Next Article in Journal
Association of Computed Tomography Perfusion Parameters with 90-Day Functional Independence After Endovascular Thrombectomy
Previous Article in Journal
Antifungal Drug Efficacy Profiles Against Vaginal Candida albicans: A Multi-Drug Comparative Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleep Quality, Pain, Worry, and Rumination in Fibromyalgia: Results from Mediation Analyses

1
ISAL Foundation, Institute for Research on Pain, 47921 Rimini, Italy
2
Cognitive Psychotherapy School and Research Center, Studi Cognitivi, 20121 Milan, Italy
3
Centre of Reference for Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (Italian National Institute of Health), 00161 Rome, Italy
4
Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University, 20143 Milan, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2025, 14(20), 7267; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm14207267
Submission received: 29 August 2025 / Revised: 7 October 2025 / Accepted: 13 October 2025 / Published: 15 October 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Clinical Neurology)

Abstract

Background/Objectives: Fibromyalgia (FM) is a chronic pain syndrome frequently associated with severe pain, sleep disturbances, worry, and depressive rumination. Although previous studies have shown links among these factors, no study has specifically examined the mediating role of sleep disturbances in the relationship between forms of Repetitive Negative Thinking (i.e., worry and rumination) and pain intensity. This study aimed to investigate whether sleep disturbances mediate the relationship between: (1) worry and pain intensity and (2) depressive rumination and pain intensity. Methods: An online cross-sectional survey was conducted with a sample of 867 Italian adults who reported having received an FM diagnosis from a rheumatologist or pain physician. After screening, 733 participants (97.3% female; mean age = 51.0 ± 9.95 years) were included in the analyses. Participants completed the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, the Ruminative Response Scale, the Brief Pain Inventory, and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Mediation analyses were performed using Hayes’ PROCESS macro (Model 4). Results: Depressive rumination was associated with pain intensity both directly (B = 0.021, 95% Confidence Intervals [CIs] 0.012, 0.030) and indirectly through sleep disturbances (B = 0.014, 95% CIs 0.010, 0.020), indicating partial mediation. In contrast, worry showed no direct effect on pain intensity (B = 0.011, 95% CIs −0.003, 0.025) but demonstrated a significant indirect effect via sleep disturbances (B = 0.018, 95% CIs 0.012, 0.025), consistent with full mediation. Conclusions: Pain intensity, sleep quality, worry, and depressive rumination are interrelated in FM. Depressive rumination plays a particularly strong role in pain perception, independent of sleep quality. Interventions that integrate cognitive–behavioral and metacognitive strategies with sleep-focused treatments may help improve both sleep and pain outcomes in individuals with FM.

1. Introduction

Chronic pain is a major public health concern in Western countries, with point prevalence estimates ranging from 12% to 48% of the adult population [1,2]. It encompasses a wide range of clinical conditions, including migraine, vulvodynia, low back pain, and other chronic pain syndromes [3,4]. Chronic pain imposes a substantial burden on affected individuals and is associated with high healthcare utilization and indirect costs, such as work disability and reduced quality of life [5].
Fibromyalgia (FM) is among the most disabling chronic pain syndromes [6]. It affects between 0.2% and 6.6% of the general population, predominantly women, with prevalence increasing with age [7,8]. Its clinical impact is amplified by diagnostic challenges, limited treatment options, and frequent social invalidation [9,10]. According to the 2016 American College of Rheumatology criteria [11], FM is primarily characterized by widespread pain lasting more than three months, often accompanied by other symptoms such as fatigue, cognitive difficulties, and depression, which further complicate the clinical picture [9].
Beyond persistent pain, one of the most distressing and distinctive symptoms of FM is sleep disturbance [12,13,14]. Compared to healthy controls, individuals with FM report poorer sleep quality and efficiency, reflected in longer wake times after sleep onset, shorter sleep duration, and lighter sleep in objective assessments, as well as greater difficulties initiating sleep in subjective reports [13]. In a longitudinal study, Bigatti et al. [14] found that baseline sleep disturbances predicted pain one year later, suggesting that sleep plays a pivotal role in the exacerbation of FM symptoms. Evidence also points to sleep dysfunction as a potential pathogenic factor in FM. Experimental studies have shown that sleep deprivation can induce FM-like symptoms in healthy individuals and impair descending pain-inhibitory pathways. Moreover, clinical trials have demonstrated that improving sleep quality leads to reductions in pain [15]. These findings highlight the importance of identifying sleep disturbances and the factors that contribute to poor sleep in individuals with FM, to guide targeted interventions aimed at improving sleep quality.
According to Harvey’s Cognitive Model of Insomnia [16], excessive and negatively valanced cognitive activity can heighten pre-sleep arousal and increase selective attention to internal threat cues (e.g., somatic tension, intrusive thoughts), thereby delaying sleep onset. This delay reinforces the perception of poor sleep quality, which in turn represents a stressor maintaining the cycle of disturbed sleep. Two common forms of heightened cognitive activity are worry and rumination. Worry refers to a chain of uncontrollable thoughts, images, and doubts concerning potential future threats [17]. Rumination, on the other hand, involves a passive and repetitive focus on the causes, implications, and consequences of past stressful events and negative emotions, rather than on their solutions [18]. Worry and rumination are commonly conceptualized as forms of Repetitive Negative Thinking (RNT) [19,20]. Both processes have been observed across a range of psychological and physical health conditions [21,22,23,24,25,26,27].
A recent meta-analysis revealed a consistent association between higher RNT (i.e., worry and rumination) and poorer sleep quality in non-clinical populations [28]. In the context of chronic pain, however, relatively few studies have examined the role of worry and rumination in sleep disturbances. In a sample of patients with benign chronic pain, Smith et al. [29] reported that pre-sleep pain-related thoughts were significantly associated with sleep continuity, independent of depression and nightly pain severity. In a study of adolescents with chronic pain, Palermo et al. [30] found that bedtime worry significantly predicted self-reported sleep quality. Moreover, in a sample of patients with myofascial temporomandibular disorder, Buenaver et al. [31] observed that the rumination component of catastrophizing (i.e., worrying about pain with an inability to shift attention away from pain-related thoughts [32]) had a significant indirect effect on pain intensity through sleep disturbance. This effect was not observed for the helplessness or magnification components of catastrophizing. Qualitative evidence further supports these findings. In a study by Edwards et al. [33], over one-quarter of participants spontaneously reported experiencing nocturnal rumination. Many described being unable to “switch off” when trying to fall asleep, with some even ruminating about their inability to sleep. Notably, approximately 30% of participants indicated that rumination negatively impacted their sleep, primarily by delaying sleep onset.
Taken together, these findings underscore the role of RNT processes, such as worry and rumination, in disrupting sleep among individuals with chronic pain. Importantly, some evidence also suggests that worry and rumination may exacerbate pain itself, contributing to greater pain intensity by heightening attentional focus on pain-related cues and sustaining negative affective states [34,35,36,37,38]. However, other studies have reported no significant correlation between pain intensity and worry or rumination [39,40]. These inconsistencies may reflect methodological differences, such as variability in sample characteristics or measurement instruments.
Understanding these relationships is critical for identifying modifiable targets for interventions aimed at reducing both pain and sleep disturbances in individuals with FM. Despite growing research on the complex interplay between pain intensity, sleep quality, worry, and rumination, a critical gap remains: to date, no study has systematically examined whether sleep quality mediates the associations between these distinct RNT processes and pain intensity.
The present study aimed to address this gap by investigating two separate mediation models in a large sample of individuals with FM. Specifically, we examined whether sleep quality mediate the relationship between: (1) worry and pain intensity, and (2) depressive rumination and pain intensity. We hypothesized that both worry and depressive rumination would be indirectly associated with higher pain intensity through increased sleep disturbances.

