) trajectory.
Healthcare 10 02155 g004
Table 1. Baseline differences in sociodemographic, clinical, social cognitive, and metacognitive variables between the psychoeducation and the MCT groups.
Table 1. Baseline differences in sociodemographic, clinical, social cognitive, and metacognitive variables between the psychoeducation and the MCT groups.
MCT (n = 28)Psychoeducation (n = 34)
Mean/%SDMean/%SDt/χ2p
Age
Sex (% males)75.00 73.50 0.170.90
Number of hospital admissions
Education (%) 4.6650.46
Incomplete primary education21.40 11.80
Complete primary education28.60 17.60
Incomplete secondary education10.70 20.60
Complete secondary education25.00 26.50
Incomplete superior education3.60 14.70
Complete superior education10.70 8.80
Diagnosis (%) 4.9290.43
Schizophrenia46.40 55.90
Non-specified psychotic disorder21.40 11.80
Schizoaffective disorder3.60 11.80
Delusional disorder7.10 11.80
Brief psychotic episode10.70 11.80
Schizophreniform disorder7.10 8.80
PANSS
Positive symptoms12.253.87412.534.4870.260.80
Negative symptoms14.547.16215.154.7940.400.69
General symptoms27.367.79027.265.920−0.050.96
Total54.1415.71754.9412.1100.230.82
GAF66.6113.33157.5011.758−2.860.01
SUMD global score5.822.6956.033.6390.250.80
Beads task
85–153.62963.854555.35294.277371.6320.11
60–406.44443.993588.52945.165361.7260.09
Affective 6.37044.507677.79414.117811.2861.286
IPSAQ
Externalizing bias0.14813.449681.55883.854881.4860.14
Personalizing bias1.25690.726671.21830.50730−0.240.81
Hinting task4.71431.013144.67650.97610−0.150.88
Rosenberg self-esteem27.57146.4200427.38245.03933−0.130.90
Faces test17.67861.8268317.47061.39773−0.510.61
Estimated premorbid IQ99.423114.8544296.718813.71421−0.720.48
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ferrer-Quintero, M.; Fernández, D.; López-Carrilero, R.; Díaz-Cutraro, L.; Verdaguer-Rodríguez, M.; García-Mieres, H.; Huerta-Ramos, E.; Gómez-Benito, J.; Peláez, T.; Birulés, I.; et al. Heterogeneity in Response to MCT and Psychoeducation: A Feasibility Study Using Latent Class Mixed Models in First-Episode Psychosis. Healthcare 2022, 10, 2155. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10112155

AMA Style

Ferrer-Quintero M, Fernández D, López-Carrilero R, Díaz-Cutraro L, Verdaguer-Rodríguez M, García-Mieres H, Huerta-Ramos E, Gómez-Benito J, Peláez T, Birulés I, et al. Heterogeneity in Response to MCT and Psychoeducation: A Feasibility Study Using Latent Class Mixed Models in First-Episode Psychosis. Healthcare. 2022; 10(11):2155. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10112155

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ferrer-Quintero, Marta, Daniel Fernández, Raquel López-Carrilero, Luciana Díaz-Cutraro, Marina Verdaguer-Rodríguez, Helena García-Mieres, Elena Huerta-Ramos, Juana Gómez-Benito, Trini Peláez, Irene Birulés, and et al. 2022. "Heterogeneity in Response to MCT and Psychoeducation: A Feasibility Study Using Latent Class Mixed Models in First-Episode Psychosis" Healthcare 10, no. 11: 2155. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10112155

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop