Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(3) | a. | má | ||
VIEWP3 | ||||
b. |
2. Empirical and Analytical Background
2.1. Background on Shupamem
2.2. - Movement in Shupamem
(15) | a. | * Whoi did they inform himi that Joan would call ___? (Postal 1993, p. 543) |
b. | * Whoi did hisi sister call ___ a moron? (Postal 1993, p. 540) |
(17) | A | quién | dijo | cada | testigo | que | María | le-quería | pegar? |
to | whom | said | each | witness | that | Maria | him-wanted | to.hit | |
‘Whom did each witness say that María wanted to hit?’ (Aguero Bautista 2001, p. 172) |
3. Extraction out of Clausal Domains Is Pervasive
3.1. Sentential Subject Constructions
3.2. Complex Noun Phrase Constructions
3.3. Adjunct Clauses
3.3.1. Temporal Clauses
3.3.2. Reason Clauses
3.3.3. Conditional Clauses
4. Arguments for - Movement out of Clausal Domains
4.1. Sentential Subject Constructions
4.2. Complex Noun Phrase Constructions
4.3. Adjunct Clauses
4.3.1. Temporal Clauses
4.3.2. Reason Clauses
4.3.3. Conditional Clauses
4.4. Interim Summary
5. Additional Evidence for Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem
6. Other (Indecisive) Diagnostics for - Movement out of Clausal Domains
6.1. Parasitic Gap Licensing
(55) | a. | Here is the influential professor that John sent his book to ___ in order to impress ___. (Engdahl 1983, p. 11) |
b. | * John sent his book to the influential professor in order to impress ___. |
6.2. Superiority Effects
6.3. Idiom Formation
6.4. Sluicing
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | The notion of extraction assumes a movement operation has taken place. We refer the reader to Section 2.2 and Section 4, where - movement diagnostics for the relevant “islands” are deployed. |
2 | All data are based on fieldwork with one native speaker of the language, the third author. We transcribe the data using the International Phonetic Alphabet even though Shupamem has a writing system (see Omniglot [https://omniglot.com/writing/bamum.htm (accessed on 17 September 2023)] or LearnBamun [http://www.learnbamum.com/study-now (accessed on 17 September 2023)]). Abbreviations follow the Leipzig Glossing Rules with minor deviations and include: 1 first person; 2 second person; comp complementizer; conj conjunction; cop copula; dem demonstrative; expl expletive; foc focus; fut future; inan inanimate; inf infinitive; irr irrealis; loc locative; ncl noun class; neg negative; obl oblique; part particle; pl plural; prog progressive; pst past; q question; rel relativizer; sg singular; ViewP viewpoint |
3 | Nchare and Terzi (2014) discuss prenominal viewpoint markers in Shupamem. See Nchare and Terzi 2014 for a prepositional analysis of viewpoint markers in the language and further details on their extraction properties. |
4 | For reasons of space, we focus on a subset of suspected strong island configurations to the exclusion of weak islands in this article. See Schurr forthcoming for discussion of weak islands and successful extraction from them in the language. |
5 | Shupamem has four surface tones: high (), low (), rising () and falling (). We have taken great pains in this paper to represent the tones as accurately as possible. Our tonal transcriptions are surface tonal transcriptions. The reader can rest assured that most, if not all, apparent inconsistencies in the representation of tones are due to various rules and alternations that make the Shupamem tone system rather complex. |
6 | By basic word order, we refer to the “elsewhere” common order in indicative clauses in the absence of pragmatically informative functions such as topic shift or identificational focus (see Dryer 1995; Van der Wal 2015). |
7 | See Schurr forthcoming for a more detailed discussion of resumption in the language. |
8 | We assume that resumptive pronouns are obligatorily spelled out copies (Landau 2006; Pesetsky 1998). Nchare (2012, pp. 519–20) provides an overview of personal pronouns in Shupamem. See Schurr forthcoming for further discussion of resumption, epithets and epithet-like expressive terms in the language. |
9 | A negative copula is licit in negative focus-cleft contructions, but yields unacceptability in negative topicalization constructions (Nchare 2012, p. 455). We note one other possible position for copulae in the - configurations discussed in this article. We have recently discovered that in focus clefts (though not in topicalization structures), an overt pre-nasalized copula (Yiangnigni 2016, p. 122) may surface post-nominally under certain conditions (i). Crucially, however, the overt copula pǎ may not occur in this environment. |
10 | Cardinaletti (2019)’s proposal regarding a relation in the Romance language family between clause-internal resumption and the availability of extraction points in the same direction, in so far as cliticization implicates extraction from a Big DP. |
11 | The expletive subject surfaces with an H tone in indicative clauses, but in interrogative clauses it takes an L tone alongside a clause-final relative particle that bears an interrogative L tone, as in (16) (see Nchare 2012, sct. 5, p. 497ff). |
12 | By ‘picture noun’ anaphors we refer to expressions such as a picture of himself in example (ii). (ii) Tomi believes that there is [a picture of himselfi] hanging in the post office. (Jackendoff 1972, p. 133) We acknowledge an anonymous reviewer’s comment that such expressions may be locally bound by a null logophoric pronoun (in the spirit of Charnavel and Bryant 2023). In fact, preliminary results of an exploratory study in Shupamem suggest this may be the case in the language. See Schurr forthcoming for further discussion. |
13 | A rather widely accepted view is that R-expressions reconstruct for Condition C if they are embedded inside arguments, but Condition C is not violated when R-expressions are embedded inside adjuncts (first reported in Riemsdijk and Williams 1981). Against this view, Bruening and Al Khalaf (2019) bring forth evidence from English that the correct generalization distinguishes R-expression complements of nouns, which do not reconstruct for Condition C, from R-expression complements of (non-nominal) prepositions, which do reconstruct, yielding a Condition C violation. This special status of nouns appears to apply in Shupamem, making Condition C a potentially reliable diagnostic for - dependencies. For example, in VP ex situ structures, a Condition C violation is observed when focus-clefting a transitive verb with its proper name object, excluding coreference betweeen it and the embedded third person subject pronoun in example (iii.a). However, if the rigid designator is more deeply embedded as the complement of a noun in the object position (‘the brothers of Mimshe’ in (iii.b)), Condition C is not violated. The discrepancy between the observed reconstruction in (iii.a) and lack thereof (iii.b) in VP ex situ structures could, in principle, be attributed to further nesting of the R-expression in the complement of the ex situ verb in (iii.b). If this were true, proper name complements of displaced constituents would be expected to reconstruct and yield a Condition C violation, as in the base sentence in (iv.a), contrary to fact. Condition C is not violated when the object Mèfìrè (’the junior brothers of Mefire’) is displaced to the left-edge in focus-cleft constructions (iv.b). We leave considerations from Condition C regarding the transparency of clausal domains for future research. Condition C in Shupamem is briefly discussed by Nchare (2012, p. 547). Schurr (forthcoming) explores Condition C with respect to suspected clausal islands in more detail. |
14 | Wh- quantifier interactions are similarly used as a movement diagnostic in Shona, a Bantu language of Zimbabwe and Mozambique (see Zentz 2016). |
15 | The absence of cross-clausal syntactic connectivity effects with the licensing discourse environment (the immediately preceding discourse in (22A) forces us to assume the quantifier modifies a null pronoun in example (22B), not a trace. We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting that we test whether quantifiers may be licensed by in situ pronouns. |
16 | Extraction out of definite relative clauses gives rise to weak crossover effects regardless of whether the RC head is an RC-internal argument, as in example (33), or not, as illustrated below in example (v). |
17 | We acknowledge the existence of alternative analyses in which NCI-licensing involves movement that is sensitive to clause boundaries, as Haegeman and Zanuttini (1991) put forth for West Flemish. Whether or not (covert) movement is implicated in deriving the relevant configurations in Shupamem, we consider this evidence against their status as opaque clausal domains. |
18 | Schurr (forthcoming) discusses additional indecisive considerations regarding strong crossover effects and reconstruction for Condition A of the binding theory in the language. |
19 | An anonymous reviewer remarks that “light verb idioms”, composed of a non-straightforwardly predictable verb and a more transparent contribution on the part of a collocated noun, do support the distinction between A- and - dependencies in English. In Shupamem, such idioms, as in (vi.a) using the light verb fà (‘gave’), appear to pattern with the “opaque” idioms we study in this section in view of the absence of an idiomatic interpretation in the ex situ object variant (vi.b). We conclude that idiom formation cannot be used to detect - dependencies in the language. |
20 | Wh- in situ sources of sluices are also reported in other languages, such as English (Hankamer 1979; Kimura 2010; Morgan 1973), German (Ott and Struckmeier 2016), Dutch (Ott and Struckmeier 2016), Japanese (Abe 2015), and Spanish (Stigliano 2022). |
21 | Paradigm (vii) demonstrates the distinction between L and H tone inanimate wh- objects in clause-internal position (vii.a) and focus-cleft constructions (vii.b), respectively. |
22 | An analysis of N-word fragment answers in Shupamem as in situ sluices aligns with Kroll (2019)’s observation of polarity reversal under sluicing in English. While N-words cannot be fronted in Shupamem (73), they may occupy in situ focus positions in inversion constructions, in which the verb precedes the in situ logical subject (viii.b). This dovetails nicely with an in situ derivation of the polarity-reversed fragment answer in (72B) along the lines of the wh- in situ sluice in (71). |
23 | Accounts of the opacity of islands as due to the interaction between the syntactic component and the interfaces (Fox and Pesetsky 2005; Pesetsky 1982) or due to the opacity of some domains for agreement processes (and therefore to any movement that involves Agree relations, e.g., Boeckx 2003; Rackowski and Richards 2005) may fare differently. |
24 | The existence of uncontroversial effects of processing difficulty encountered in or at the edge of island domains does not exclude any role for narrow syntax in the formal makeup of islands (as argued previously in Phillips 2013a). |
25 | See Müller 2019 for a recent review of the permeability of adjunct islands in Mainland Scandiniavian. |
26 | Schurr(forthcoming) considers the derivation of categorially diverse prominent constituents in - configurations in the language. See also endnote 13. |
27 | Major and Torrence (2021) show that the transparency is only apparent because the structure in question is actually a serial verb construction, not a clausal complement of N. |
28 | Fominyam (2021) argues that these dependencies do not actually implicate - movement. On the other hand, covert - extraction is arguably involved in licensing in situ wh- items in the same domains (Fominyam 2021). |
29 | We have not included possible violations of the Coordinate Structure Constraint in Table 3 since these do not necessarily reflect clausal domains, which make up the focus of this article. To show the promise of an Africanist perspective in this regard, it suffices to mention a number of apparent violations of the Coordinate Structure Constraint in African languages. Possible - extraction from Coordinate NP constructions is documented in Igbo, in which clausal domains including sentential subjects, complex NPs and adjunct clauses all have the status of islands (Georgi and Amaechi 2020; Goldsmith 1981), and Medumba (Keupdjio 2020). In the same vein, Khoekhoegowab shows an asymmetry in extraction from VP conjuncts where the object can topicalize from the first conjunct but not from the second (Kusmer 2018). |
References
- Abe, Jun. 2015. The In-Situ Approach to Sluicing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Abrusán, Márta. 2014. Weak Island Semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Adesola, Oluseye. 2006. On the Absence of Superiority and Weak Crossover Effects in Yoruba. Linguistic Inquiry 37: 309–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aguero Bautista, Calixto. 2001. Cyclicity and the Scope of Wh-Phrases. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Aranovich, Raúl. 1994. Negative Concord in Spasnish and In-situ Licensing. In Proceedings of the 12th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Edited by Eric Duncan, Donka Farkas and Philip Spaelti. Standford: CSLI, pp. 203–18. [Google Scholar]
- Baunaz, Lena. 2008. Floating Quantifiers: French Universal Quantifiers and N-words. In Rivista di Grammatica Generativa 33 (Selected Proceedings of the 34th Incontro di Grammatica Generativa). Edited by Paola Benincà, Federico Damonte and Nicoletta Penello. Padova: Unipress, pp. 25–42. [Google Scholar]
- Boeckx, Cedric. 2003. Islands and Chains: Resumption as Stranding. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Bondevik, Ingrid, Dave Kush, and Terje Lohndal. 2021. Variation in Adjunct Islands: The Case of Norwegian. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 44: 223–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bresnan, Joan. 1990. African Languages and Syntactic Theories. In Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 20 (The Contribution of African Linguistics to Linguistic Theory: Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference on African Linguistics). Edited by Eyamba G. Bokamba, Amy C. Cheatham, Dorothy E. Evans and Rick Treece. Urbana: Department of Linguistics, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, pp. 35–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bresnan, Joan, and Sam A. Mchombo. 1987. Topic, Pronoun, and Agreement in Chicheŵa. Language 63: 741–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruening, Benjamin, and Eman Al Khalaf. 2019. No Argument–Adjunct Asymmetry in Reconstruction for Binding Condition C. Journal of Linguistics 55: 247–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cardinaletti, Anna. 2019. Cliticization as Extraction: The Big DP Hypothesis Revisited. Revista da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística 5: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carstens, Vicki, and Loyiso Mletshe. 2016. Negative Concord and Nominal Licensing in Xhosa and Zulu. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 34: 761–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charnavel, Isabelle, and Shannon Bryant. 2023. The Whole Picture: Disentangling Locality, Logophoricity and Subjecthood in English Picture Noun Anaphora. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 41: 547–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist Inquiries: The Framework. In Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Edited by Roger Martin, David Michaels and Juan Uriagereka. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 89–155. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by Phase. In Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Edited by Michael J. Kenstowicz. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2008. On Phases. In Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud. Edited by Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero and Maria Luisa Zubizarreta. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 133–66. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam, and Howard Lasnik. 1993. The Theory of Principles and Parameters. In Syntax: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research. Edited by Joachim Jacobs, Arnim von Stechow, Wolfgang Sternefeld and Theo Vennemann. Berlin: de Gruyter, pp. 506–69. [Google Scholar]
- Cinque, Guglielmo. 2020. The Syntax of Relative Clauses: A Unified Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cole, Peter, and Gabriella Hermon. 1994. Is there LF Wh-movement? Linguistic Inquiry 25: 239–62. [Google Scholar]
- Condoravdi, Cleo. 2010. NPI Licensing in Temporal Clauses. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28: 877–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demirdache, Hamida, and Orin Percus. 2011. Resumptives, Movement and Interpretation. In Resumptive Pronouns at the Interfaces. Edited by Alain Rouveret. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 369–93. [Google Scholar]
- Devlin, Kerri, Travis Major, and Harold Torrence. 2021. A Note on Wh- Questions in Avatime. In Celebrating 50 Years of ACAL: Selected Papers from the 50th Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Edited by Akinbiyi Akinlabi, Lee Bickmore, Michael Cahill, Michael Diercks, Laura J. Downing, James Essegbey, Katie Franich, Laura McPherson and Sharon Rose. Berlin: Language Science Press, pp. 55–72. [Google Scholar]
- Downing, Laura J., and Larry M. Hyman. 2016. Information Structure in Bantu. In The Oxford Handbook of Information Structure. Edited by Caroline Féry and Shinichiro Ishihara. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dryer, Matthew S. 1995. Frequency and Pragmatically Unmarked Word Order. In Word Order in Discourse. Edited by Pamela A. Downing and Michael Noonan. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 105–35. [Google Scholar]
- Eberhard, David M., Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig. 2021. Ethnologue: Languages of the World Languages, 24th ed. Dallas: SIL International. [Google Scholar]
- Engdahl, Elisabet. 1983. Parasitic Gaps. Linguistics and Philosophy 6: 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faarlund, Jan Terje. 1992. Morfologi: Bøyingssystemet i Nynorsk og Bokmål. Oslo: Det Norske Samlaget. [Google Scholar]
- Fominyam, Henry Zamchang. 2021. Aspects of Awing Grammar and Information Structure. Ph.D. thesis, Universität Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany. [Google Scholar]
- Fox, Danny, and David Pesetsky. 2005. Cyclic Linearization of Syntactic Structure. Theoretical Linguistics 31: 1–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgi, Doreen, and Mary Amaechi. 2020. Resumption and Islandhood in Igbo. In Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society. Edited by Mariam Asatryan, Yixiao Song and Ayana Whitmal. Amherst: GLSA, pp. 261–74. [Google Scholar]
- Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2006. N-words and Negative Concord. In The Blackwell Companion to Syntax 3. Edited by Martin Everaert and Henk Van Riemsdijk. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 327–91. [Google Scholar]
- Givón, Talmy. 1976. Topic, Pronoun and Grammatical Agreement in Subject and Topic. In Subject and Topic. Edited by Charles N. Li. New York: Academic Press, pp. 149–88. [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith, John. 1981. The Structure of Wh- Questions in Igbo. Linguistic Analysis 7: 367–93. [Google Scholar]
- Gould, Isaac, and Tessa Scott. 2019. On the Derivation of Swahili Amba Relative Clauses: Evidence for Movement. In Theory and Description in African Linguistics: Selected Papers from the 47th Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Edited by Emily Clem, Peter Jenks and Hannah Sande. Berlin: Language Science Press, pp. 441–62. [Google Scholar]
- Guéron, Jacqueline. 1979. Relations de Coréférence dans la Phrase et dans le Discours. Langue Française, 42–79. [Google Scholar]
- Haegeman, Liliane, and Raffaella Zanuttini. 1991. Negative Heads and the Neg Criterion. The Linguistic Review 8: 233–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hankamer, Jorge. 1979. Deletion in Coordinate Structures. New York: Garland Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, John A. 1999. Processing Complexity and Filler-gap Dependencies across Grammars. Language 75: 244–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hein, Johannes. 2020a. Island Constraints Are Category-Sensitive: The Case of Asante Twi and Limbum. Be’erSheva: Ben Gurion University of the Negev. [Google Scholar]
- Hein, Johannes. 2020b. Selective Island-Sensitivity in Asante Twi and Limbum. Amherst: University of Massachussetts. [Google Scholar]
- Hein, Johannes. n.d. Category-sensitive Escape from Islands in Limbum and Asante Twi. To appear in this volume.
- Hein, Johannes, and Doreen Georgi. 2021. Asymmetries in Asante Twi -movement: On the Role of Noun Type in Resumption. In Proceedings of the 51st Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society. Edited by Alessa Farinella and Angelica Hill. Amherst: GSLA, pp. 223–36. [Google Scholar]
- Henderson, Brent. 2006. Multiple Agreement and Inversion in Bantu. Syntax 9: 275–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henderson, Brent. 2011. African Languages and Syntactic Theory: Impacts and Directions. In Selected Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Edited by Eyamba G. Bokamba, Ryan K. Shoosted and Bezza Tesfaw Ayalew. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 15–25. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, Ray S. 1972. Semantic Interpretation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jespersen, Otto. 1922. Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin. London: George Allen and Unwin. [Google Scholar]
- Kandybowicz, Jason, and Abdoulaye Laziz Nchare. 2023. Integrated Non-restrictive Relative Clauses in Shupamem. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 41: 655–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kandybowicz, Jason. 2009. Externalization and Emergence: On the Status of Parameters in the Minimalist Program. Biolinguistics 3: 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kandybowicz, Jason, Bertille Baron, Philip T. Duncan, and Hironori Katsuda. 2023. Ikpana Interrogatives. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kayne, Richard S. 1994. The Antisymmetry of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Keupdjio, Hermann Sidoine. 2020. The Syntax of -dependencies in Bamileke Medumba. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Kimura, Hiroko. 2010. A Wh-in situ Strategy for Sluicing. English Linguistics 27: 43–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kluender, Robert. 1998. On the Distinction between Strong and Weak Islands: A Processing Perspective. In The Limits of Syntax. Edited by Peter Culicover and Louise McNally. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 241–79. [Google Scholar]
- Korsah, Sampson, and Andrew Murphy. 2019. Islands and Resumption in Asante Twi. In Proceedings of the 36th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Edited by Richard Stockwell, Maura O’Leary, Zhongshi Xu and Z. L. Zhou. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Projects, pp. 226–36. [Google Scholar]
- Kroll, Margaret. 2019. Polarity Reversals under Sluicing. Semantics and Pragmatics 12: 1–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kush, Dave Whitney, Terje Lohndal, and Jon Sprouse. 2018. Investigating Variation in Island Effects: A Case Study of Norwegian Wh-Extraction. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 36: 743–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kusmer, Leland P. 2018. An Itsy-Bitsy Puzzle: Asymmetric Extraction from Coordination in Khoekhoegowab. Paper presented at 49th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA, March 22–25. [Google Scholar]
- Landau, Idan. 2006. Chain Resolution in Hebrew V(P)-fronting. Syntax 9: 32–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Major, Travis, and Harold Torrence. 2021. “Say” Chains, Not “Say” Complementation. In Proceedings of the 38th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Edited by Rachel Soo, Una Y. Chow and Sander Nederveen. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Projects, pp. 283–93. [Google Scholar]
- McCloskey, James. 2000. Quantifier Float and Wh-movement in an Irish English. Linguistic Inquiry 31: 57–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mendes, Gesoel, and Jason Kandybowicz. 2023. Salvation by Deletion in Nupe. Linguistic Inquiry 54: 299–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merchant, Jason. 2001. The Syntax of Silence: Sluicing, Islands, and the Theory of Ellipsis. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Morgan, Jerrold. 1973. Sentence Fragments and the Notion ‘Sentence’. In Issues in Linguistics: Papers in Honor of Henry and Renée Kahane. Edited by Braj B. Kachru, Robert B. Lees, Yakov Malkiel, Angelina Pietrangeli and Sol Saporta. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, pp. 719–51. [Google Scholar]
- Müller, Christiane. 2019. Permeable Islands: A Contrastive Study of Swedish and English Adjunct Clause Extractions. Ph.D. thesis, Lund University, Lund, Sweden. [Google Scholar]
- Nchare, Abdoulaye Laziz. 2012. The Grammar of Shupamem. Ph.D. thesis, New York University, New York, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Nchare, Abdoulaye Laziz, and Arhonto Terzi. 2014. Licensing Silent Structure: The Spatial Prepositions of Shupamem. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 32: 673–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ott, Dennis, and Volker Struckmeier. 2016. Deletion in Clausal Ellipsis: Remnants in the Middle Field. In Proceedings of the 39th Annual Penn Linguistics Conference. U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 22(1). Edited by Sunghye Cho. Philadelphia: Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania, pp. 225–34. [Google Scholar]
- Patel-Grosz, Pritty. 2012. (Anti)locality at the Interfaces. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Pesetsky, David. 1982. Paths and Categories. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Pesetsky, David. 1998. Some Optimality Principles of Sentence Pronunciation. In Is the Best Good Enough? Optimality and Competition in Syntax. Edited by Pilar Barbosa, Danny Fox, Martha McGinnis and David Pesetsky. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 337–83. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips, Colin. 2013a. On the Nature of Island Constraints I: Language Processing and Reductionist Accounts. In Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Edited by Jon Sprouse and Norbert Hornstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 64–108. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips, Colin. 2013b. On the Nature of Island Constraints II: Language Learning and Innateness. In Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Edited by Jon Sprouse and Norbert Hornstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 132–57. [Google Scholar]
- Postal, Paul. 1971. Cross-Over Phenomena. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [Google Scholar]
- Postal, Paul. 1993. Remarks on Weak Crossover Effects. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 539–56. [Google Scholar]
- Rackowski, Andrea, and Norvin Richards. 2005. Phase Edge and Extraction: A Tagalog Case Study. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 565–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reinhart, Tanya. 1976. The Syntactic Domain of Anaphora. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Riemsdijk, Henk van, and Edwin Williams. 1981. NP-Structure. The Linguistic Review 1: 171–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ross, John Robert. 1969. Guess Who? In Papers from the Fifth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Edited by Robert I. Binnick, Alice Davison, Georgia M. Green and Jerry L. Morgan. Chicago: Department of Linguistics, University of Chicago, pp. 252–86. [Google Scholar]
- Saah, Kofi. 1992. Null Object Constructions in Akan. In Proceedings of the Kwa Comparative Syntax Workshop. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 17. Edited by Chris Collins and Victor Manfredi. Cambridge, MA: MIT Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, pp. 219–44. [Google Scholar]
- Saah, Kofi. 1994. Studies in Akan Syntax, Acquisition, and Sentence Processing. Ph.D. thesis, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Safir, Ken. 2004. The Syntax of (In)dependence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schurr, Hagay. Forthcoming. Absence of Clausal Island Effects in Shupamem. Ph.D. thesis, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, USA.
- Scott, Tessa. 2021. Two Types of Resumptive Pronouns in Swahili. Linguistic Inquiry 52: 812–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sichel, Ivy. 2014. Resumptive Pronouns and Competition. Linguistic Inquiry 45: 655–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sichel, Ivy. 2018. Anatomy of a Counterexample: Extraction from Relative Clauses. Linguistic Inquiry 49: 335–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stigliano, Laura. 2022. P-omission in Ellipsis in Spanish: Evidence for Syntactic Identity. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 40: 1353–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torrence, Harold, and Jason Kandybowicz. 2015. Wh-question Formation in Krachi. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 36: 253–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Wal, Jenneke. 2015. Bantu Syntax. In Oxford Handbook Topics in Linguistics. (Online Editoin, Oxford Academic, 5 December 2014). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Fintel, Kai. 1999. NPI Licensing, Strawson Entailment, and Context Dependency. Journal of Semantics 16: 97–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wasow, Thomas. 1979. Anaphora in Generative Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Yiangnigni, Abass Ngoungouo. 2016. The Morpho-Syntax of Adverbs in Shupamem. Master’s thesis, University of Yaounde I, Yaoundé, Cameroon. [Google Scholar]
- Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2008. Negative Concord is Syntactic Agreement. Unpublished manuscript. University of Amsterdam. [Google Scholar]
- Zentz, Jason. 2016. Forming Wh-questions in Shona: A Comparative Bantu Perspective. Ph.D. thesis, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
Expletive Subject | Copula | Focus Marker | Relativizer | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Indicative Focus-cleft | ✓ | ✓ | ✗ | ✓ |
Interrogative Focus-cleft | ✓ | ✗ | ✗ | ✓ |
Crossover Effects | Reconstruction Effects | Parasitic Gap Licensing | Quantifier Float | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sentential Subjects | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Definite Relative Clauses | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Cl. Complements of N | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Temporal Clauses | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Reason Clauses | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Conditional Clauses | ✓ | ✓ | (✓) | ✓ |
Akan | Avatime | Awing | Ikpana | Limbum | Medumba | Swahili | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sentential Subjects | ✓ | – | – | ✗ | – | N/A | – |
Definite RCs | ✓ | ✗ | ✓ | ✗ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
Cl. Compls. of N | ✓ | ✓ | – | ✗ | ✓ | ✓ | – |
Temporal Clauses | – | ✗ | ✓ | ✓ | – | ✓ | ✓ |
Reason Clauses | ✓ | – | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | – | ✓ |
Conditional Cls. | – | – | – | ✓ | – | – | – |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Schurr, H.; Kandybowicz, J.; Nchare, A.L.; Bucknor, T.; Ma, X.; Markowska, M.; Tapia, A. Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem. Languages 2024, 9, 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9010007
Schurr H, Kandybowicz J, Nchare AL, Bucknor T, Ma X, Markowska M, Tapia A. Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem. Languages. 2024; 9(1):7. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9010007
Chicago/Turabian StyleSchurr, Hagay, Jason Kandybowicz, Abdoulaye Laziz Nchare, Tysean Bucknor, Xiaomeng Ma, Magdalena Markowska, and Armando Tapia. 2024. "Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem" Languages 9, no. 1: 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9010007
APA StyleSchurr, H., Kandybowicz, J., Nchare, A. L., Bucknor, T., Ma, X., Markowska, M., & Tapia, A. (2024). Absence of Clausal Islands in Shupamem. Languages, 9(1), 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9010007