2. Materials and Methods

This cross-sectional study involved a convenient sample of adults with FM (n = 867) recruited online in March 2024 through the Facebook Page of “Comitato Fibromialgici Uniti-Italia Odv”, an Italian association of FM patients.
To be eligible, participants had to be aged 18 years or older and report having received a diagnosis of FM from a rheumatologist or pain physician.
The Ethics Committee of the Sigmund Freud University (Milan, Italy) approved the study (Ref. ID22N2WHC11M1V90684, 20 February 2024). Study objectives and procedures were explained to participants, who provided informed consent before starting the online survey. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964, as revised in October 2024).

2.1. Measures

The online survey collected information on socio-demographic characteristics, FM diagnosis, time lag between symptoms onset and FM diagnosis, pain duration, and perceived effectiveness of current treatments. Treatment effectiveness was assessed using a single 11-point Numerical Rating Scale: “If you take medications or undergo therapies for pain, how effective do you consider them?”. Responses ranged from 0 (not effective at all) to 10 (completely effective).
Worry was measured using the Italian version of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ) [41]. The PSWQ is a 16-item self-report measure that assesses the general tendency to worry, focusing on its pervasiveness, persistence, and uncontrollability, rather than on the specific content of anxious thoughts (e.g., Item 7: “I’m always worrying about something”). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“Not at all typical of me”) to 5 (“Very typical of me”). Total scores range from 16 to 80, with higher scores indicating greater levels of worry. In the present study, the PSWQ demonstrated excellent internal consistency (α = 0.92).
Depressive rumination was assessed using the Italian version of the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) [42]. This 22-item self-report questionnaire, based on the Response Style Theory, conceptualizes rumination as a passive and repetitive focus on depressive symptoms, their causes, and consequences. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). Total scores range from 22 to 88, with higher scores indicating greater levels of depressive rumination. In the present study, the RRS showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.94).
Pain intensity was measured using the pain intensity subscale of the Italian version of the Brief Pain Inventory (BPI) [43], a widely used multidimensional instrument for assessing pain. The subscale includes four items: (1) worst pain in the past 24 h, (2) least pain in the past 24 h, (3) average pain in the past 24 h, and (4) current pain. Each item is rated on a numerical scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (pain as bad as you can imagine). Higher scores indicate greater pain intensity. The total pain intensity score was calculated by averaging the four items, yielding a score from 0 to 10. In the present study, internal consistency for the pain intensity subscale was good (α = 0.87).
Sleep quality were assessed using the Italian version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality index (PSQI) [44]. The PSQI consists of 19 items and five additional questions used for clinical purposes. The questionnaire evaluates various aspects of sleep, including duration, latency, frequency, and severity of sleep problems. Each item contributes to one of seven subscales, scored from 0 to 3, with a global score ranging from 0 to 21. Higher scores indicate poorer sleep quality (to improve clarity in the Results section, we refer to higher PSQI scores as “sleep disturbances”, emphasizing that higher values reflect worse sleep quality). In the internal consistency analysis, the subscale “Use of sleep medication” showed a low corrected item–total correlation (0.104). Including this subscale yielded a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.62, whereas removing it, alpha increased to 0.70, indicating acceptable internal consistency for the remaining subscales. Consequently, the subscale was excluded from the calculation of the total PSQI score to enhance reliability. Similar issues with this subscale have been reported in previous studies, suggesting that it often performs differently from the other PSQI components across various populations [45,46].

