Next Article in Journal
Nexus between Intellectual Capital and Bank Productivity in India
Next Article in Special Issue
Determinants of Financial Literacy: Analysis of the Impact of Family and Socioeconomic Variables on Undergraduate Students in the Slovak Republic
Previous Article in Journal
Strengthening Formal Credit Access and Performance through Financial Literacy and Credit Terms in Micro, Small and Medium Businesses
Previous Article in Special Issue
Financial Inclusion and Intersectionality: A Case of Business Funding in the South African Informal Sector
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Is More Financial Literacy Always Beneficial? An Investigation through a Mediator

1
Economics Department, Colby College, Waterville, ME 04901, USA
2
Baruch College and Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY 10017, USA
3
Maine Business School, University of Maine, Orono, ME 04469, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Risk Financial Manag. 2023, 16(1), 53; https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm16010053
Submission received: 26 October 2022 / Revised: 31 December 2022 / Accepted: 31 December 2022 / Published: 16 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Financial Literacy and Financial Inclusion)

Abstract

:
We study the impact of financial literacy on financial risk preference. When financial literacy is measured jointly by actual and self-assessed scores, we find compelling evidence of a valley-shaped relationship between actual financial literacy and risk preference. At a given level of self-assessment, as actual financial literacy increases, the willingness to take risks initially decreases and then rises. Actual financial literacy is modeled to impact risk preference through self-assessed financial literacy, the mediator; this mediation effect is significant. Furthermore, increasing actual financial literacy has a positive (negative) effect in underconfident (overconfident) individuals on several financial behaviors.
JEL Classification:
D14; G53

