Is the Suppliance of L2 Inflectional Morphology Subject to Covert Contrasts? An Analysis of the Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement by L1 Bengali Speakers
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(1) |
(a) Last night I boil |
(b) He always watch |
(c) There are three dog |
Background to the Current Study
(2) Do adult L1 Bengali speakers of L2 English show evidence of covert contrasts in cases where obligatory third person agreement appears to be omitted?
2. A Brief Introduction and Comparison of Bengali and English Prosodic Representation
2.1. Prosodic Representation of English Number and Agreement
(3a) | pwd internal | (3b) | pwd adjoined |
‘swept’ | ‘likes’ | ||
(3c) | pwd adjoined | (3d) | pwd adjoined |
‘clocks’ | ‘wiped’ | ||
(4) | a. | [hɛlm] ‘helm’ |
b. | [hɛmp] ‘hemp’ | |
c. | *[hɛlmp] | |
(5) | a. | [rijm] ‘ream’ |
b. | [rijp] ‘reap’ | |
c. | *[rijmp] | |
(6) | a. | [dijp] ‘deep’ |
b. | [dɛpθ] ‘depth’ | |
c. | *[dijpθ] |
(7) | a. | [ki:p] ‘keep’ |
b. | [kɛpt] ‘kept’ | |
c. | *[ki:pt] |
2.2. PTH and L2 English Production of Plural Number and Third Person Singular Agreement: Some Previous Studies
2.3. Available Prosodic Representation in French and Mandarin for Required L2 English Representation of Third Person Singular Agreement and Plural Number on Noun
(8a) | (mai3—lə5) |
buy—perf | |
‘bought already’ | |
(8b) | |
(8c) | |
(8d) | |
(9) | a. | because he understand a lot |
b. | I borrow a lot of book from her |
3. Bengali Prosodic Representation
3.1. Prosodic Representation of Bengali Tense and Person Agreement
(10) | bujʰlam | (‘I understood’) |
(11) | |
(12) | |
(13) | |
3.2. Prosodic Representation of Bengali Plural Noun Agreement
(14) | (((pakhi)STEM -ra) STEM) PWd |
bird -PL | |
‘(the) birds’ |
(15) | (((pakhi)STEM) PWd -gulo) PWd |
bird -PL | |
‘the birds’ |
3.3. Interim Summary
4. Order of Child L1 Morpheme Acquisition Versus Adult L2 Acquisition Studies
4.1. Order of Acquisition of Inflectional Morphemes in L1 and L2 Acquisition
4.2. Covert Contrasts in L1 Acquisition and Adult L2 Acquisition
4.3. Child Acquisition of English Third Person Singular Agreement and Plural Number on Nouns
4.4. Interim Summary
5. An Analysis of L1 Bengali Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement and Plural Number on Nouns
(16) Do L1 Bengali speakers of L2 English show evidence of covert contrasts in cases where obligatory third person agreement appears to be omitted?
5.1. Method
5.1.1. Participants
5.1.2. Proficiency Levels
5.1.3. Experiment Design
5.1.4. Data Analysis
5.2. Findings
This tentative result of no covert contrasts in the analysis of the production of non-suppliance of the ‘-s’ morpheme on third person singular agreement by L1 Bengali speakers of L2 English is illustrated below with samples from two participants, one at Beginner proficiency level and one at Elementary proficiency level. The data presented here are typical of participants within and across the proficiency levels. Participant BEA1 was selected to illustrate the potentiality for suspected covert contrasts, here, interestingly, in the suppliance of ‘-s’ on the noun ‘egg’ in the medial rather than end position (in the production of ‘egg-s’ as seen in Section 5.2.1, e.g., Figure 2)17, and Participant BEB1 was selected to illustrate the clear contrast between suppliance and omission of inflectional morpheme ‘-s’ without any evidence of covert contrast (in the suppliance of ‘sit’ illustrated in Section 5.2.2, e.g., Figure 6).Evidence of systematic covert contrasts: There was no evidence of covert contrasts in the spectrograms of any learner from Beginner to Intermediate proficiency levels.