2.2. Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to summarize participants’ demographic and pain-related characteristics. Missing data were first examined by calculating the percentage of missing responses for each item. When the percentage of missing values was below 10%, missing data were imputed using the item mean. Cases with more than 10% missing data were excluded through listwise deletion [47]. Preliminary checks for normality were performed by inspecting skewness and kurtosis, with acceptable values ranging between −1 and +1. Pearson’s correlations were calculated among the study variables. Associations between pain intensity (outcome) and potential covariates (e.g., age, perceived treatment efficacy) were also examined. For variables with non-normal distributions, Spearman’s rho was used instead. For the interpretation of correlation magnitudes, we followed the guidelines of Gignac and Szodorai [48], who recommend considering correlations of 0.10, 0.20, and 0.30 as relatively small, typical, and relatively large, respectively. Variables showing a significant correlation with either the mediators or the outcome (r ≥ 0.30) were included as covariates [49]. Multivariate outliers were identified using Mahalanobis distance, applying a chi-square cut-off of p < 0.001 [50]. Multicollinearity was assessed through the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF), with values below 5 considered acceptable [51].
Mediation analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics version 25.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) using the PROCESS macro (model 4; Version 4.1). Two mediation models were tested to examine whether sleep quality mediated the relationships between (1) worry and pain intensity, and between (2) depressive rumination and pain intensity. Indirect effects were estimated using bootstrapping with 5000 resamples. A 95% confidence intervals (CIs) that did not include zero was considered as evidence of a statistically significant indirect effect [52]. All other analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics version 25.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). All statistical tests were two-tailed and results were considered significant at p < 0.05.

3. Results

Of the 867 questionnaires initially collected, 89 were excluded due to more than 10% missing responses on PSQI, resulting in a valid response rate of 89.7%. A comparison between respondents and non-respondents revealed no significant differences in age, pain duration, perceived treatment effectiveness, or worry. However, significant differences emerged in pain intensity (F = 14.614, p < 0.001) and depressive rumination (F = 7.603, p < 0.01).
Among the 778 participants with valid responses, 734 (94.3%) reported having received a FM diagnosis from either a rheumatologist or a pain physician and were therefore considered eligible for inclusion in the study. The final sample thus comprised 734 participants.
Table 1 summarizes the sociodemographic and pain-related characteristics of the final sample.

3.1. Preliminary Analysis

A multivariate outlier was identified and removed from the database, resulting in a final working sample of 733 participants. The ranges of skewness (−0.44 to 0.20) and kurtosis (−0.60 to 0.30) for all study variables supported the assumption of normality, except for pain duration, which showed a non-normal distribution (skewness = 1.332; kurtosis = 1.854). VIF values (1.077–1.56) were below the recommended threshold, indicating no issues with multicollinearity.
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among the study variables. In the correlation analyses, pain intensity showed a relatively small positive correlation with worry (r = 0.142), a relatively typical correlation with depressive rumination (r = 0.279), and a relatively large correlation with sleep disturbances (r = 0.363). Among the cognitive and sleep variables, worry and rumination were strongly correlated (r = 0.600, well above the large threshold), worry and sleep disturbances showed a relatively typical association (r = 0.250), and depressive rumination and sleep disturbances showed a moderately large correlation (r = 0.380). These findings indicate that all variables are positively related, with the strongest links observed between worry and depressive rumination and between depressive rumination and sleep disturbances, highlighting the close interplay among these cognitive and sleep-related processes. Regarding potential covariates: age was negatively correlated with rumination (r = −0.081, p < 0.01; small correlation); perceived treatment effectiveness was negatively correlated with pain intensity (r = −0.195, p < 0.001; small-to-typical correlation), sleep disturbances (r = −0.191, p < 0.001; small-to-typical correlation), worry (r = −0.155, p < 0.001; small correlation), and rumination (r = −0.104, p < 0.001; small correlation); pain duration was positively correlated with sleep disturbances (rho = 0.101, p < 0.01; small correlation). All correlation coefficients were below the cut-off of r = 0.30; therefore, no covariates were included in the mediation models.

3.2. Testing Mediation Models

3.2.1. Simple Mediation with Pain Intensity as the Outcome and Depressive Rumination as the Predictor

In the mediation analysis with depressive rumination as the predictor, higher levels of depressive rumination were directly associated with greater pain intensity. Regarding the indirect pathway, depressive rumination also exerted a significant indirect effect on pain intensity via sleep disturbances. Specifically, higher depressive rumination was linked to more severe sleep disturbances, which, in turn, were associated with higher pain intensity. These findings indicate a partial mediation, suggesting that while depressive rumination directly contributes to pain perception, part of its effect operates through sleep disturbances. Overall, the model explained 8% (R2 = 0.08) of the variance in pain intensity. The detailed regression-based mediation results are reported in Table 3. The total, direct, and indirect effects are presented in Table 4. Standardized path coefficients are shown in Figure 1.

3.2.2. Simple Mediation with Pain Intensity as the Outcome and Worry as the Predictor

In the mediation analysis with worry as the predictor, the direct effect of worry on pain intensity was non-significant. However, worry showed a significant indirect effect on pain intensity via sleep disturbances, indicating full mediation. Specifically, higher levels of worry were associated with more severe sleep disturbances, which, in turn, were linked to higher pain intensity. These results suggest that sleep disturbances fully account for the relationship between worry and pain intensity in this sample. Overall, the model explained 2% (R2 = 0.02) of the variance in pain intensity. The corresponding regression-based mediation results are reported in Table 5. The total, direct, and indirect effects are shown in Table 6. Standardized path coefficients are shown in Figure 2.