1. Introduction

Financial literacy is the understanding of financial concepts, providing individuals with the skills they need to make informed decisions (Lusardi 2014).1 Many papers document the robust positive influence of financial literacy on financial decisions and outcomes (Lusardi and Mitchell 2007, 2008; Van Rooij et al. 2011a, 2012; Clark et al. 2017; Bannier and Schwarz 2018; Darriet et al. 2020; Fanta and Mutsonziwa 2021). Studies also report positive roles of financial literacy in helping individuals overcome behavioral bias (Jonsson et al. 2017; Baker et al. 2019). Moreover, financial literacy is found to positively affect individuals’ risk taking (e.g., Almenberg and Dreber 2015). Similarly, individuals’ risk aversion to investment is found to drop if they are better able to process and understand financial information (e.g., Riley and Chow 1992).
There are two measures of financial literacy: objective (actual) and subjective (self-perceived). Numerous studies (Lusardi and Mitchell 2007; Van Rooij et al. 2011b; Chatterjee et al. 2017) document the evident perception bias that can be evaluated as perceived minus actual financial literacy. Traditionally, the objective financial literacy is the preferred measure (Van Rooij et al. 2011b; Bucher-Koenen and Lusardi 2011; Boisclair et al. 2017; Jonsson et al. 2017). Recent research has begun to employ both measures. Van Rooij et al. (2012) find a pronounced impact from actual financial literacy, but the effect of perceived literacy is less consistent. Chatterjee et al. (2017) find that both measures contribute, but self-perceived financial literacy is more impactful. Bazley et al. (2020) support the positive influence of both actual literacy and self-belief in one’s literacy on equity ownership. Besides the mixed results, most cited literature devotes little effort to their effects by cohort.2 In the cohort of high perceived but low actual financial literacy (i.e., the cohort with positive perception bias, the overconfident), Allgood and Walstad (2016) conclude that the positive and significant effect of actual financial literacy usually found in other cohorts on many financial decisions turns weak or negligible. This prompts us to explore the interplay of the two measures across cohorts.3
Most of the above studies treat the two measures as separate predictors in one regression equation. Our study questions the ‘equal exogeneity’ of actual and self-assessed financial literacy. Imagine someone improves his or her actual financial knowledge; wouldn’t he or she consequently acquire higher self-assessment? Moreover, even people who are unaware of their actual financial knowledge act on their self-perceived literacy to make financial decisions. In such cases, the coefficient estimates in a single-equation regression may provide an inaccurate assessment of the marginal effect of actual financial literacy on financial outcomes, as the coefficient does not account for the change in self-assessed financial literacy.
Therefore, it is natural to conjecture that actual financial literacy (X) affects self-perceived financial literacy (M), which in turn affects risk preference (Y). There are models hypothesizing a chain of relations between the independent variable (X) and the dependent variable (Y) such that X first causes a change in a variable M, which then causes a change in Y. Used in many disciplines, the X → M → Y model is often termed mediation in psychology (Baron and Kenny 1986). We thus propose self-perceived financial literacy to be the mediator.
Our contribution to financial literacy research is two-fold. First, unlike Allgood and Walstad (2016), who use a single equation approach, we propose an ‘endogenous’ self-perceived financial literacy, and through a two-equation structure, we investigate the inter-connected roles the two measures play regarding risk preference. We find compelling evidence of a valley-shaped relationship: risk preference first declines and then surges as actual financial literacy increases, underpinning the role of perception bias. In addition, we confirm the endogeneity of self-assessed financial literacy, which mediates between actual financial literacy and risk preference. A bootstrapping method proves the significance of the mediation effect. Another approach by Balasubramnian and Sargent (2020) uses the gap between perceived and actual literacy (which are both classified into high, medium, and low) to center their analysis on a particular cohort labeled ‘blind spot’, i.e., the cohort of high perceived but low actual literacy. By regressing on the ‘blind spot’ dummy, perceived literacy, and other covariates, they essentially investigate the effect of actual literacy in the overconfident cohort. Our modeling, in contrast, is flexible enough to account for both overconfident and underconfident groups.
Second, not only are we the first to detect the positive (negative) effect of actual financial literacy in the underconfident (overconfident) cohort on risk preference (thereafter termed ‘dual effects’ of actual financial literacy or ‘duality’), but we also find these dual effects on other financial decisions. Traces of these dual effects have emerged in Allgood and Walstad (2016). Similarly, Anderson et al. (2017) find that precautionary savings and retirement planning decisions are driven by self-perceived financial literacy as opposed to actual financial literacy, particularly among the low-literacy respondents. We further document that the effect of actual financial literacy could be significantly negative on numerous financial behaviors (e.g., credit cards, college savings, emergency funds).4 This duality is more substantial than what prior studies suggest.
In summary, our findings are: (1) the dual effects of actual financial literacy on risk preference related to the over- and underconfident cohorts; (2) the significant mediation effect within the total effect of actual financial literacy on risk preference; and (3) the extension of dual effects of actual financial literacy to other financial behaviors.

2. Materials

The National Financial Capability Study (NFCS) surveyed U.S. households in four waves (2009, 2012, 2015, and 2018), with 25,000 respondents participating in each.5 We provide summary statistics for the key variables in Table 1.
The three core variables are actual financial literacy, self-perceived financial literacy, and risk preference. Actual financial literacy (Actual FL) is measured by the scores on five multiple-choice questions (2009 and 2012) and later by scores on six questions (2015 and 2018). The scores range from 0–5 (2009 and 2012) or 0–6 (2015 and 2018) in integers. The four-wave sample can be used for the original five questions, whereas the two-wave (2015, 2018) sample can be used for the six questions. We refer to them as the two datasets. Participants report (on a 7-point scale) self-perception of financial literacy (Self-Perceived FL) for all four years. Risk preference (on a 10-point scale) is based on self-assessment of willingness to take risks in financial investments.
Financial planning decisions are binary variables based on Yes or No answers to questions or statements: (1) In the past 12 months, I always paid my credit cards in full. (2) Are you setting aside some money for your children’s college education? (3) Have you set aside emergency or rainy-day funds that would cover your expenses in case of sickness, job loss, economic downturn, or other emergency? Controls are mainly demographic variables, such as age, gender, ethnicity, education, income, and employment, all of which take either binary or ordinal categorical values.6
A complete description of all the variables used in our study is presented in Appendix A.

3. Methods and Results

Figure 1 illustrates the triangular mechanism (i.e., the mediation model) through which Actual FL (X) influences Risk Preference (Y) with the mediator Self-Perceived FL (M) (Hayes 2013). We propose a two-equation model (1) and (2) in which Self-Perceived FL (M) is now an endogenous variable predicted by Actual FL (X). The model accounts for the direct effect ( γ 2 ), the mediation effect ( β 2 γ 1 ), and the total effect ( γ 2 + β 2 γ 1 ) of X on Y.
M i = α 1 + γ 1 X i + θ 1 C i + ε 1 i
Y i = α 2 + β 2 M i + γ 2 X i + θ 2 C i + ε 2 i
Equation (1) regresses M i on X i , while controlling for demographic variables and year dummies ( C i ). Equation (2) regresses Y i on M i and X i , along with control variables ( C i ).

3.1. Mediation Effect

Self-assessed financial literacy (7-point scale) and actual financial literacy (5-question scale or 6-question scale) are not directly comparable because of their different scales, but the relative positions of respondents within the distribution of self-assessed literacy and actual literacy do provide information. For instance, an individual with a medium score on actual literacy rating himself/herself a medium score on self-assessed literacy would be considered accurate in self-perception. The percentile distribution in Table 1 shows that a medium individual in actual literacy scores 3 out of 5 questions, while a medium individual in self-assessed literacy rates himself/herself 5 out of 7. The lowest 5% in actual literacy scores 0 out of 5 questions, while the lowest 5% in self-assessment still rate themselves as high as 3 out of 7. As the distribution of actual and self-assessed literacy in Table 1 shows that at almost any percentile the perceived literacy is higher than actual literacy by at least 2 points, we define as the overconfident cohort the subset for which self-assessed FL – actual FL > 2 and as the underconfident cohort the subset for which self-assessed FL – actual FL < 1.7
Table 2 presents Equation (1) estimation results. Columns 2 and 4 reveal that among the underconfident, actual financial literacy along with control variables substantially explain the variation in perceived literacy (0.72 and 0.74 R 2 ), and less so (columns 1 and 3) among the overconfident (0.37 and 0.36 R 2 ). The disparity between the under- and the overconfident becomes overwhelming in the regression results of Equation (2), reported in Table 3. Actual financial literacy has a positive influence on risk preference among the underconfident (columns 3 and 6) but a negative influence among the overconfident (columns 2 and 5), holding self-perceived literacy constant. Table 3 also reports a consistently positive effect of perceived literacy on risk preference.8 Our findings from both Equations (1) and (2) are robust to the two datasets.
If we hold the self-assessment level fixed and consider four individuals, two overconfident and two underconfident, then among the two overconfident (underconfident), the one with higher actual financial literacy is less (more) willing to take risks. The inferred decline and rise in the individual’s risk preference exhibits a valley-shaped relationship, which is also observed at the aggregate levels (see Figure 2).9 Holding self-perception constant, the more overconfident appear to be more risk-loving.
This valley-shaped relationship is the direct effect of actual financial literacy on risk preference. The combination of heightened self-assessment resulting from enhanced actual knowledge captured in Equation (1) and the elevated risk preference fueled by heightened self-assessment captured in Equation (2) funnels the indirect (mediation) effect. The two-equation model (1) and (2) represents the total effect in accordance with Figure 1’s triangle relationship.10
The dual effects of actual financial literacy on risk preference among the over- and underconfident is evident. Nevertheless, additional control variables can be included to check for robustness. Several additional control variables available in the NFCS dataset and commonly used in the literature include a categorical variable that classifies living arrangements (living as the only adult/with spouse/in parents’ home/with friends or roommates), a dummy variable on health insurance (Covered or Not), and a dummy variable on retirement benefits (Have or Not through an employer). Tables S3 and S4 in Supplementary Materials present estimation results when these additional control variables are added. From the highly similar outputs, the disparity between the under- and the overconfident and the dual effects of actual financial literacy are shown to remain robust.
A formal test of the mediation effect is to test the significance of the product of the two coefficients, γ 1 β 2 .11 Table 4 summarizes the results of several tests. Based on 5000 bootstrap replicates, the mediation effect is significant. For instance, the 95% confidence interval of the mediation effect for the underconfident is (0.341, 0.351), accounting for about 71% of the total effect of actual financial literacy on risk preference.12

