Language Competence in Italian Heritage Speakers: The Contribution of Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Heritage Languages
1.2. Differentiating Bilingualism and Language Impariment
1.3. Clinical Markers for Italian in Bilinguals: Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition
1.4. The Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Background Tasks
2.2.1. LEAP-Q
2.2.2. TROG-2
2.2.3. Comprendo
2.3. Experimental Tasks
2.3.1. Clitic Production
(1) | a. | Preamble: In questa storia, una signora vuole dipingere una maschera. Guarda, cosa fa la signora alla maschera? | ||
‘‘In this story, a lady wants to paint a mask. Look, what is the lady doing with the mask?" | ||||
b. | Elicited answer: | la | dipinge | |
pro it-CL.FEM | paint-3SG | |||
‘she is painting it.’ |
(2) | a. | Preamble: In questa storia, una signora vuole dipingere una maschera. Guarda, cosa fa la signora? | ||
‘‘In this story, a lady wants to paint a mask. Look, what is the lady doing?" | ||||
b. | Elicited answer: | dipinge | la maschera | |
pro paint.3SG | the mask | |||
‘she is painting the mask.’ |
2.3.2. Nonword Repetition Task
(3) | BA (1 syllable) |
BLOZ | |
RORDO (2 syllables) | |
SASFRA | |
TASTOLA (3 syllables) |
2.3.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Tasks
3.2. Clitic Production Task
(4) | L’ha | presa |
pro-CL has | taken | |
‘(he/she) took it.’ |
3.3. NWR Task
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Argyri, Efrosyni, and Antonella Sorace. 2007. Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10: 79–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Armon-Lotem, Sharon, John H. De Jong, and Natalia Meir, eds. 2015. Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Arosio, Fabrizio, Chiara Branchini, Lina Barbieri, and Maria Teresa Guasti. 2014. Failure to produce direct object clitic pronouns as a clinical marker of SLI in school-aged Italian speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 28: 639–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan. 2003. Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders 36: 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedore, Lisa M., and Laurence B. Leonard. 2001. Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44: 905–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belletti, Adriana, and Cornelia Hamann. 2004. On the L2/bilingual acquisition of French by two young children with different source languages. In The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts: Focus on Functional Categories. Edited by Philippe Prévost and Johanne Paradis. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 147–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benmamoun, Elabbas, Silvina Montrul, and Maria Polinsky. 2013. Heritage languages and their speakers: Opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 39: 129–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Giulia. 2012. Gender in Italian-German bilinguals: A comparison with German L2 learners of Italian. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 538–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bice, Kinsey, and Judith F. Kroll. 2021. Grammatical processing in two languages: How individual differences in language experience and cognitive abilities shape comprehension in heritage bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics 58: 100963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, Dorothy V. M. 2003. Test for the Reception of Grammar—Version 2 (TROG-2). London: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, Dorothy V. M. 2017. Why is it so hard to reach agreement on terminology? The case of developmental language disorder (DLD). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 52: 671–80. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, Dorothy V. M., Tony North, and Chris Donlan. 1996. Nonword repetition as a phenotypic marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37: 391–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, Elena, Tessel Boerma, and Jan De Jong. 2019. First Language Attrition and Developmental Language Disorder. In Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition. Edited by Monika S. Schmid and Barbara Köpke. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 108–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bortolini, Umberta, M. Cristina Caselli, Patricia Deevy, and Laurence B. Leonard. 2002. Specific language impairment in Italian: The first steps in the search for a clinical marker. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 37: 77–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bortolini, Umberta, Barbara Arfé, M. Cristina Caselli, Luisa Degasperi, Patricia Deevy, and Laurence B. Leonard. 2006. Clinical markers for specific language impairment in Italian: The contribution of clitics and non-word repetition. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 41: 695–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, Roger. 1973. Development of the first language in the human species. American Psychologist 28: 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cardinaletti, Anna, and Michael Starke. 1999. The typology of structural deficiency: A case study of the three classes of pronouns. In Clitics in Languages of Europe 8. Edited by Henk van Riemsdijk. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 145–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casalini, Claudia, Daniela Brizzolara, Anna Chilosi, Paola Cipriani, Stefania Marcolini, Chiara Pecini, Silvia Roncoli, and Cristina Burani. 2007. Non-word repetition in children with specific language impairment: A deficit in phonological working memory or in long-term verbal knowledge? Cortex 43: 769–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cecchetto, Carlo, Alberto di Domenico, Maria Garraffa, and Costanza Papagno. 2012. Comprendo. Batteria per la Comprensione di Frasi negli Adulti. Milano: Raffello Cortina Editore. [Google Scholar]
- Conti-Ramsden, Gina. 2003. Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Language, Speech, and Hearing Research 46: 1029–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornoldi, Cesare, Lidio Miato, Adriana Dal Molin, and Silvana Poli. 2009. PRCR-2/2009. Prove di Prerequisito per la Diagnosi delle Difficoltà di Lettura e Scrittura. Firenze: Giunti. [Google Scholar]
- Cuza, Alejandro, and Nelleke Strik. 2012. Patterns of morphosyntactic convergence and child L1 attrition: Evidence from subject-verb inversion in Spanish-English bilingual children. In Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL42). Cedar City: Southern Utah University. [Google Scholar]
- Dollaghan, Chris, and Thomas F. Campbell. 1998. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41: 1136–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dromi, Esther, Laurence B. Leonard, Galit Adam, and Sara Zadunaisky-Ehrlich. 1999. Verb agreement morphology in Hebrew-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42: 1414–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenyvesi, Anna. 2005. Hungarian in the United States. In Hungarian Language Contact outside Hungary: Studies on Hungarian as a Minority Language. Edited by Anna Fenyvesi. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 265–318. [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari, Ida. 2006. Acquisition of object clitics in Italian by two German/Italian bilingual children. In Language Acquisition and Development. Proceedings of GALA 2005. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 173–83. [Google Scholar]
- Fishman, Joshua A. 2001. 300-plus years of heritage language education in the United States. In Heritage Languages in America: Preserving a National Resource. Edited by Joy Kreeft Peyton, Donald A. Ranard and Scott McGinnis. Washington, DC and McHenry: Center for Applied Linguistics and Delta Systems, pp. 81–97. [Google Scholar]
- Garraffa, Maria, Maria Vender, Antonella Sorace, and Maria T. Guasti. 2019. Is it possible to differentiate multilingual children and children with developmental language disorder? Languages, Society and Policy. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Genesee, Fred, Johanne Paradis, and Martha Crago. 2004. Dual Language Development and Disorders: A Handbook on Bilingualism and Second Language Learning. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Granfeldt, Jonas, and Suzanne Schlyter. 2004. Cliticisation in the acquisition of French as L1 and L2. In The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts: Focus on Functional Categories. Edited by Philippe Prévost and Johanne Paradis. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 333–70. [Google Scholar]
- Grimm, Angela, and Petra Schulz. 2014. Specific language impairment and early second language acquisition: The risk of over- and underdiagnosis. Child Indicators Research 7: 821–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guasti, Maria T., Silvia Palma, Elisabetta Genovese, Paolo Stagi, Gabriella Saladini, and Fabrizio Arosio. 2016. The production of direct object clitics in pre-school–and primary school–aged children with specific language impairments. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 30: 663–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamann, Cornelia. 2012. Bilingual development and language assessment. In Proceedings of BUCLD 36. Edited by Alia K. Biller, Esther Y. Chung and Amelia E. Kimball. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Hamann, Cornelia, and Adriana Belletti. 2006. Developmental patterns in the acquisition of complement clitic pronouns: Comparing different acquisition modes with an emphasis on French. Rivista di Grammatica Generativa 31: 39–78. [Google Scholar]
- Hamann, Cornelia, Stéphanie Ohayon, Sébastien Dubé, Ulrich H. Frauenfelder, Luigi Rizzi, Michal Starke, and Pascal Zesiger. 2003. Aspects of grammatical development in young French children with SLI. Developmental Science 6: 151–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulk, Aafke, and Natascha Müller. 2000. Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3: 227–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jakubowicz, Celia. 1984. On markedness and binding principles. In Proceedings of NELS 14. Edited by Charles Jones and Peter Sells. Amherst: GLSA UMass/Amherst, pp. 154–82. [Google Scholar]
- Jakubowicz, Celia, Lea Nash, Catherine Rigaut, and Christophe-Loic Gerard. 1998. Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition 7: 113–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kayne, Richard S. 1975. French Syntax: The Transformational Cycle. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kupisch, Tanya, and Jason Rothman. 2018. Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 22: 564–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Laleko, Oksana, and Maria Polinsky. 2016. Between syntax and discourse: Topic and case marking in heritage speakers and L2 learners of Japanese and Korean. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6: 396–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leonard, Laurence B. 1998. Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard, Laurence B. 2014. Children with Specific Language Impairment, 2nd ed. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard, Laurence B., and Umberta Bortolini. 1998. Grammatical morphology and the role of weak syllables in the speech of Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41: 1363–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, Laurence B., and Marco Dispaldro. 2013. The effects of production demands on grammatical weakness in Specific Language Impairment: The case of clitic pronouns in Italian. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 56: 1272–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leonini, Chiara. 2006a. Object clitics and determiners in the acquisition of Italian as L1 and L2. In Language Acquisition and Development. Proceedings of Gala 2005. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Press, pp. 343–48. [Google Scholar]
- Leonini, Chiara. 2006b. The Acquisition of Object Clitics and Definite Articles. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Siena, Firenze, Italy. [Google Scholar]
- Madsen, Christen N., II. 2018. De-Centering the Monolingual: A Psychophysiological Study of Heritage Speaker Language Processing. Ph.D. dissertation, City University of New York, New York, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Marian, Viorica, Henrike K. Blumenfeld, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2007. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50: 940–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McKee, Cecile. 1992. A comparison of pronouns and anaphors in Italian and English acquisition. Language Acquisition 2: 21–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montgomery, James W. 2003. Working memory and comprehension in children with specific language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders 36: 221–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2008. Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-Examining the Age Factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2016. The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller, Natascha, and Aafke Hulk. 2001. Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Padilla José, A. 1990. On the Definition of Binding Domains in Spanish: Evidence from Child Language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2004. On the relevance of specific language impairment to understanding the role of transfer in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 25: 67–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2010. The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Keynote article for special issue with peer commentaries. Applied Psycholinguistics 31: 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Johanne, and Martha Crago. 2000. Tense and Temporality: A Comparison between Children Learning a Second Language and Children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43: 834–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirce, Jonathan, Jeremy R. Gray, Sol Simpson, Michael MacAskill, Richard Höchenberger, Hiroyuki Sogo, Erik Kastman, and Jonas Kristoffer Lindeløv. 2019. PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods 51: 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Polinsky, Maria. 2011. Reanalysis in adult heritage language: New evidence in support of attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33: 305–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Polinsky, Maria. 2018. Heritage Languages and Their Speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, Maria, and Gregory Scontras. 2020. Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 15 May 2020).
- Rice, Mabel L. 2004. Growth Models of Developmental Language Disorders. In Developmental Language Disorders: From Phenotypes to Etiologies. Edited by Mabel L. Rice and Steven F. Warren. Mahwah: Erlbaum, pp. 214–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, Mabel L., and Kenneth Wexler. 1996. Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 39: 1239–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, Vivienne. 2010. Syntactic Development in the Second Language Acquisition of French by Instructed English Learners. Ph.D. dissertation, Newcastle University, Newcastle, UK. Available online: http://theses.ncl.ac.uk/jspui/handle/10443/3168 (accessed on 12 February 2022).
- Romano, Francesco. 2020. Ultimate attainment in heritage language speakers: Syntactic and morphological knowledge of Italian accusative clitics. Applied Psycholinguistics 41: 347–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Romano, Francesco. 2021. L1 versus Dominant Language Transfer Effects in L2 and Heritage Speakers of Italian: A Structural Priming Study. Applied Linguistics 42: 945–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rothman, Jason. 2009. Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism 13: 155–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schaeffer, Jeanette C. 2000. The Acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling and Clitic Placement: Syntax and Pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, vol. 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmid, Monika S. 2011. Language Attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Available online: https://www.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=hZCXdKKuGX0C&oi=fnd&pg=PR7&dq=Schmid,+Monika+S.+2011.+Language+Attrition.+Cambridge+University+Press&ots=bu7Wgw6Ek9&sig=Bo597fhjSgDNpAufD7-2ew6rlkI (accessed on 12 February 2022).
- Schmitz, Katrin, and Anna-Lena Scherger. 2019. Effects of age and education on variable but native heritage grammars: Theoretical and empirical implications for the Null Subject Parameter. Applied Linguistics Review 10: 443–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmitz, Katrin, Laura Di Venanzio, and Anna-Lena Scherger. 2016. Null and overt subjects in Italian and Spanish heritage speakers in Germany. Lingua 180: 101–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scontras, Gregory, Maria Polinsky, and Zuzanna Fuchs. 2018. In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3: 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Serratrice, Ludovica, Antonella Sorace, and Sandra Paoli. 2004. Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax–pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English–Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 183–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sorace, Antonella. 2011. Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1: 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorace, Antonella, and Francesca Filiaci. 2006. Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research 22: 339–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tedeschi, Roberta. 2008. Referring expressions in early Italian: A study on the use of lexical objects, pronouns and null objects in Italian pre-school children. LOT Occasional Series 8: 201. [Google Scholar]
- Thordardottir, Elin. 2015. Proposed diagnostic procedures for use in bilingual and cross-linguistic contexts. In Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Edited by Sharon Armon-Lotem, Jan de Jong and Natalia Meir. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 331–58. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin, Bruce J., Nancy L. Records, Paula Buckwalter, Xuyang Zhang, Elaine Smith, and Marlea O’Brien. 1997. Prevalence of Specific Language Impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 40: 1245–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tuller, Laurice, Hélène Delage, Cécile Monjauze, Anne-Gaëlle Piller, and Marie-Anne Barthez. 2011. Clitic pronoun production as a measure of atypical language development in French. Lingua 121: 423–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuller, Laurice, Cornelia Hamann, Solveig Chilla, Sandrine Ferré, Eléonore Morin, Philippe Prevost, Christophe Dos Santos, Lina Abed Ibrahim, and Racha Zebib. 2018. Identifying language impairment in bilingual children in France and in Germany. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 53: 888–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vender, Maria, Maria Garraffa, Antonella Sorace, and Maria T. Guasti. 2016. How early L2 children perform on Italian clinical markers of SLI: A study of clitic production and nonword repetition. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 30: 150–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vernice, Mirta, Fabrizio Arosio, Chiara Branchini, Lina Barbieri, Elena Roncaglione, Eleonora Carravieri, Heather KJ Van Der Lely, and Maria Teresa Guasti. 2013. CLAD-ITA GAPS: Un test di screening delle abilità fonologiche e morfo-sintattiche in bambini con un Disturbo Specifico del Linguaggio. Psicologia clinica Dello Sviluppo 17: 291–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wexler, Kenneth. 1994. Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In Verb Movement. Edited by David Lightfoot and Norbert Hornstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 305–50. [Google Scholar]
- White, Lydia. 1996. Clitics in L2 French. In Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. Edited by Harald Clahsen. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 335–68. [Google Scholar]
- Yip, Virginia, and Stephen Matthews. 2000. Syntactic transfer in a Cantonese–English bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3: 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zirnstein, Megan, Kinsey Bice, and Judith Kroll. 2019. Variation in language experience shapes the consequences of bilingualism. In Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and Insights. Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 35–47. [Google Scholar]
Target (SD) | Incorrect Clitic (SD) | |
---|---|---|
Albanian | 0.48 (0.50) | 0.33 (0.47) |
Arabic | 0.29 (0.45) | 0.24 (0.42) |
Romanian | 0.51 (0.50) | 0.29 (0.45) |
Monolingual | 0.73 (0.44) | 0.16 (0.36) |
Number | Age | AoE | Level of Education | Formal Education in IT | Years in the UK | TROG-2 | COMPRENDO | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adult Immigrants (AI) | 29 | 39.31 (11.76) | birth | Higher: 29 | 16.18 (2.5) | 15.25 (8.9) | 77.6/80 (2.02) | 28.7/30 (0.2) |
Heritage speakers (HS) | 30 | 35.7 (12.29) | birth | Secondary: 10, Higher: 20 | 0.04 (0.19) | 35.4 (11.98) | 79.1/80 (0.99) | 28.1/30 (0.22) |
Clitic Production | NWR | |
---|---|---|
AI | 5.6/7 (0.40) | 59.7/60 (0.69) |
HS | 2.5/7 (0.48) | 58.4/60 (1.9) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Smith, G.; Spelorzi, R.; Sorace, A.; Garraffa, M. Language Competence in Italian Heritage Speakers: The Contribution of Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition. Languages 2022, 7, 180. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030180
Smith G, Spelorzi R, Sorace A, Garraffa M. Language Competence in Italian Heritage Speakers: The Contribution of Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition. Languages. 2022; 7(3):180. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030180
Chicago/Turabian StyleSmith, Giuditta, Roberta Spelorzi, Antonella Sorace, and Maria Garraffa. 2022. "Language Competence in Italian Heritage Speakers: The Contribution of Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition" Languages 7, no. 3: 180. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030180
APA StyleSmith, G., Spelorzi, R., Sorace, A., & Garraffa, M. (2022). Language Competence in Italian Heritage Speakers: The Contribution of Clitic Pronouns and Nonword Repetition. Languages, 7(3), 180. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030180