5.2.1. Sample 1: L1 Bengali Speaker of L2 English at Beginner Proficiency Level
BEA1 | Number of Elicited Tokens | Supplied | Omitted | Other19 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Third person singular | 34 | 1 | 29 | 4 |
Plural noun | 16 | 5 | 9 | 2 |
5.2.2. Sample 2: L1 Bengali Speaker of L2 English at Elementary Proficiency Level
6. Discussion
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- (a)
- Third person singular agreement
- (b)
- Plural number marking on nouns
(i) | dog-s |
(ii) | dog |
Appendix B
Token | Stem Sub-Category |
---|---|
drives | voiced (V) |
feels | V |
finds | V |
keeps | unvoiced (U) |
leaves | V |
needs | V |
shows | V |
types | U |
works | U |
writes | U |
asks | U |
chops | U |
cracks | U |
drills | V |
drops | U |
gives | V |
lives | V |
looks | U |
picks | U |
pins | V |
plans | V |
pulls | V |
sits | U |
tells | V |
wants | U |
yells | V |
crosses | U |
faces | U |
kisses | U |
misses | U |
presses | U |
races | U |
washes | U |
watches | U |
Token | Stem Sub-Category |
---|---|
apples | voiced (V) |
bins | V |
books | unvoiced (U) |
boxes | U |
boys | V |
cats | U |
clocks | U |
days | V |
dogs | V |
flies | V |
foxes | U |
glasses | U |
grapes | U |
pills | V |
toys | V |
watches | U |
1 | Some participants were also speakers of Sylheti. Sylheti is a dialect which is widely spoken by Bengali speakers across Bangladesh and India (see, for example, Chalmers 1996 for an alternative account regarding whether it should be considered a language rather than dialect). For the purposes of the current study, whether participants were speakers of both the Sylheti dialect and Bengali is taken into account as a variable in the statistical analysis. See Ingham (2019) for a brief discussion of aspects of phonology, prosodic representation, and transfer of Sylheti prosodic representation in relation to Bengali. |
2 | The cause of this apparent asymmetry is subject to debate beyond the scope of the current study. Kahoul (2014), for example, proposes that the difference observed between the suppliance of plural and grammatical markings could potentially be attributable to the fact that AgrP is yet to emerge or is unstable. |
3 | See, for example, Goad et al. (2003); Goad and White (2004, 2006, 2019) for a full discussion of the theoretical background of the PTH. |
4 | Examples (8a) and (8b) reproduced from Ingham (2019); reproduced and adapted from Goad et al. (2003, p. 248). |
5 | Examples (8c) and (8d) reproduced from Ingham (2019); reproduced and adapted from Goad et al. (2003, p. 252) and Li and Thompson (1981, p. 49). |
6 | See also Li and Yang (2022) for an alternative interpretation of the availability of comparative inflectional morphology in Mandarin, compatible with the Representational Deficit Hypothesis (Hawkins and Liszka 2003). |
7 | Each finite tense (present, future, and past) has a dedicated person marker (e.g., first person in the present -i, in the future -o, and in the past -am). Furthermore, second and third person markers are also subject to formality. |
8 | |
9 | It should be noted that the three children under observation (spontaneous speech production) were not interconnected, and different researchers were responsible for each child (Brown 1973). |
10 | This does not have to be contrasted in terms of ‘stick’ versus ‘tick’; the key point is that there should be a lack of aspiration in the context where ‘stick’ would be the appropriate lexeme, then a contrast is identified between a stop as the initial or following sound in the onset of a syllable. |
11 | See also Conover and Huntley Bahr (2023) and Munson et al. (2017) for discussion on the effect of methodology and rating scales on the analysis of perceptual ability. |
12 | The participants were recruited both in the UK and Bangladesh to ensure a range of proficiency levels. As reported in the original study (Ingham 2019), the generalised estimating equations modelling approach was used to estimate associations between variables (e.g., Sylheti speaker, college education, verb type, log frequency, proficiency, etc.) and the suppliance of inflectional morphology in spoken data. |
13 | It was established that there was interaction between college education and the suppliance of inflectional morphology, and this was factored into the model build. |