3.2. Dual Effects of Financial Literacy

Our study is the first to document the dual effects of actual financial literacy on risk preference among the over- and underconfident. This duality is also present with respect to other financial behaviors.
Table 5 presents the output of logistic regressions of binary financial behaviors on financial literacy and controls. Three financial behaviors, (1) credit cards, (2) college savings, and (3) emergency funds, are measured as binary variables that equal 1 if the respondents reply Yes to the following statements or questions (Appendix A): (1) In the past 12 months, I always paid my credit cards in full. (2) Are you setting aside some money for your children’s college education? (3) Have you set aside emergency or rainy-day funds that would cover your expenses in case of sickness, job loss, economic downturn, or other emergency?
Table 5 results support the insignificant effect reported by Allgood and Walstad (2016) but also reveal the negative effect on various financial behaviors for the overconfident cohort. We document the uniformly positive contributions of actual financial literacy on three financial decisions in the underconfident cohort. However, for people of high self-assessment, the contributions turn significantly negative on credit cards and college savings, and insignificant on emergency funds. This is evidence of the pervasiveness of the dual effects.

4. Discussion

We have demonstrated the importance of modeling individuals’ self-perception as a mediator. The mediation effect is significant. Furthermore, increasing actual financial literacy has a positive (negative) effect in underconfident (overconfident) individuals on several financial behaviors. These dual effects should be considered in policymaking.
We start by examining the impact of financial literacy on financial risk preference. In addition to finding support for the positive effect of financial literacy established in previous studies (Lusardi and Mitchell 2007, 2008; Van Rooij et al. 2011a, 2012; Almenberg and Dreber 2015; Bannier and Schwarz 2018; Darriet et al. 2020; Fanta and Mutsonziwa 2021), we discover and identify the negative effect of actual financial literacy among the overconfident cohort. These opposite effects of actual financial literacy in over- and underconfident individuals we call dual effects of actual financial literacy. This duality is hinted at in some previous studies (Allgood and Walstad 2016; Anderson et al. 2017), but we show that it is more substantial than earlier studies admit. Previous studies often investigate the undivided population for the effects of the two measures of financial literacy: actual and self-perceived (Van Rooij et al. 2012; Chatterjee et al. 2017). In contrast, our study clearly differentiates the two cohorts and their effects.
Modeling self-perceived financial literacy as a mediator between actual financial literacy and risk preference is a new approach, whereas most previous studies treat the two measures as equally exogenous predictors in one regression equation. Through the mediation model, we have shown that the mediation effect accounts for a significant portion of the total effect of actual financial literacy on risk preference.
In practice, the dual effects of actual financial literacy will have implications for policy interventions. Policymakers should distinguish between underconfident and overconfident cohorts and subsequently treat the two groups separately, as they trigger different risk preference dynamics based on actual financial literacy. To do so, classifying individuals into underconfident and overconfident cohorts requires effectively measuring self-estimation of financial knowledge. For example, it might be optimal for policymakers to further encourage increases in actual financial literacy of the underconfident individuals, as this will result in sounder credit card behaviors. However, encouraging an increase in actual financial literacy of the overconfident individuals might not be optimal, as it leads to undesired results. Similarly, but more broadly, the dual effects, for instance on credit card behavior, will have implications for risk management as well. Credit card companies that appraise clients’ creditworthiness and likelihood of paying card debt in full should also archive client profiles in two pools: the over- and underconfident.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jrfm16010053/s1. Table S1: Determinants of self-perceived financial literacy; Table S2: Factors that determine risk preference; Table S3: Determinants of self-perceived financial literacy (additional control variables are in italics); Table S4: Factors that determine risk preference (additional control variables are in italics).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.I.G. and J.L.; Methodology, B.C., C.I.G. and J.L.; Software, J.L.; Validation, B.C., C.I.G. and J.L.; Formal analysis, B.C., C.I.G. and J.L.; Data, B.C. and J.L.; Writing–original draft, J.L.; Writing–review & editing, B.C., C.I.G. and J.L.; Supervision, C.I.G.; Project administration, C.I.G.; Funding acquisition, B.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.usfinancialcapability.org, accessed on 1 February 2021.

Acknowledgments

We thank the editor and the two anonymous referees for their comments that helped us greatly improve our paper. We also thank Meg Radke and Dave Radke for their help with editing an earlier draft of the paper. Research support from the Colby College Research Funding is gratefully acknowledged. The usual caveat applies.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Description of data variables.
Table A1. Description of data variables.
Credit cardBinary, Yes = 1, ‘In the past 12 months, I always paid my credit cards in full’.
Emergency fundsBinary, Yes = 1, ‘Have you set aside emergency or rainy day funds that would cover your expenses in case of sickness, job loss, economic downturn, or other emergency?’
College savingsBinary, Yes = 1, ‘Are you setting aside some money for your children’s college education?’
Actual financial literacyNumber of financial literacy questions answered correctly: 0—5 (2009, 2012, 2015, and 2018) or 0—6 (2015 and 2018).
Questions (https://www.usfinancialcapability.org/downloads/NFCS_2018_State_by_State_Qre.pdf, accessed on 1 February 2021):
M6) Suppose you had $100 in a savings account and the interest rate was 2% per year. After 5 years, how much do you think you would have in the account if you left the money to grow?
M7) Imagine that the interest rate on your savings account was 1% per year and inflation was 2% per year. After 1 year, how much would you be able to buy with the money in this account?
M8) If interest rates rise, what will typically happen to bond prices?
M9) A 15-year mortgage typically requires higher monthly payments than a 30-year mortgage, but the total interest paid over the life of the loan will be less
M10) Buying a single company’s stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund.
M31) Suppose you owe $1000 on a loan and the interest rate you are charged is 20% per year compounded annually. If you didn’t pay anything off, at this interest rate, how many years would it take for the amount you owe to double? (New question in 2015 and 2018)
Self-assessed financial literacyRespondents’ assessment of their overall financial knowledge, on a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 means very low and 7 means very high.
Question (https://www.usfinancialcapability.org/downloads/NFCS_2018_State_by_State_Qre.pdf, accessed on 1 February 2021):
M4) On a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 means very low and 7 means very high, how would you assess your overall financial knowledge?
Risk preferenceResponse to question ‘When thinking of your financial investments, how willing are you to take risks?’ on a 10-point scale, where 1 means “not at all willing” and 10 means “very willing”.
Gender1. Male; 0. Female
Age1. 18–24; 2. 25–34; 3. 35–44; 4. 45–54; 5. 55–64; 6. 65+
Ethnicity1. White; 0. Non-white
Marital status1. Married; 0. Single, Separated, Divorced, or Widowed/Widower
ChildrenNumber of financially dependent children respondents have
Education
  • Did not complete high school;
  • High school graduate;
  • Some college;
  • College graduate;
  • Post graduate
Income
  • less than USD $15,000;
  • $15,000–$24,999;
  • $25,000–$34,999;
  • $35,000–$49,999;
  • $50,000–$74,999;
  • $75,000–$99,999;
  • $100,000–$149,999;
  • $150,000 or more
Employment1. Employed; 0. Unemployed
Home=1 if respondents reporting they or their spouse/partner currently own their home
Disabled=1 if respondents describing current employment or work status as permanently sick, disabled, or unable to work

Notes

1
2
Among the exceptions are Allgood and Walstad (2016) and Balasubramnian and Sargent (2020).
3
Cupák et al. (2018), Bannier and Neubert (2016), and Almenberg and Dreber (2015) are among the recent attempts to explain gender differences in financial literacy, financial risk preference, and investment participation. However, the gender angle is not explored in this work and is left to future research.
4
In contrast, many papers (e.g., Van Rooij et al. 2011a; Darriet et al. 2020) identify only the positive effect on various financial results (e.g., lessened money illusion, better retirement planning).
5
6
The inclusion of ethnicity as a demographic control is consistent with the literature (Lusardi and Mitchell 2007; Allgood and Walstad 2016; Balasubramnian and Sargent 2020).
7
This is still a coarse classification based on the distribution of about 100,000 respondents into only 7 (and fewer) discrete score points.
8
See Supplementary Materails for the full outputs of Tables S1 and S2 that report all details of controls.
9
Figure 2 plots the sample means of risk preference for each cohort of actual financial literacy 0 to 5, holding perceived financial literacy at either 5 or 6 (respectively, the 25th and 75th percentiles as Table 1 reports). Regardless of actual financial literacy measured in 5 or 6 questions, the valley-shaped patterns are persistent.
10
To assess collinearity, we compute the variance inflation factor (VIF) from Equation (1). The resulting VIFs (1.3 to 3.8) indicate no major collinearity in the model.
11
The test is needed even if each of the two coefficients is significant on its own.
12
Based on the estimation in Table 2 and Table 3,   β 2 = 0.39 , γ 1 = 0.886 ,   γ 2 = 0.14 , the mediation effect as a percentage of total effect is 0.886 × 0.39 ÷ ( 0.886 × 0.39 + 0.14 ) = 71.17 % .

References

  1. Allgood, Sam, and William Walstad. 2016. The effects of perceived and actual financial literacy on financial behaviors. Economic Inquiry 54: 675–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Almenberg, Johan, and Anna Dreber. 2015. Gender, stock market participation and financial literacy. Economics Letters 137: 140–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Anderson, Anders, Forest Baker, and David T. Robinson. 2017. Precautionary savings, retirement planning and misperceptions of financial literacy. Journal of Financial Economics 126: 383–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Baker, H. Kent, Satish Kumar, Nisha Goyal, and Vidhu Gaur. 2019. How financial literacy and demographic variables relate to behavioral biases. Managerial Finance 45: 124–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Balasubramnian, Bhanu, and Carol Springer Sargent. 2020. Impact of inflated perceptions of financial literacy on financial decision making. Journal of Economic Psychology 80: 1023064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bannier, Christina E., and Milena Neubert. 2016. Gender differences in financial risk taking: The role of financial literacy and risk tolerance. Economics Letters 145: 130–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bannier, Christina E., and Milena Schwarz. 2018. Gender- and education-related effects of financial literacy and confidence on financial wealth. Journal of Economic Psychology 67: 66–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny. 1986. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51: 1173–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bazley, William J., Yosef Bonaparte, and George M. Korniotis. 2020. Financial self-awareness: Who knows what they don’t know? Finance Research Letters 38: 101445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Boisclair, David, Annamaria Lusardi, and Pierre-Carl Michaud. 2017. Financial literacy and retirement planning in Canada. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance 16: 277–96. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bucher-Koenen, Tabea, and Annamaria Lusardi. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement planning in Germany. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance 10: 565–84. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chatterjee, Swarn, Lu Fan, Ben Jacobs, and Robin Haas. 2017. Risk tolerance and goals-based savings behavior of households: The role of financial literacy. Journal of Personal Finance 16: 66–77. [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark, Robert, Annamaria Lusardi, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2017. Financial knowledge and 401 (k) investment performance: A case study. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance 16: 324–47. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cupák, Andrej, Pirmin Fessler, Alyssa Schneebaum, and Maria Silgoner. 2018. Decomposing gender gaps in financial literacy: New international evidence. Economics Letters 168: 102–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Darriet, Elisa, Marianne Guille, Jean-Christophe Vergnaud, and Mariko Shimizu. 2020. Money illusion, financial literacy and numeracy: Experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Psychology 76: 102211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fanta, Ashenafi, and Kingstone Mutsonziwa. 2021. Financial literacy as a driver of financial inclusion in Kenya and Tanzania. Journal of Risk and Financial Management 14: 561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Goodman, Leo A. 1960. On the exact variance of products. Journal of the American Statistical Association 55: 708–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hayes, Andrew F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Jonsson, Sara, Inga-Lill Söderberg, and Mats Wilhelmsson. 2017. An investigation of the impact of financial literacy, risk attitude, and saving motives on the attenuation of mutual fund investors’ disposition bias. Managerial Finance 43: 282–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lusardi, Annamaria. 2014. APEC Guidebook on Financial and Economic Literacy in Basic Education. Singapore: Asia-Pacific Economic Cooperation. [Google Scholar]
  21. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2007. Baby boomer retirement security: The roles of planning, financial literacy, and housing wealth. Journal of Monetary Economics 54: 205–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2008. Planning and financial literacy: How do women fare? American Economic Review: Papers & Proceedings 98: 413–17. [Google Scholar]
  23. Riley, William B., and K. Victor Chow. 1992. Asset allocation and individual risk aversion. Financial Analysts Journal 48: 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sobel, Michael E. 1982. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology 13: 290–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Van Rooij, Maarten C. J., Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob J. M. Alessie. 2011a. Financial literacy and retirement planning in the Netherlands. Journal of Economic Psychology 32: 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Van Rooij, Maarten C. J., Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob J. M. Alessie. 2011b. Financial literacy and stock market participation. Journal of Financial Economics 101: 449–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Van Rooij, Maarten C. J., Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob J. M. Alessie. 2012. Financial literacy, retirement planning and household wealth. The Economic Journal 122: 449–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. X-M-Y triangle relationship.
Figure 1. X-M-Y triangle relationship.
Jrfm 16 00053 g001
Figure 2. Plots of mean risk preference against actual financial literacy at given self-assessed literacy.
Figure 2. Plots of mean risk preference against actual financial literacy at given self-assessed literacy.
Jrfm 16 00053 g002
Table 1. Summary statistics of key variables.
Table 1. Summary statistics of key variables.
VariablesObs.MeanStd. Dev.Min5%10%25%50%75%Max
Actual financial literacy93,2273.1171.4040012345 (or 6)
Self-assessed financial literacy93,2275.1571.251345567
Risk preference93,2274.7812.6351 10
College savings34,6810.3770.4850 1
Credit cards72,9360.5420.4980 1
Emergency funds90,4330.4420.4970 1
Note: This table presents summary statistics of key variables and percentile distribution of actual and self-assessed financial literacy.
Table 2. Determinants of self-perceived financial literacy (regression results for Equation (1)).
Table 2. Determinants of self-perceived financial literacy (regression results for Equation (1)).
1234
Actual FL ( X i )0.470 *** (0.004)0.943 *** (0.006)0.460 *** (0.006)0.886 *** (0.006)
Constant ( α 1 )4.626 *** (0.027)−0.628 *** (0.042)4.782 *** (0.043)−0.588 *** (0.062)
Sample size37,09115,36918,65810,771
Adj R-sq0.3710.7240.3590.741
Note: Each column shows regression results from a subsample: (1) 4-wave (2009, 2012, 2015, and 2018) overconfident; (2) 4-wave underconfident; (3) 2-wave (2015 and 2018) overconfident; (4) 2-wave underconfident. Demographic and year controls are not shown here but are included in Supplementary Materials. Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 1 % .
Table 3. Factors that determine risk preference (regression results for Equation (2)).
Table 3. Factors that determine risk preference (regression results for Equation (2)).
123456
Actual FL (Xi)0.075 *** (0.006)−0.168 *** (0.014)0.150 *** (0.033)0.056 *** (0.007)−0.121 *** (0.020)0.143 *** (0.033)
Self-assessed FL (Mi)0.461 *** (0.006)0.676 *** (0.016)0.337 *** (0.027)0.527 *** (0.009)0.738 *** (0.023)0.391 *** (0.030)
Constant (α2)1.934 *** (0.059)1.128 *** (0.112)2.251 *** (0.143)2.461 *** (0.092)1.661 *** (0.172)2.588 *** (0.199)
Sample size93,22737,09115,36947,09118,65810,771
Adj R-sq0.2290.2560.1850.2450.2860.219
Note: Each column shows estimation results from a subsample: (1) 4-wave (2009, 2012, 2015, and 2018) whole sample; (2) 4-wave overconfident; (3) 4-wave underconfident; (4) 2-wave (2015 and 2018) whole sample; (5) 2-wave overconfident; (6) 2-wave underconfident. Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 1%.
Table 4. Test of mediation effect.
Table 4. Test of mediation effect.
Type of Method # of Replicates95% Confidence Interval
Bootstrap 50000.341 to 0.351
Sobel (1982)NA 0.293 to 0.399
Goodman (1960)NA 0.293 to 0.399
Note: The sample size of each bootstrap replicate equals that in Table 2 and Table 3. Sobel’s test is derived using the delta method based on a first order Taylor approximation. Goodman (1960) is based on the unbiased variance of the product of two normal variables.
Table 5. Logistic estimation of financial planning decisions.
Table 5. Logistic estimation of financial planning decisions.
College-over-4wave College-under-4waveCredit Card-over-2waveCredit Card-under-2waveEmergency-over-4waveEmergency-under-4wave
Actual FL −0.119 *** (0.022)0.077 ** (0.034) −0.129 *** (0.020) 0.173 *** (0.040) −0.017 (0.013) 0.113 *** (0.040)
Self-assessed FL 0.296 *** (0.025)0.160 *** (0.028) 0.463 *** (0.024) 0.217 *** (0.035) 0.403 *** (0.015) 0.402 *** (0.033)
Sample size14,66113,54113,792945235,71214,975
Pseudo R-sq0.1980.1730.1080.1140.2010.226
Note: Each column regresses a decision for a subsample: (1) college fund decision, 4-wave, and overconfident cohort; (2) college fund decision, 4-wave, and underconfident cohort; (3) credit card decision, 2-wave, and overconfident cohort; (4) credit card decision, 2-wave, and underconfident cohort; (5) emergency fund decision, 4-wave, and overconfident cohort; (6) emergency fund decision, 4-wave, and underconfident cohort. Standard errors in parentheses. ** p < 5%; *** p < 1%.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, B.; Giannikos, C.I.; Lou, J. Is More Financial Literacy Always Beneficial? An Investigation through a Mediator. J. Risk Financial Manag. 2023, 16, 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm16010053

AMA Style

Chen B, Giannikos CI, Lou J. Is More Financial Literacy Always Beneficial? An Investigation through a Mediator. Journal of Risk and Financial Management. 2023; 16(1):53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm16010053

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Biwei, Christos I. Giannikos, and Jun Lou. 2023. "Is More Financial Literacy Always Beneficial? An Investigation through a Mediator" Journal of Risk and Financial Management 16, no. 1: 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm16010053

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop