The Effect of Pitch Accent on the Perception of English Lexical Stress: Evidence from English and Mandarin Chinese Listeners
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Stress and Pitch Accent in English
1.2. Weighting of Acoustic Cues to English Lexical Stress
1.3. Lexical Stress in Spoken Word Recognition
- Are H*-accented di- and trisyllabic words with initial stress more accurately identified than deaccented disyllabic and trisyllabic words with initial stress?
- Is f0 as effective as vowel quality in signaling initial stress in H*-accented and deaccented di- and trisyllabic words?
- How do native Mandarin listeners perceive English lexical stress in H*-accented and deaccented di- and trisyllabic words in comparison to native English listeners?
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Stimuli
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Native English Listeners
3.2. Native Mandarin Chinese Listeners
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Non-Vowel-Reduction Condition | Vowel-Reduction Condition | ||
---|---|---|---|
Target | Competitor | Target | Competitor |
antonym | antenna | advocate | advisor |
archive | arcade | column | cologne |
campus | campaign | commerce | command |
carnival | carnation | continent | container |
distance | distaste | concept | concern |
instrument | instructor | concert | conceit |
intellect | intestine | confluence | confusion |
introvert | intruder | promise | promotion |
monster | monsoon | motive | material |
musical | museum | congress | congratulate |
mister | mistake | parrot | parade |
particle | partition | polisher | policeman |
booking | bouquet | programmer | procurer |
ambulance | ambition | providence | provider |
district | destroy | purchase | pursue |
Target | Competitor | Target | Competitor |
---|---|---|---|
motel | mystery | activity | absolute |
Manhattan | monarch | antique | alternate |
parole | Paris | biologist | blackmail |
dismissal | diplomat | canteen | capture |
suggest | summer | cartel | carefree |
amnesia | altitude | diameter | deviate |
inspection | illustrate | incise | iceberg |
Mercedes | mediate | invasion | ignorance |
trustee | truthful | machine | motion |
receipt | relative | pursuit | protest |
reward | rocket | review | rubric |
gorilla | governor | routine | ribbon |
consent | clumsy | deception | discount |
reactor | relevant | percussion | prospect |
direction | dialect | distinct | diamond |
retention | registry | distress | delegate |
Target | Competitor | Target | Competitor |
---|---|---|---|
habit | hotel | secrete | season |
cancel | collapse | bizarre | bargain |
butter | balloon | between | biscuit |
tunnel | tattoo | neglect | nocturne |
captain | canal | polite | panel |
atmosphere | approach | promote | principle |
debut | dilute | conceal | canvass |
pavement | patrol | freebie | forever |
funeral | forget | ferment | fellow |
passport | possess | guitar | garden |
solider | suppose | lament | legend |
option | obscure | reserve | rescue |
college | cocoon | career | carbon |
empire | emission | peculiar | porridge |
formula | forbid | success | summon |
palace | pecan | respond | ruthless |
Appendix B
Word Frequency M (SD) | Word Prevalence M (SD) | No. of Letters M (SD) | No. of Syllables M (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
non-vowel-reduction | ||||
target | 3.63 (0.97) | 2.33 (0.18) | 7.73 (1.16) | 2.53 (0.52) |
competitor | 3.56 (0.73) | 2.27 (0.22) | 7.73 (1.16) | 2.47 (0.52) |
vowel-reduction | ||||
target | 3.61 (0.76) | 2.29 (0.33) | 7.87 (1.41) | 2.40 (0.51) |
competitor | 3.70 (0.76) | 2.27 (0.28) | 7.93 (1.53) | 2.73 (0.70) |
1 | Available at: http://phonetics.linguistics.ucla.edu/facilities/acoustic/IntensityScaler.txt (accessed on 16 September 2020). |
References
- Baayen, R. Harald, Richard Piepenbrock, and Leon Gulikers. 1996. The CELEX Lexical Database (cd-rom). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckman, Mary E. 1986. Intonational structure in English and Japanese. Phonology Yearbook 3: 255–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckman, Mary E., and Gayle Ayers. 1997. Guidelines for ToBI labelling. The OSU Research Foundation 3: 30. [Google Scholar]
- Beckman, Mary E., and Julia Hirschberg. 1994. The ToBI Annotation Conventions. Columbus: Ohio State University. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, Ryan, Kevin Tang, and Juan Ajsivinac Sian. 2018. Statistical and acoustic effects on the perception of stop consonants in Kaqchikel (Mayan). Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology 9: 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2021. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer. Available online: http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ (accessed on 12 June 2020).
- Bolinger, Dwight. 1989. Intonation and Its Uses: Melody in Grammar and Discourse. Redwood City: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brysbaert, Marc, and Boris New. 2009. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41: 977–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, Marc, Pawel Mandera, and Emmanuel Keuleers. 2018. The Word Frequency Effect in Word Processing: An Updated Review. Current Directions in Psychological Science 27: 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, Marc, Pawel Mandera, Samantha F. McCormick, and Emmanuel Keuleers. 2019. Word prevalence norms for 62,000 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods 51: 467–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Campbell, Nick, and Mary Beckman. 1997. Stress, prominence, and spectral tilt. Paper presented at ESCA Tutorial and Research Workshop on Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications, Athens, Greece, September 18–20. [Google Scholar]
- Chrabaszcz, Anna, Matthew Winn, Candise Y. Lin, and William J. Idsardi. 2014. Acoustic cues to perception of word stress by English, Mandarin, and Russian speakers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57: 1468–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connell, Katrina, Simone Hüls, Maria Teresa Martínez-García, Zhen Qin, Seulgi Shin, Hanbo Yan, and Annie Tremblay. 2018. English Learners’ Use of Segmental and Suprasegmental Cues to Stress in Lexical Access: An Eye-Tracking Study. Language Learning 68: 635–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connine, Cynthia M., Dawn G. Blasko, and Jian Wang. 1994. Vertical similarity in spoken word recognition: Multiple lexical activation, individual differences, and the role of sentence context. Perception & Psychophysics 56: 624–36. [Google Scholar]
- Connine, Cynthia. M., Debra Titone, Thomas Deelman, and Dawn Blasko. 1997. Similarity mapping in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 37: 463–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cooper, Nicole, Anne Cutler, and Roger Wales. 2002. Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non-native listeners. Language and Speech 45: 207–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cutler, Anne. 1986. Forbear is a Homophone: Lexical Prosody Does Not Constrain Lexical Access. Language and Speech 29: 201–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cutler, Anne, and Charles Clifton. 1984. The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes. Edited by H. Bouma and D. G. Bouwhuis. London: Erlbaum, pp. 183–96. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, Lisa, Jason Shaw, and Tuuli Adams. 2007. The effect of word learning on the perception of non-native consonant sequences. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 122: 3697–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Bot, Kees, Wander Lowie, and Marjolijn Verspoor. 2007. A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10: 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fear, Beverley D., Anne Cutler, and Sally Butterfield. 1995. The strong/weak syllable distinction in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97: 1893–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- FindingFive Team. 2019. FindingFive: A Web Platform for Creating, Running, and Managing Your Studies in One Place. Cherry Hill: FindingFive Corporation (Nonprofit). Available online: https://www.findingfive.com (accessed on 8 September 2020).