4. Discussion

Despite growing evidence on the relationship between pain intensity, sleep quality, worry, and rumination, no study to date has examined whether sleep quality mediates the associations between these RNT processes and pain intensity.
Our findings support Harvey’s Cognitive Model of Insomnia [16], highlighting a pathway in which worry and depressive rumination contribute to increased sleep disturbances. This is consistent with previous research showing that RNT processes interfere with sleep continuity and quality in both clinical and non-clinical populations [28,29,30,31,33]. By demonstrating that these mechanisms also operate in FM, our results extend previous work and underscore the role of maladaptive cognitive activity in perpetuating poor sleep quality in this population.
Furthermore, our results underscore the negative impact of sleep disturbances on pain intensity. This finding aligns with a substantial body of evidence indicating that sleep disturbances are a robust predictor of pain exacerbation across various chronic pain conditions, including FM. Notably, longitudinal and micro-longitudinal studies have shown that sleep impairments more consistently predict subsequent pain than the reverse, supporting the primacy of sleep dysfunction in the pain–sleep relationship [53]. Several mechanisms may explain why poor sleep quality has such a strong effect on pain. Mediation studies have highlighted the roles of depression, anxiety, attention to pain, pain helplessness, stress, fatigue, and reduced physical activity as important factors linking sleep disturbances to greater pain intensity [54]. At the neurobiological level, preliminary evidence suggests that alterations in dopaminergic and opioidergic systems may contribute to this relationship. However, these mechanisms remain incompletely understood and require further investigation [53].
Another important finding of the present study concerns the direct effect of depressive rumination on pain intensity, independent of sleep disturbances. This suggests that rumination may exacerbate pain not only through its disruptive effects on sleep but also by maintaining negative affective states and attentional biases toward pain-related cues, thereby amplifying the subjective experience of pain. This interpretation is consistent with experimental and neuroimaging evidence indicating that rumination intensifies attentional and anticipatory processes related to pain. For instance, Brookes et al. [55] demonstrated that experimentally induced rumination increased both distress and pain perception during an acute pain task, partly by promoting a vigilance-avoidance attentional pattern toward pain-related stimuli. Similarly, Kokonyei et al. [56] found that trait rumination was associated with heightened neural responses during both pain anticipation and perception, as well as with impaired detection of unexpected relief, suggesting that rumination may intensify pain through altered cognitive and emotional processing. Collectively, these findings reinforce the notion that depressive rumination is a cognitive factor that directly exacerbates pain perception, highlighting its relevance as a potential target for psychological interventions in chronic pain management.
From a clinical perspective, these findings highlight the importance of interventions specifically aimed at reducing RNT, which appear to play a role in disrupting sleep and amplifying pain in FM. Metacognitive Therapy (MCT) [57] and Rumination-Focused Cognitive–Behavioral Therapy (RF-CBT) [20] represent promising approaches. Specifically, MCT focuses on identifying and modifying dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs (e.g., “My worrying is uncontrollable”, “Worrying helps me to cope…”) that perpetuate worry and rumination. In doing so, it helps individuals disengage attention from intrusive thoughts and RNT, rather than trying to challenge or suppress their content. MCT has demonstrated efficacy in treating a range of emotional disorders, including depressive disorders, generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, and other conditions characterized by excessive worry and rumination [58,59,60,61]. These findings suggest that MCT could be adapted to address similar cognitive processes in FM, although it has not yet been tested in this specific population.
Similarly, RF-CBT specifically addresses depressive rumination by helping individuals recognize and modify unhelpful thinking styles, such as abstract, repetitive, and passive thought patterns, and replace them with more concrete and constructive coping strategies. This intervention has been shown to reduce depressive rumination and improve emotional functioning in individuals with depression [20]. However, to date, no randomized controlled trials have tested this intervention in patients with FM or chronic pain.
Although these approaches have not yet been tested in FM or chronic pain populations, their theoretical framework directly target the cognitive mechanisms identified in this study. Integrating them into multimodal pain management programs could potentially improve outcomes by breaking the cycle of negative thinking, poor sleep, and heightened pain perception.
In addition, mindfulness-based interventions have demonstrated positive effects on both sleep quality and emotional regulation in chronic pain populations, including FM. A recent study showed that a mindfulness-based stress reduction program improved sleep quality over time [62]. This benefit may result from reducing physiological and cognitive arousal while promoting present-moment awareness and non-judgmental acceptance, thereby counteracting rumination and worry that frequently disrupt sleep.
Future clinical trials are needed to evaluate whether interventions explicitly targeting rumination and worry can improve sleep quality and, consequently, pain outcomes. If confirmed, this would broaden the current range of psychological treatments for chronic pain beyond traditional CBT approaches, opening avenues for tailored interventions addressing transdiagnostic cognitive mechanisms such as RNT.
This study has several limitations. First, the cross-sectional design precludes causality inferences. Future longitudinal studies could provide a dynamic understanding of changes in sleep, cognitive processes, and pain over time, allowing stronger conclusions regarding causal relationships. Second, online data collection through FM-specific patients associations, while cost-effective, may have introduced selection bias and reduced sample representativeness. Individuals without Internet access or with limited digital literacy were likely excluded, and the sample may have been biased toward more engaged participants, limiting generalizability. Third, the lack of control over the testing environment inherent to online surveys may have affected response quality. Future studies could consider diversified recruitment strategies and incorporate attention checks or social desirability measures to improve data quality. Fourth, the internal consistency of the PSQI was lower than typically recommended. In line with prior research [63,64,65,66], one subscale with a particularly low item–total correlation was excluded to improve reliability. While this step strengthened the measure’s internal consistency, it may have reduced comparability with studies using the full scale. Fifth, a preliminary comparison between respondents and non-respondents revealed significant differences in pain intensity and depressive rumination. This suggests a potential response bias, with participants who completed the survey possibly experiencing higher levels of pain and rumination, further limiting generalizability. Sixth, the proposed mediation models accounted for a small proportion of variance in pain intensity. This is not be surprising, since FM pain is a multifaceted phenomenon influenced by numerous biopsychosocial factors [67]. While rumination and worry contribute to sleep disturbances and pain, they are insufficient to fully explain their variability. Other factors, such as physical activity, comorbid mood or anxiety disorders, and pain coping strategies, may also play important roles and should be considered in future research. Moreover, hormonal, neurochemical, and inflammatory changes may further contribute to the complexity of sleep disturbances and pain mechanisms in FM [68,69,70,71,72]. The absence of a healthy control group or other clinical comparison cohorts represents a further limitation of the present study. Therefore, the specificity of these findings to FM cannot be determined, and future studies should include healthy controls and clinical groups with other chronic pain or affective conditions to disentangle the FM-specific versus transdiagnostic nature of these associations. This could be particularly relevant considering that previous research have shown that certain forms of RNT (specifically, worry and anger rumination) may be more pronounced in individuals with FM compared to both healthy controls and patients with other rheumatologic conditions [73]. Moreover, the mean PSQI global score observed in our sample (M = 13.45) was substantially higher than the clinical cutoff of 5 and also exceeded the mean value reported in a comparative study involving six medical populations (M = 8.0) [74]. Finally, since anxiety and depression are common comorbidities in FM and were not controlled in the present analyses. We avoided adding further self-report measures to limit participant burden and maximize data quality, considering the frequent attention and concentration difficulties reported by people with FM. However, future research should include these variables to clarify their potential confounding roles.
In conclusion, our findings extend current knowledge on the interplay among worry, rumination, sleep quality, and pain in FM. They emphasize the importance of targeting worry and rumination in psychological interventions aimed at improving sleep quality and pain intensity. Future research should investigate whether reducing these RNT processes can concurrently improve sleep and pain outcomes. If confirmed, such findings could inform the development of more tailored and mechanistically driven treatments for FM and other chronic pain populations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.T. and V.T.; methodology, M.T., W.R. and S.P.; validation, C.F. and E.M.; formal analysis, M.T. and C.F.; investigation, M.B., V.B., N.B., L.M., G.P. and L.S.; data curation, M.B., V.B., N.B., L.M., G.P. and L.S.; writing—original draft preparation, M.B., V.B., N.B., L.M., G.P. and L.S.; writing—review and editing, M.T., V.T., C.F., E.M. and S.P.; supervision, W.R. and V.T.; funding acquisition, W.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Sigmund Freud University, Milan, Italy (Ref. ID22N2WHC11M1V90684, approved on 2 February 2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors express their gratitude to Comitato Fibromialgici Uniti-Italia Odv and to all the fibromyalgia patients who generously participated in the online survey.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
BPIBrief Pain Inventory
FMFibromyalgia
PSQIPittsburgh Sleep Quality Index
PSWQPenn State Worry Questionnaire
RNTRepetitive Negative Thinking
RRSRuminative Response Scale
SDStandard Deviation