14 | Fewer tokens for third person singular agreement and plural number on nouns were included in the original study compared to regular simple past tense due to the focus on the suppliance of inflection on regular simple past; the elicitation of non-past inflectional morphology was conducted primarily in relation to analysis of prosodic representation and comparison data. |
15 | As pointed out by an anonymous reviewer, the comparison of inflection on verbs in utterance-medial position and nouns in utterance-final position introduces a potential source of asymmetry. Song et al. (2009, p. 626) consider the effect of utterance position in L1 acquisition, and note that as a SVO language, English-acquiring children will be more likely to hear third person singular verbs in utterance-medial position compared to plurals in utterance-final position in parental speech (and this corresponded to the majority of third person singular agreement in the subjects’ production). It follows that adult L2 learners may also be exposed to third person singular verbs and plural nouns in medial and final position, respectively, and that, subsequently, elicited spontaneous production in the current study mirrors this. Manipulation of utterance position would be of value in future studies. |
16 | |
17 | As stated in Section 5.1.3, only number on nouns in utterance-final position were included in the data analysis. |
18 | |
19 | The category ‘other’ was sub-analysed in instances where an unsolicited inflectional ending was supplied. These data were not analysed for the current paper, but it remains an area to be investigated, particularly in relation to the utterance-medial versus utterance-end position of target forms. |
20 | An anonymous reviewer pointed out a third option, which is to use existing L1 representations. This is investigated in depth by Goad and White (2006) in relation to the suppliance of past and perfective forms of both regular and irregular verbs, such as ‘wrapped’ and ‘kept’, by L1 Mandarin speakers. The authors argue that phonetic evidence (primarily the presence or absence of fortis release in VCC combinations in relation to syllabification and domain boundary) provides clues as to when a regular form is inflected in a target-like pwd adjoined representation versus a non-target-like (but more readily available) L1 pwd internal representation. In contrast to Mandarin, Bengali requires inflectional material to be prosodified both internally and adjoined to the pwd. The potential task for the L1 Bengali speaker is arguably to allow minimal adaptations in the relicensing of prosidc structure to L2 forms. |
References
- Aljutaily, Mohammad, and Raghad Alqweefl. 2023. The covert contrast in the acquisition of/u/-/uː/: Production evidence from non-Arabic speakers. Migration Letters 20: 840–63. [Google Scholar]
- Bayer, Josef, and Aditi Lahiri. 1990. Bengali emphatic clitics in the lexicon-syntax interface. In Contemporary Morphology. Edited by Wolfgang U. Dressler, Hans C. Luschützky, Oskar E. Pfeiffer and John R. Rennison. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 3–16. [Google Scholar]
- Bley-Vroman, Robert. 1989. What is the logical problem of foreign language learning? In Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Susan M. Gass and Jacquelyn Schachter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, chap. 2. pp. 41–68. [Google Scholar]
- Bley-Vroman, Robert. 2009. The evolving context of the fundamental difference hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31: 175–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2023. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer. Version 6.3.09. Available online: https://www.praat.org (accessed on 5 October 2023).
- Boyle David, Anne. 2015. Descriptive Grammar of Bangla. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cabrelli Amaro, Jennifer, Gonzalo Campos Dintrans, and Jason Rothman. 2018. The role of L1 phonology and L2 morphological production. L2 English past tense production by L1 Spanish, Mandarin, and Japanese speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40: 503–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campos Dintrans, Gonzalo Santiago. 2011. Acquisition of Morphosyntax in the Adult Second Language: The Phonology Factor. Ph.D. thesis, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA. Available online: http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/2677 (accessed on 10 October 2023).
- Chalmers, Rod. 1996. Learning Sylheti. Dhaka: Centre for Bangladeshi Studies. [Google Scholar]
- Conover, Laura, and Ruth Huntley Bahr. 2023. Covert contrasts and categorical perception: A comparison of two measurement tools. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 37: 143–56. [Google Scholar]
- Dulay, Heidi C., and Marina K. Burt. 1973. Should we teach children syntax? Language Learning 23: 245–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eckman, Fred, Gregory Iverson, and Jae Yung Song. 2015. Overt and covert contrasts in L2 phonology. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 1: 254–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitzpatrick-Cole, Jennifer. 1996. Reduplication meets the phonological phrase in Bengali. The Linguistic Review 13: 305–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gass, Susan M. 2013. Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course, 4th ed. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbon, Fiona E., and Alice Lee. 2017a. Electropalatographic (EPG) evidence of covert contrasts in disordered speech. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 4–20. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbon, Fiona E., and Alice Lee. 2017b. Preface to the special issue on covert contrasts. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 1–3. [Google Scholar]
- Goad, Heather, and Lydia White. 2004. Ultimate attainment of L2 inflection: Effects of L1 prosodic structure. In EUROSLA Yearbook 4. Edited by Susan H. Foster-Cohen, Michael Sharwood Smith, Antonella Sorace and Mitsuhiko Ota. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 119–45. [Google Scholar]
- Goad, Heather, and Lydia White. 2006. Ultimate attainment in interlanguage grammars: A prosodic approach. Second Language Research 22: 243–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goad, Heather, and Lydia White. 2008. Prosodic structure and the representation of L2 functional morphology: A nativist approach. Lingua 118: 577–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goad, Heather, and Lydia White. 2019. Prosodic effects on L2 grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9: 1–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goad, Heather, Lydia White, and Jeffrey Steele. 2003. Missing inflection in L2 acquisition: Defective syntax or L1-constrained prosodic representations? The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 48: 243–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldschneider, Jennifer M., and Robert DeKeyser. 2005. Explaining the ‘Natural Order of L2 Morpheme Acquisition’ in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Language Learning 55: 26–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harel, Daphna, Elaine Russo Hitchcock, Daniel Szeredi, José Oritz, and Tara McAllister Byun. 2017. Finding the experts in the crowd: Validity and reliability of crowdsourced measures of children’s gradient speech contrasts. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 104–17. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, Roger, and Cecilia Yuet-hung Chan. 1997. The partial availability of universal grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional features hypothesis’. Second Language Research 13: 187–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawkins, Roger, and Sarah Ann Liszka. 2003. Locating the source of defective past tense marking in advanced L2 English speakers. In The Lexicon-Syntax Interface in Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Roeland van Hout, Aafke Hulk, Folkert Kuiken and Richard J. Towell. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 21–44. [Google Scholar]
- Ingham, Jacqueline. 2019. L2 Inflectional Morphology and Prosody: The Case of L1 Bengali Speakers of L2 English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Ingham, Jacqueline. 2022. Transfer of prosodic representation: L1 Bengali production of L2 English regular simple past tense. In Generative SLA in the Age of Minimalism: Features, Interfaces, and Beyond. Edited by Tania Leal, Elena Shimanskaya and Casilde A. Isabelli. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 157–87. [Google Scholar]
- Kahoul, Walid. 2014. Arabic and Chinese Learners’ Production, Perception and Processing of Past Tense and Verbal Agreement Morphology in L2 English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Khan, Sameer ud Dowla. 2008. Intonational Phonology and Focus Prosody of Bengali. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Lahiri, Aditi. 2000. Hierarchical restructuring in the creation of verbal morphology in Bengali and Germanic: Evidence from phonology. In Analogy, Levelling and Markedness: Principles of Change in Phonology and Morphology. Edited by Aditi Lahiri. Berlin: Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Lardiere, Donna. 2007. Ultimate Attainment in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Lardiere, Donna. 2008. Feature assembly in second language acquisition. In The Role of Formal Features in Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Juana M. Liceras, Helmut Zobl and Helen Goodluck. New York: Routledge, pp. 106–40. [Google Scholar]
- Leech, Geoffrey, Paul Rayson, and Andrew Wilson. 2001. Word Frequencies in Written and Spoken English: Based on the British National Corpus. London: Longman. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Charles N., and Sandra A. Thompson. 1981. Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Gramar. Berkley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Fangfang, Jan Edwards, and Mary E. Beckman. 2008. Contrast and covert contrast: The phonetic development of voiceless sibilant fricatives in English and Japanese toddlers. Journal of Phonetics 37: 111–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Ni, and Lianrui Yang. 2022. Testing the representational deficit hypothesis: From the aspect of Chinese learners’ acquistion of affixation ‘-s’ for third person singular verbs and plural nouns. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Long, Michael H., and Charlene J. Sato. 1984. Methodological issues inn interlanguage studies: An interactionist perspective. In Interlanguage. Edited by Alan Davies, Clive Criper and Anthony Hewitt. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, pp. 253–79. [Google Scholar]
- Macken, Marlys A., and David Barton. 1980. A longitudinal study of the acquisition of the voicing contrast in American-English word-initial stops, as measured by voice onset time. Journal of Child Language 7: 41–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munson, Benjamin, Sarah K. Schellinger, and Jan Edwards. 2017. Bias in the perception of phonetic detail in children’s speech: A comparison of categorical and continuous rating scales. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 57–79. [Google Scholar]
- Murakami, Akira, and Theodora Alexopoulou. 2016. L1 influence on the acquisition order of English grammatical morphemes: A learner corpus study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 38: 365–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, Bao T. T., and Jonathan Newton. 2022. Production of third-person singular -s and be copula in communication tasks by Vietnamese EFL learners: Acquisition orderr and learner orientation to form. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 25: 19–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prévost, Philippe, and Lydia White. 2000. Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research 16: 103–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slabakova, Roumyana. 2016. Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Song, Jae Yung, and Fred Eckman. 2019. Covert contrasts in the acquisition of English high front vowels by native speakers of Korean, Portuguese, and Spanish. Language Acquisition 26: 436–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, Jae Yung, and Fred Eckman. 2021. Using ultrasound tongue imaging to study coverts contrasts in second-language learners’ acquisition of English vowels. Language Acquisition 28: 344–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, Jae Yung, Megha Sundara, and Katherine Demuth. 2009. Phonological constraints on children’s production of English third person singular -s. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52: 623–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thompson, Hanne-Ruth. 2012. Bengali. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- White, Lydia. 2003. Fossilization in steady state L2 grammars: Persistent problems with inflectional morphology. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6: 129–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Lydia. 2008. Some puzzling features of L2 features. In The Role of Features in Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Juana M. Liceras, Helmut Zobl and Helen Goodluck. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 300–26. [Google Scholar]
- White, Lydia. 2009. Grammatical theory: Interfaces and L2 knowledge. In The New Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by William C. Ritchie and Tej K. Bhatia. Leeds: Emerald Group Publishing Limited, pp. 49–68. [Google Scholar]
- Zharkova, Natalia, Fiona E. Gibbon, and Alice Lee. 2017. Using ultrasound tongue imaging to identiify covert contrasts in children’s speech. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 21–34. [Google Scholar]
Proficiency Level | Number of Participants |
---|---|
Beginner | 7 |
Elementary | 8 |
Intermediate | 8 |
Advanced | 5 |
Predictor | QICC | ΔQICC | |
---|---|---|---|
Overview | proficiency, word type, proficiency*word type | 2120.221 | - |
(a) Proficiency | (b) Number of Participants | (c) 3 sg | (d) pl |
---|---|---|---|
Beginner | 7 | 0.08 | 0.44 |
Elementary | 8 | 0.28 | 0.76 |
Intermediate | 8 | 0.57 | 0.95 |
Advanced | 5 | 0.96 | 1.0 |
BEB1 | Number of Elicited Tokens | Supplied | Omitted | Other |
---|---|---|---|---|
Third person singular | 34 | 9 | 15 | 10 |
Plural noun | 16 | 13 | 2 | 1 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ingham, J. Is the Suppliance of L2 Inflectional Morphology Subject to Covert Contrasts? An Analysis of the Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement by L1 Bengali Speakers. Languages 2024, 9, 165. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050165
Ingham J. Is the Suppliance of L2 Inflectional Morphology Subject to Covert Contrasts? An Analysis of the Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement by L1 Bengali Speakers. Languages. 2024; 9(5):165. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050165
Chicago/Turabian StyleIngham, Jacqueline. 2024. "Is the Suppliance of L2 Inflectional Morphology Subject to Covert Contrasts? An Analysis of the Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement by L1 Bengali Speakers" Languages 9, no. 5: 165. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050165
APA StyleIngham, J. (2024). Is the Suppliance of L2 Inflectional Morphology Subject to Covert Contrasts? An Analysis of the Production of L2 English Third Person Singular Agreement by L1 Bengali Speakers. Languages, 9(5), 165. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050165