- Flege, James E., and Ratree Wayland. 2019. The role of input in native Spanish Late learners’ production and perception of English phonetic segments. Journal of Second Language Studies 2: 1–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, Alexander L., and Howard C. Nusbaum. 2002. Selective attention and the acquisition of new phonetic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, Alexander L., Valter Ciocca, Lian Ma, and Kimberly Fenn. 2008. Perceptual learning of Cantonese lexical tones by tone and non-tone language speakers. Journal of Phonetics 36: 268–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fry, Dennis Butler. 1955. Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 27: 765–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fry, Dennis Butler. 1958. Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech 1: 126–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fry, Dennis Butler. 1965. The dependence of stress judgments on vowel formant structure. In Phonetic Sciences. Basel: Karger Publishers, pp. 306–11. [Google Scholar]
- Goldinger, Stephen D., Paul A. Luce, and David B. Pisoni. 1989. Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language 28: 501–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guion, Susan G., and Eric Pederson. 2007. Investigating the role of attention in phonetic learning. Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning 17: 57–77. [Google Scholar]
- Gussenhoven, Carlos. 1994. Focus and sentence accents in English. Focus and Natural Language Processing 3: 83–92. [Google Scholar]
- Halle, Morris, and Jean-Roger Vergnaud. 1987. Stress and the cycle. Linguistic Inquiry 18: 45–84. [Google Scholar]
- Hammond, Robert M. 1995. Foreign accent and phonetic interference: The application of linguistic research to the teaching of second language pronunciation. In Second Language Acquisition Theory and Pedagogy. London: Taylor & Francis Group, pp. 293–304. [Google Scholar]
- Hockett, Charles Francis. 1955. A Manual of Phonology. No. 11. Baltimore: Waverly Press. [Google Scholar]
- Holt, Lori L., and Andrew J. Lotto. 2006. Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second language acquisition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: 3059–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iverson, Paul, Patricia K. Kuhl, Reiko Akahane-Yamada, Eugen Diesch, Yoh’ich Tohkura, Andreas Kettermann, and Claudia Siebert. 2003. A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition 87: B47–B57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- King, Adam, and Andrew Wedel. 2020. Greater early disambiguating information for less-probable words: The lexicon is shaped by incremental processing. Open Mind 4: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ladd, D. Robert. 2008. Intonational Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Aike, and Brechtje Post. 2014. L2 acquisition of prosodic properties of speech rhythm: Evidence from L1 Mandarin and German learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 36: 223–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lieberman, Philip. 1965. On the acoustic basis of the perception of intonation by linguists. Word 21: 40–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Yaobin. 2019. The influence of pitch contour on Mandarin speakers’ perception of English stress. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 25: 20. [Google Scholar]
- Marslen-Wilson, William. 1990. Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives. Edited by Gerry T. M. Altmann. Cambridge: The MIT Press, pp. 148–72. [Google Scholar]
- Marslen-Wilson, William, and Paul Warren. 1994. Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review 101: 653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martinet, André. 1964. Elements of General Linguistics. Translated by Elisabeth Palmer. London: Faber and Faber. First published 1960. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire, Grant L. 2007. Phonetic Category Learning. Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. [Google Scholar]
- McQueen, James M., Dennis Norris, and Anne Cutler. 1994. Competition in spoken word recognition: Spotting words in other words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20: 621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McQueen, James M., Dennis Norris, and Anne Cutler. 1999. Lexical influence in phonetic decision making: Evidence from subcategorical mismatches. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25: 1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qin, Zhen, Yu-Fu Chien, and Annie Tremblay. 2017. Processing of word-level stress by Mandarin-speaking second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics 38: 541–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2022. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 22 October 2020).
- Raphael, Lawrence J. 2021. Acoustic cues to the perception of segmental phonemes. In The Handbook of Speech Perception. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 603–31. [Google Scholar]
- Reinisch, Eva, and Andrea Weber. 2012. Adapting to suprasegmental lexical stress errors in foreign-accented speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 132: 1165–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell, vol. 1, pp. 550–69. [Google Scholar]
- Sluijter, Agaath M. C., and Vincent J. Van Heuven. 1996a. Acoustic correlates of linguistic stress and accent in Dutch and American English. Paper presented at Fourth International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, ICSLP’96, Philadelphia, PA, USA, October 3–6; vol. 2, pp. 630–33. [Google Scholar]
- Sluijter, Agaath M. C., and Vincent J. Van Heuven. 1996b. Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100: 2471–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Small, Larry H., Stephen D. Simon, and Jill S. Goldberg. 1988. Lexical stress and lexical access: Homographs versus nonhomographs. Perception & Psychophysics 44: 272–80. [Google Scholar]
- Soto-Faraco, Salvador, Núria Sebastián-Gallés, and Anne Cutler. 2001. Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language 45: 412–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Surendran, Dinoj, and Gina-Anne Levow. 2004. The functional load of tone in Mandarin is as high as that of vowels. Paper presented at International Conference on Speech Prosody 2004, Nara, Japan, March 23–26. [Google Scholar]
- Tyler, Lorraine K. 1984. The structure of the initial cohort: Evidence from gating. Perception & Psychophysics 36: 417–27. [Google Scholar]
- Van Donselaar, Wilma, Mariëtte Koster, and Anne Cutler. 2005. Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 58: 251–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Heuven, Walter J. B., Pawel Mandera, Emmanuel Keuleers, and Marc Brysbaert. 2014. Subtlex-UK: A New and Improved Word Frequency Database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67: 1176–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, Yue, Michelle M. Spence, Allard Jongman, and Joan A. Sereno. 1999. Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106: 3649–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wayland, Ratree P., and Susan G. Guion. 2004. Training English and Chinese listeners to perceive Thai tones: A preliminary report. Language Learning 54: 681–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, Yi. 2013. ProsodyPro—A Tool for Large-scale Systematic Prosody Analysis. Paper presented at Tools and Resources for the Analysis of Speech Prosody (TRASP 2013), Aix-en-Provence, France, August 30; pp. 7–10. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, Yanhong, and Alexander Francis. 2010. The weighting of vowel quality in native and non-native listeners’ perception of English lexical stress. Journal of Phonetics 38: 260–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Carrier Sentence | No Vowel Reduction | Vowel Reduction |
---|---|---|
ARchive (vs. arCADE) | CONcept (vs. conCERN) | |
ARchive (vs. arCADE) | CONcept (vs. conCERN) |
Carrier Sentence (a) | Max f0 (Hz) | Min f0 (Hz) | Mean f0 (Hz) | Mean Intensity (dB) | Duration (ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mary | 231 | 207 | 222 | 72 | 327 |
said | 221 | 202 | 208 | 68 | 214 |
Carrier Sentence (b) | |||||
Mary | 212 | 175 | 194 | 72 | 398 |
said | 182 | 160 | 171 | 61 | 215 |
Predictors | β | SE | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 1.468 | 0.125 | 11.762 | <0.001 |
Pitch accent (deaccented) | −0.241 | 0.170 | −1.414 | 0.157 |
Vowel reduction (vowel-reduction) | 1.661 | 0.174 | 9.553 | <0.001 |
Pitch accent (deaccented): vowel red (vowel-reduction) | −0.082 | 0.340 | −0.241 | 0.809 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 3.29 | |||
τ00 stimuli | 0.21 | |||
τ00 participant | 0.27 | |||
ICC | 0.13 | |||
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 | 0.158/0.264 |
Predictors | β | SE | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.665 | 0.137 | 4.850 | <0.001 |
Pitch accent (deaccented) | −0.728 | 0.230 | −3.170 | 0.002 |
Vowel reduction (vowel-reduction) | 1.339 | 0.231 | 5.801 | <0.001 |
Pitch accent (deaccented): vowel reduction (vowel-reduction) | −0.404 | 0.459 | −0.880 | 0.379 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 3.29 | |||
τ00 stimuli | 0.59 | |||
τ00 participant | 0.16 | |||
ICC | 0.19 | |||
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 | 0.128/0.289 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wang, F.; Deng, D.; Tang, K.; Wayland, R. The Effect of Pitch Accent on the Perception of English Lexical Stress: Evidence from English and Mandarin Chinese Listeners. Languages 2024, 9, 87. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9030087
Wang F, Deng D, Tang K, Wayland R. The Effect of Pitch Accent on the Perception of English Lexical Stress: Evidence from English and Mandarin Chinese Listeners. Languages. 2024; 9(3):87. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9030087
Chicago/Turabian StyleWang, Fenqi, Delin Deng, Kevin Tang, and Ratree Wayland. 2024. "The Effect of Pitch Accent on the Perception of English Lexical Stress: Evidence from English and Mandarin Chinese Listeners" Languages 9, no. 3: 87. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9030087
APA StyleWang, F., Deng, D., Tang, K., & Wayland, R. (2024). The Effect of Pitch Accent on the Perception of English Lexical Stress: Evidence from English and Mandarin Chinese Listeners. Languages, 9(3), 87. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9030087