References

  1. Yong, R.J.; Mullins, P.M.; Bhattacharyya, N. Prevalence of Chronic Pain among Adults in the United States. Pain 2022, 163, e328–e332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Rometsch, C.; Martin, A.; Junne, F.; Cosci, F. Chronic Pain in European Adult Populations: A Systematic Review of Prevalence and Associated Clinical Features. Pain 2025, 166, 719–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Raffaeli, W.; Tenti, M.; Corraro, A.; Malafoglia, V.; Ilari, S.; Balzani, E.; Bonci, A. Chronic Pain: What Does It Mean? A Review on the Use of the Term Chronic Pain in Clinical Practice. J. Pain Res. 2021, 14, 827–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Treede, R.-D.; Rief, W.; Barke, A.; Aziz, Q.; Bennett, M.I.; Benoliel, R.; Cohen, M.; Evers, S.; Finnerup, N.B.; First, M.B.; et al. A Classification of Chronic Pain for ICD-11. Pain 2015, 156, 1003–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Dueñas, M.; Ojeda, B.; Salazar, A.; Mico, J.A.; Failde, I. A Review of Chronic Pain Impact on Patients, Their Social Environment and the Health Care System. J. Pain Res. 2016, 9, 457–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bucourt, E.; Martaillé, V.; Goupille, P.; Joncker-Vannier, I.; Huttenberger, B.; Réveillère, C.; Mulleman, D.; Courtois, A.R. A Comparative Study of Fibromyalgia, Rheumatoid Arthritis, Spondyloarthritis, and Sjögren’s Syndrome; Impact of the Disease on Quality of Life, Psychological Adjustment, and Use of Coping Strategies. Pain Med. 2021, 22, 372–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Marques, A.P.; de Sousa do Espírito Santo, A.; Berssaneti, A.A.; Matsutani, L.A.; Yuan, S.L.K. Prevalence of fibromyalgia: Literature review update. Rev. Bras. Reum. Engl. Ed. 2017, 57, 356–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Häuser, W.; Ablin, J.; Fitzcharles, M.-A.; Littlejohn, G.; Luciano, J.V.; Usui, C.; Walitt, B. Fibromyalgia. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2015, 1, 15022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Siracusa, R.; Paola, R.D.; Cuzzocrea, S.; Impellizzeri, D. Fibromyalgia: Pathogenesis, Mechanisms, Diagnosis and Treatment Options Update. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2021, 22, 3891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ghavidel-Parsa, B.; Bidari, A.; Tohidi, S.; Shenavar, I.; Kazemnezhad Leyli, E.; Hosseini, K.; Khosousi, M.-J. Implication of Invalidation Concept in Fibromyalgia Diagnosis. Clin. Rheumatol. 2021, 40, 2369–2376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wolfe, F.; Clauw, D.J.; Fitzcharles, M.-A.; Goldenberg, D.L.; Häuser, W.; Katz, R.L.; Mease, P.J.; Russell, A.S.; Russell, I.J.; Walitt, B. 2016 Revisions to the 2010/2011 Fibromyalgia Diagnostic Criteria. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 2016, 46, 319–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Roizenblatt, S.; Neto, N.S.R.; Tufik, S. Sleep Disorders and Fibromyalgia. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 2011, 15, 347–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wu, Y.-L.; Chang, L.-Y.; Lee, H.-C.; Fang, S.-C.; Tsai, P.-S. Sleep Disturbances in Fibromyalgia: A Meta-Analysis of Case-Control Studies. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017, 96, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Bigatti, S.M.; Hernandez, A.M.; Cronan, T.A.; Rand, K.L. Sleep Disturbances in Fibromyalgia Syndrome: Relationship to Pain and Depression. Arthritis Rheum. 2008, 59, 961–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Choy, E.H.S. The Role of Sleep in Pain and Fibromyalgia. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 2015, 11, 513–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Harvey, A.G. A Cognitive Model of Insomnia. Behav. Res. Ther. 2002, 40, 869–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Borkovec, T.D.; Robinson, E.; Pruzinsky, T.; DePree, J.A. Preliminary Exploration of Worry: Some Characteristics and Processes. Behav. Res. Ther. 1983, 21, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Responses to Depression and Their Effects on the Duration of Depressive Episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1991, 100, 569–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ehring, T.; Watkins, E. Repetitive Negative Thinking as a Transdiagnostic Process. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 2008, 1, 192–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Watkins, E.R. Rumination-Focused Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Depression; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  21. Palmieri, S.; Mansueto, G.; Scaini, S.; Fiore, F.; Sassaroli, S.; Ruggiero, G.M.; Borlimi, R.; Carducci, B.J. Role of Rumination in the Relationship between Metacognition and Shyness. World J. Psychiatry 2018, 8, 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Palmieri, S.; Mansueto, G.; Scaini, S.; Caselli, G.; Sapuppo, W.; Spada, M.M.; Sassaroli, S.; Ruggiero, G.M. Repetitive Negative Thinking and Eating Disorders: A Meta-Analysis of the Role of Worry and Rumination. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 2448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Palmieri, S.; Sassaroli, S.; Ruggiero, G.M.; Caselli, G.; Spada, M.M.; Mansueto, G. Emotion Dysregulation in Patients with Eating Disorders: The Role of Metacognitions and Repetitive Negative Thinking. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2023, 47, 655–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fortune, D.G.; Richards, H.L.; Main, C.J.; Griffiths, C.E.M. Pathological Worrying, Illness Perceptions and Disease Severity in Patients with Psoriasis. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2000, 5, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Marcus, D.; Hughes, K.; Arnau, R. Health Anxiety, Rumination, and Negative Affect: A Mediational Analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 64, 495–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Soo, H.; Burney, S.; Basten, C. The Role of Rumination in Affective Distress in People with a Chronic Physical Illness: A Review of the Literature and Theoretical Formulation. J. Health Psychol. 2009, 14, 956–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hamonniere, T.; Milan, L.; Varescon, I. Repetitive Negative Thinking, Metacognitive Beliefs, and Their Interaction as Possible Predictors for Problematic Cannabis Use. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2022, 29, 706–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Clancy, F.; Prestwich, A.; Caperon, L.; Tsipa, A.; O’Connor, D.B. The Association between Worry and Rumination with Sleep in Non-Clinical Populations: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 427–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Smith, M.T.; Perlis, M.L.; Carmody, T.P.; Smith, M.S.; Giles, D.E. Presleep Cognitions in Patients with Insomnia Secondary to Chronic Pain. J. Behav. Med. 2001, 24, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Palermo, T.; Toliver-Sokol, M.; Fonareva, I.; Koh, J. Objective and Subjective Assessment of Sleep in Adolescents with Chronic Pain Compared to Healthy Adolescents. Clin. J. Pain 2007, 23, 812–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Buenaver, L.; Quartana, P.; Grace, E.; Sarlani, E.; Simango, M.; Edwards, R.; Haythornthwaite, J.; Smith, M. Evidence for Indirect Effects of Pain Catastrophizing on Clinical Pain among Myofascial Temporomandibular Disorder Participants: The Mediating Role of Sleep Disturbance. Pain 2012, 153, 1159–1166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Sullivan, M.; Bishop, S.; Pivik, J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and Validation. Psychol. Assess. 1995, 7, 524–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Edwards, M.J.; Tang, N.K.; Wright, A.M.; Salkovskis, P.M.; Timberlake, C.M. Thinking about Thinking about Pain: A Qualitative Investigation of Rumination in Chronic Pain. Pain Manag. 2011, 1, 311–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lefebvre, J.C.; Jensen, M.P. The Relationships between Worry, Happiness and Pain Catastrophizing in the Experience of Acute Pain. Eur. J. Pain 2019, 23, 1358–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fonseca das Neves, J.; Kornacka, M.; Serra, E.; Rollin, N.; Kosinski, T.; Maréchal, V.; Jehel, L.; Rusinek, S. The Impact of Rumination on Fibromyalgia Pain after Physical Activity: An Experimental Study. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 20523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Tenti, M.; Raffaeli, W.; Fontemaggi, A.; Gremigni, P. The Relationship between Metacognition, Anger, and Pain Intensity among Fibromyalgia Patients: A Serial Mediation Model. Psychol. Health Med. 2024, 29, 791–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lackner, J.; Quigley, B. Pain Catastrophizing Mediates the Relationship between Worry and Pain Suffering in Patients with Irritable Bowel Syndrome. Behav. Res. Ther. 2005, 43, 943–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Davis, C.E.; Stockstill, J.W.; Stanley, W.D.; Wu, Q. Pain-Related Worry in Patients with Chronic Orofacial Pain. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 2014, 145, 722–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Craner, J.R.; Gilliam, W.P.; Sperry, J.A. Rumination, Magnification, and Helplessness: How Do Different Aspects of Pain Catastrophizing Relate to Pain Severity and Functioning? Clin. J. Pain 2016, 32, 1028–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Eccleston, C.; Crombez, G.; Aldrich, S.; Stannard, C. Worry and Chronic Pain Patients: A Description and Analysis of Individual Differences. Eur. J. Pain 2001, 5, 309–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Morani, S.; Pricci, D.; Sanavio, E. Penn State Worry Questionnaire e Worry Domains Questionnaire. Presentazione Delle Versioni Italiane Ed Analisi Della Fedeltà. Psicoter. Cogn. Comport. 1999, 5, 195–209. [Google Scholar]
  42. Palmieri, R.; Gapsarre, A.; Lanciano, T. Una Misura Disposizionale Della Ruminazione Depressiva: La RRS Di Nolen-Hoeksema e Morrow. Psychofenia 2007, 10, 15–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Caraceni, A.; Mendoza, T.R.; Mencaglia, E.; Baratella, C.; Edwards, K.; Forjaz, M.J.; Martini, C.; Serlin, R.C.; de Conno, F.; Cleeland, C.S. A Validation Study of an Italian Version of the Brief Pain Inventory (Breve Questionario per La Valutazione Del Dolore). Pain 1996, 65, 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Curcio, G.; Tempesta, D.; Scarlata, S.; Marzano, C.; Moroni, F.; Rossini, P.M.; Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L. Validity of the Italian Version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Neurol. Sci. 2013, 34, 511–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Wang, L.; Wu, Y.-X.; Lin, Y.-Q.; Wang, L.; Zeng, Z.-N.; Xie, X.-L.; Chen, Q.-Y.; Wei, S.-C. Reliability and Validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index among Frontline COVID-19 Health Care Workers Using Classical Test Theory and Item Response Theory. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2022, 18, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Larche, C.L.; Plante, I.; Roy, M.; Ingelmo, P.M.; Ferland, C.E. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: Reliability, Factor Structure, and Related Clinical Factors among Children, Adolescents, and Young Adults with Chronic Pain. Sleep Disord. 2021, 2021, 5546484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gignac, G.E.; Szodorai, E.T. Effect Size Guidelines for Individual Differences Researchers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 102, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Frigon, J.-Y.; Laurencelle, L. Analysis of Covariance: A Proposed Algorithm. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1993, 53, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics, 3rd ed.; Harper Collins: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E. Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed.; Pearson Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (Methodology in the Social Sciences), 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  53. Finan, P.H.; Goodin, B.R.; Smith, M.T. The Association of Sleep and Pain: An Update and a Path Forward. J. Pain 2013, 14, 1539–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Whibley, D.; AlKandari, N.; Kristensen, K.; Barnish, M.; Rzewuska, M.; Druce, K.L.; Tang, N.K.Y. Sleep and Pain: A Systematic Review of Studies of Mediation. Clin. J. Pain 2019, 35, 544–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Brookes, M.L.; Sharpe, L.; Dear, B.F. Rumination Induces a Pattern of Attention Characterized by Increased Vigilance Followed by Avoidance of Affective Pain Words. Eur. J. Pain 2017, 21, 1197–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Kokonyei, G.; Galambos, A.; Edes, A.; Kocsel, N.; Szabo, E.; Pap, D.; Kozak, L.; Bagdy, G.; Juhasz, G. Anticipation and Violated Expectation of Pain Are Influenced by Trait Rumination: An fMRI Study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 19, 56–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Wells, A. Metacognitive Therapy for Anxiety and Depression; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  58. Callesen, P.; Reeves, D.; Heal, C.; Wells, A. Metacognitive Therapy versus Cognitive Behaviour Therapy in Adults with Major Depression: A Parallel Single-Blind Randomised Trial. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 7878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Papageorgiou, C.; Carlile, K.; Thorgaard, S.; Waring, H.; Haslam, J.; Horne, L.; Wells, A. Group Cognitive-Behavior Therapy or Group Metacognitive Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder? Benchmarking and Comparative Effectiveness in a Routine Clinical Service. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Nordahl, H.M.; Vogel, P.A.; Morken, G.; Stiles, T.C.; Sandvik, P.; Wells, A. Paroxetine, Cognitive Therapy or Their Combination in the Treatment of Social Anxiety Disorder with and without Avoidant Personality Disorder: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 2016, 85, 346–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Nordahl, H.M.; Borkovec, T.D.; Hagen, R.; Kennair, L.E.O.; Hjemdal, O.; Solem, S.; Hansen, B.; Haseth, S.; Wells, A. Metacognitive Therapy versus Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy in Adults with Generalised Anxiety Disorder. BJPsych Open 2018, 4, 393–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Ciacchini, R.; Conversano, C.; Orrù, G.; Scafuto, F.; Sabbatini, S.; Paroli, M.; Miniati, M.; Matiz, A.; Gemignani, A.; Crescentini, C. About Distress in Chronic Pain Conditions: A Pre–Post Study on the Effectiveness of a Mindfulness-Based Intervention for Fibromyalgia and Low Back Pain Patients. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. de la Vega, R.; Tomé-Pires, C.; Solé, E.; Racine, M.; Castarlenas, E.; Jensen, M.P.; Miró, J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: Validity and Factor Structure in Young People. Psychol. Assess. 2015, 27, e22–e27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Benhayon, D.; Youk, A.; McCarthy, F.N.; Davis, S.; Keljo, D.J.; Bousvaros, A.; Fairclough, D.; Kupfer, D.; Buysse, D.J.; Szigethy, E.M. Characterization of Relations among Sleep, Inflammation, and Psychiatric Dysfunction in Depressed Youth with Crohn Disease. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2013, 57, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Tomfohr, L.M.; Schweizer, C.A.; Dimsdale, J.E.; Loredo, J.S. Psychometric Characteristics of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in English Speaking Non-Hispanic Whites and English and Spanish Speaking Hispanics of Mexican Descent. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2013, 9, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Zhou, H.; Han, D.; Zhou, H.; Ke, X.; Jiang, D. Influencing Factors of Psychological Pain among Older People in China: A Cross-Sectional Study. Heliyon 2023, 9, e21141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Turk, D.C.; Adams, L.M. Using a Biopsychosocial Perspective in the Treatment of Fibromyalgia Patients. Pain Manag. 2016, 6, 357–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Irwin, M.R. Sleep and Inflammation: Partners in Sickness and in Health. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 2019, 19, 702–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Chen, S.; Xie, Y.; Liang, Z.; Lu, Y.; Wang, J.; Xing, F.; Mao, Y.; Wei, X.; Wang, Z.; Yang, J.; et al. A Narrative Review of the Reciprocal Relationship Between Sleep Deprivation and Chronic Pain: The Role of Oxidative Stress. J. Pain Res. 2024, 17, 1785–1792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Maurer, A.J.; Lissounov, A.; Knezevic, I.; Candido, K.D.; Knezevic, N.N. Pain and Sex Hormones: A Review of Current Understanding. Pain Manag. 2016, 6, 285–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Staud, R.; Spaeth, M. Psychophysical and Neurochemical Abnormalities of Pain Processing in Fibromyalgia. CNS Spectr. 2008, 13, 12–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Ceko, M.; Bushnell, M.C.; Gracely, R.H. Neurobiology Underlying Fibromyalgia Symptoms. Pain Res. Treat. 2012, 2012, 585419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ricci, A.; Bonini, S.; Continanza, M.; Turano, M.T.; Puliti, E.M.; Finocchietti, A.; Bertolucci, D. Worry and Anger Rumination in Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Reumatismo 2016, 68, 195–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Ebrahimi, A.A.; Ghalebandi, M.; Salehi, M.; Alavi, K. A Comparative Study of the Components of Sleep Quality in Medical Outpatients. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2009, 13, 184–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity through sleep disturbances. Standardized parameter estimates are shown, and the beta for the total effect is reported in parentheses. Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. * p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity through sleep disturbances. Standardized parameter estimates are shown, and the beta for the total effect is reported in parentheses. Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. * p < 0.001.
Jcm 14 07267 g001
Figure 2. Mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity through sleep disturbances. Standardized parameter estimates are shown, and the beta for the total effect is reported in parentheses. Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. * p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity through sleep disturbances. Standardized parameter estimates are shown, and the beta for the total effect is reported in parentheses. Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. * p < 0.001.
Jcm 14 07267 g002
Table 1. Participants’ socio-demographic and pain characteristics (n = 734).
Table 1. Participants’ socio-demographic and pain characteristics (n = 734).
Variablen (%)Mean (SD), Range
Gender
Male19 (2.6)
Female714 (97.3)
Other1 (0.1)
Age (years) 50.92 (9.87), 18–76
Education
Primary to Middle school138 (18.8)
High school386 (52.6)
University degree151 (20.6)
Postgraduate59 (8)
Occupation
Unemployed177 (24.1)
Employed474 (64.6)
Student5 (0.7)
Retired78 (10.6)
Housing situation
Living alone94 (12.8)
Living with parents47 (6.4)
Living with current family582 (79.3)
Living with other non-family people11 (1.5)
Time since FM diagnosis
Less than 1 year101 (13.8)
From 1 to 3 years264 (35.9)
From 4 to 5 years101 (13.8)
From 6 to 10 years96 (13.1)
More than 10 years172 (23.4)
Pain duration (years) (n = 718) 13.86 (10.22), 0–55
Undergoing treatment
Yes574 (78.2)
No160 (21.8)
Perceived treatment effectiveness (n = 620) 4.34 (2.37), 0–10
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among the study variables (n = 733).
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among the study variables (n = 733).
Pearson’s r Correlation
  QuestionnaireMean (SD)1234
  1. Pain intensity6.08 (1.78) 0.142 *0.279 *0.363 *
  2. Worry51.69 (8.75) 0.600 *0.250 *
  3. Depressive rumination53.14 (14.07) 0.380 *
  4. Sleep disturbances13.45 (3.79)
* p < 0.001.
Table 3. Regression-based results of the mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity.
Table 3. Regression-based results of the mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity.
CriterionPredictorsRR2Fβt95% CIs
Sleep disturbancesDepressive rumination0.380.14123.270.1015.79[7.011, 9.002]
Pain intensityDepressive rumination0.390.1566.830.024.47[0.012, 0.030]
Sleep disturbances 0.148.16[0.107, 0.175]
Table 4. Total, direct, and indirect effects in the mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity.
Table 4. Total, direct, and indirect effects in the mediation model with depressive rumination as the predictor of pain intensity.
EffectBSE(β)95% CIsβSE(β)95% CIs
Total effect0.0350.005[0.026, 0.044]0.28
Direct effect0.0210.005[0.012, 0.030]0.16
Indirect effect0.0140.002[0.010, 0.020]0.110.02[0.083, 0.149]
Table 5. Regression-based results of the mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity.
Table 5. Regression-based results of the mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity.
CriterionPredictorsRR2FBt95% CIs
Sleep disturbancesWorry0.250.0648.860.116.99[0.078, 0.139]
Pain intensityWorry0.370.1356.640.011.52[−0.003, 0.025]
Sleep disturbances 0.169.82[0.131, 0.197]
Table 6. Total, direct, and indirect effects in the mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity.
Table 6. Total, direct, and indirect effects in the mediation model with worry as the predictor of pain intensity.
  EffectBSE(B)95% CIsβSE(β)95% CIs
  Total effect0.0290.008[0.014, 0.043]0.14
  Direct effect0.0110.007[−0.003, 0.025]0.05
  Indirect effect0.0180.003[0.012, 0.025]0.090.01[0.060, 0.118]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tenti, M.; Raffaeli, W.; Fagnani, C.; Medda, E.; Basciu, M.; Benassi, V.; Boschetti, N.; Martorana, L.; Palmieri, S.; Panini, G.; et al. Sleep Quality, Pain, Worry, and Rumination in Fibromyalgia: Results from Mediation Analyses. J. Clin. Med. 2025, 14, 7267. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm14207267

AMA Style

Tenti M, Raffaeli W, Fagnani C, Medda E, Basciu M, Benassi V, Boschetti N, Martorana L, Palmieri S, Panini G, et al. Sleep Quality, Pain, Worry, and Rumination in Fibromyalgia: Results from Mediation Analyses. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2025; 14(20):7267. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm14207267

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tenti, Michael, William Raffaeli, Corrado Fagnani, Emanuela Medda, Martina Basciu, Valentina Benassi, Noemi Boschetti, Lorelay Martorana, Sara Palmieri, Giorgia Panini, and et al. 2025. "Sleep Quality, Pain, Worry, and Rumination in Fibromyalgia: Results from Mediation Analyses" Journal of Clinical Medicine 14, no. 20: 7267. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm14207267

APA Style

Tenti, M., Raffaeli, W., Fagnani, C., Medda, E., Basciu, M., Benassi, V., Boschetti, N., Martorana, L., Palmieri, S., Panini, G., Scovotto, L., & Toccaceli, V. (2025). Sleep Quality, Pain, Worry, and Rumination in Fibromyalgia: Results from Mediation Analyses. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 14(20), 7267. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm14207267

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop