Next Article in Journal
The Role of Determiners in the Processing of Gender Agreement Morphology by Heritage Speakers of Spanish
Previous Article in Journal
Translanguaging and Second-Language Reading Proficiency: A Systematic Review of Effects and Methodological Rigor
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

On the Acquisition of English Complex Predicates and Complex Word Formation: Revisiting the Parametric Approach

1
Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China
2
Department of Linguistics, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2025, 10(8), 201; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080201
Submission received: 22 March 2025 / Revised: 20 July 2025 / Accepted: 26 July 2025 / Published: 21 August 2025

Abstract

Languages vary in their availability of productive endocentric bare-stem compounds (e.g., flower hat) and a range of complex predicates (separable verb-particles, double object datives, adjectival resultatives, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports). To account for these cross-linguistic variations, two parameters have been proposed: the Compounding Parameter (TCP), which governs the formation of bare-stem compounds, separable verb-particles, and adjectival resultatives, and the Small Clause Parameter (SCP), which determines whether a verb can take a small clause complement. These parameters make testable predictions about children’s acquisition. If TCP and SCP are on the right track, we would expect correlations in the acquisition of structures governed by each parameter. This study examines these predictions by analyzing longitudinal corpora from 23 English-speaking children, assessing both the correlation between the acquisition of different structures and their acquisitional ordering. Our findings support both TCP and SCP, confirming that the acquisition of bare-stem compounds is closely associated with that of separable verb-particles, while the acquisition of (some) complex predicates is related. In addition, our results offer new insights into the potential triggers that children use to set each parameter. These findings contribute to our understanding of language variation and the role of parameter setting in first language acquisition.

1. Introduction

A central question in linguistic theory concerns the nature of cross-linguistic variation and how children make the correct choices for their target language in the process of language acquisition. In this study, we investigate one such case regarding English complex word formation and complex predicates.
A prominent proposal in this domain is the Compounding Parameter (TCP) (Snyder, 1995, 2001, et seq.), which posits that languages vary systematically in the availability of productive, endocentric, bare-stem noun-noun compounds (N-N compounds, e.g., cookie chair). Crucially, TCP has been argued to underlie not only compounding creativity but also the availability of complex predicates such as V-NP-particles (e.g., lift the table up, also referred to as separable verb-particles in the literature)1 and adjectival resultatives (e.g., wipe the table clean), as these constructions depend on a shared mechanism that combines the main verb and the secondary predicate to form a unit akin to a morphological compound at the point of semantic interpretation. Consequently, there is systematic association in terms of their availability: cross-linguistic typological surveys revealed that languages with a V-NP-particle construction consistently allow productive endocentric compounding (Snyder, 2007), and resultatives of the English type are found only in languages with productive endocentric compounding (Snyder, 2001).
Further extending this framework, Snyder (2012) introduces the Small Clause Parameter (SCP), which governs the availability of small clause complements, to explain why some languages (e.g., Japanese) allow endocentric N-N compounds but lack V-NP-particles and some type of adjectival resultatives. This parameter is hypothesized to explain the availability of a broader set of complex predicates, including V-NP-particles, (at least some) adjectival resultatives, double object (DO) datives, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports, as illustrated in (1).
(1)a. They lifted the table up.(V-NP-particle)
b. I hammered the metal flat. (Adjectival resultative)
c. Mary gave me a book. (DO dative)
d. I put the apple on the table. (Put-locative)
e. Jenny made the kid cry. (Make-causative)
f. John saw the boy leave. (Perceptual report)
Together, TCP and SCP generate two key acquisition predictions: (i) in their acquisition, V-NP-particles, resultatives, and novel N-N compounds are predicted to be related with each other, reflecting their shared reliance on TCP, and (ii) in their acquisition, small-clause-dependent predicates may associate with each other, reflecting their shared reliance on SCP.
Empirical support for these predictions has emerged from acquisition studies in English, Japanese, and German (Snyder & Stromswold, 1997; Sugisaki & Isobe, 2000; Snyder, 2001, 2007; Wang et al., 2020, 2022). However, there remain some open questions. While correlations have been observed both among complex predicates and between complex predicates and N-N compounding (Snyder & Stromswold, 1997; Snyder, 2001, 2007), some prior research did not control for potential confounding factors, raising the question of whether these constructions are acquired together because they share some linguistic prerequisites or simply because they are part of the “grammar explosion”. Some studies, while controlling for the general trend of language development, examined only a subset of the relevant structures, leaving open the question of whether the acquisition of the remaining structures is correlated.
This study revisits these questions by examining the longitudinal corpora of 23 English-acquiring children. Employing statistical controls to disentangle parameter-driven acquisition from broader developmental factors, we investigate potential association between the acquisition of productive N-N compounds and complex predicates (V-NP-particles, put-locatives, DO datives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports). As a preview of the results, our findings confirm and extend some key findings in Snyder’s series of work, namely that a child’s acquisition of English V-NP-particles is closely connected to the acquisition of N-N compounds and that the acquisition of (some) complex predicates is interrelated.
By addressing some of the remaining questions in prior work, this study aims to provide more empirical evidence for the parametric approach of language acquisition. We hope the findings will shed light on discussions about the role of macroparameters in linguistic theory and the extent to which abstract parameters guide language development.
The rest of this paper is structured as follows: Section 2 reviews the relevant background of TCP and SCP, alongside prior acquisitional studies of these two parameters. Section 3 presents, in detail, the methodology, data analyses, and results of our corpus study. Section 4 summarizes the major findings, discusses their broader implications, identifies unresolved questions, and outlines directions for future research. Section 5 concludes the paper.

2. Background

2.1. On the Nature of Variations in Root Compounding and Complex Predicates: TCP and SCP

This section reviews Snyder’s parametric accounts for cross-linguistic variations in N-N compounding and complex predicate formation. Snyder (1995, 2001) proposed that languages vary parametrically with respect to whether the grammar allows creative endocentric, bare-stem N-N compounding, termed the Compounding Parameter (TCP). The positive setting of this binary parameter makes it possible for speakers to create novel endocentric compounds from uninflected roots on the fly, and for listeners to readily interpret their meanings based on context. For example, in English, a [+TCP] language, the compound flower hat could refer to a hat with flowers on it or a hat shaped like a flower. Furthermore, N-N compounds can be recursively formed, as in flower hat store, which might denote either a store selling flower hats or a store decorated with them (but not necessarily selling them). In contrast, [−TCP] languages do not permit speakers to freely create and interpret novel bare-stem N-N compounds. While bare-stem N-N compounds may exist, they are typically lexicalized, meaning that their interpretations are fixed and stored in the lexicon rather than creatively generated. For example, in Spanish, a [−TCP] language, the compound hombre rana (literally “man frog”) has a rigid, conventionalized meaning: underwater diver. In contrast, the English counterpart frog man could mean underwater diver but could just as easily refer to any man associated with frogs—a man studying frogs, a man with a pet frog, or even a man dressed in a frog costume.
Crucially, Snyder argues that the availability of English V-NP-particles and adjectival resultatives, as exemplified in (1a) and (1b), depends on the positive setting of this parameter.2 This link between N-N compounds and these complex predicates has gained support from cross-linguistic surveys. For example, Snyder (2001) reported that if a language allows adjectival resultatives of the English-type, it also has productive N-N compounding (see also Son, 2007). In addition, languages with a V-NP-particle construction consistently allow productive endocentric compounding (Snyder, 2007).
To formally characterize whether a language permits novel N-N compounding and its association with the availability of V-NP-particles and resultatives, Snyder (2012) proposes that a single semantic interpretation mechanism, Generalized Modification (GM, as defined in (2)), is responsible for interpreting both novel compounds and these complex predicates. Accordingly, TCP is defined in terms of GM, as in (3).3
(2)Generalized Modification (GM)
  If α and β are syntactic sisters under the node γ, where α is the head of γ, and if α denotes a kind, then interpret γ semantically as a subtype of α’s kind that stands in a pragmatically suitable relation to the denotation of β.
(Snyder, 2012, p. 285, ex. 11.5)
(3)The Compounding Parameter (TCP)
The language (does/does not) permit Generalized Modification.
(Snyder, 2012, p. 285, ex. 11.4)
Under this formulation, a [+TCP] language (e.g., English) permits GM, which allows two root nouns (e.g., frog and man) to combine syntactically into a bare-stem N-N compound, where man serves as the head and denotes individuals. The resulting compound frog man is interpreted as a subtype of men that stands in a pragmatically suitable relation to frogs—whether this means a man who studies frogs, a man who owns a frog, or a man dressed in a frog costume.4 The notion of a pragmatically suitable relation is deliberately underspecified, enabling speakers to derive contextually appropriate interpretations on the fly.
GM can apply not only to individual-kind predicates (such as frog and man), but also to “eventuality-kind” predicates, thereby allowing the formation of V-NP-particles and adjectival resultatives. Consider an English V-NP-particle lift the table up. The verb lift combines with a small clause (4) (Kayne, 1985; Hoekstra, 1988; Den Dikken, 1995, a.o.). Extending Chierchia’s (1998) specific proposal of semantic kinds, Snyder assumes that the verb lift denotes a kind of activity, while the small clause complement denotes a kind of state. Applying GM, the entire phrase is interpreted as a subtype of the “lifting” event-kind that stands in a pragmatically suitable relation to the result state (the table being up).5
(4)[VP lift [SC the table up]]
Consider an adjectival resultative like wipe the table clean, whose syntactic structure is given in (5). In this structure, the verb wipe combines with the AP clean, and the object the table occupies the specifier position of the VP (5a). The verb wipe undergoes head movement and adjoins to v, thereby surfacing to the left of the table (5b). Snyder assumes that verb wipe denotes (in effect) a kind of activity, while clean denotes a kind of state. Applying GM, the whole phrase is construed as a subtype of the “wiping” event-kind that stands in a pragmatically suitable relation to the result state (the “clean” kind of state). The result is a kind of accomplish event, with “wiping” as its development and “clean” as its culmination.
(5)a. [VP [the table] [V’ wiped [AP clean]]]](before head movement)
b. [vP [wiped v] [VP [the table] [V’ <wiped> [AP clean]]]]
While GM being available ([+TCP]) enables the formation of certain complex predicates, such as V-NP-particles and adjectival resultatives, it does not guarantee their availability. Even among [+TCP] languages, the availability of specific complex predicates may vary. For example, Japanese, like English, is [+TCP], allowing endocentric N-N compounds as a fully productive and creative process. However, it does not permit V-NP-particles or “strong” resultatives of the English type (6).6
(6)Strong resultatives:
a. ??John-gakinzoku-opetyanko-nitatai-ta.
John-NOMmetal-ACC flat pound-PAST
“John pounded the metal flat.”
(Washio, 1997, p. 5, (16b))
b. *boku-wazibun-okutakuta-niodot-ta.
I-TOPself-ACCtireddance-PAST
“I danced myself tired.”
(Washio, 1997, p. 20, (67c))
Snyder (2012) proposed that the licensing of small clause complements requires the positive setting of another parameter, which he called the SCP, defined in (7). Specifically, languages vary with respect to whether a verb can take a small clause complement. [+SCP] languages (e.g., English) allow small clause complementation whereas [−SCP] languages (e.g., Japanese) do not (8). It is worth noting that there exist multiple views and definitions of small clauses. In Snyder’s usage, a small clause is characterized primarily by two properties: (i) its subject receives structural accusative case from its selecting verb, and (ii) if the predicate of the small clause is verbal, it appears in the form of a bare infinitive.
(7)The Small Clause Parameter (SCP)
The language (does/does not) allows small clause complements to V.
(8)Perceptual reports
a. English: John saw [SC Mary walk].
b. Japanese:
*John-ga [SC Mary-oaruku] mita.
John-NOM Mary-ACC walk see-PAST
“John saw Mary walk.”
(Snyder, 2012, p. 296, 11.24)
According to Snyder, the formation of English complex predicates in (1), including V-NP-particles, strong adjectival resultatives, DO datives, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports, requires the positive setting of the SCP, in the sense that the main verbs take small clauses as their complements. Table 1 summarizes the constructions that require a positive setting of TCP or SCP.

2.2. Previous Acquisition Studies

TCP and SCP make distinct yet interrelated predictions for the acquisition of creative endocentric N-N compounds and complex predicates. TCP predicts that the acquisition of creative endocentric N-N compounds, adjectival resultatives, and V-NP-particles are inherently linked, as all three structures depend on the positive setting of the parameter. SCP predicts that the acquisition of the complex predicates in (1), excluding weak resultatives, should be interrelated, as they all require the positive setting of SCP.
Previous acquisition studies report findings consistent with these predictions. Snyder and Stromswold (1997) investigated the acquisition of English complex predicates by analyzing longitudinal corpora from 12 English-acquiring children in the CHILDES database. Their results showed that DO datives, V-NP-particles, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports tend to emerge as a group, typically before age three. This finding is consistent with the predictions of SCP. However, it is possible that these constructions emerge together as part of the “grammar explosion”—a developmental phase in which children acquire multiple syntactic structures in rapid succession. This confound gives rise to a concern about the absence of control measures to rule out this alternative explanation.
Snyder (2001) investigated the relationship between creative N-N compounds and various complex predicates, including V-NP-particles, DO datives, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports in terms of children’s acquisition. Analyzing 10 children’s longitudinal corpora, this study incorporated rigorous controls to eliminate confounding factors like the “grammar explosion”. A key finding was that there was an exceptionally strong correlation between V-NP-particles and N-N compounds (r = 0.98, t(8) = 12.9, p < 0.0001) and a near identity relation in children’s age of acquisition of these two constructions. These findings were later replicated in Snyder’s (2007) expanded analysis of a larger sample (19 children), despite the fact that he used a different measure to estimate when children acquire certain constructions. Similar findings were reported for German in Hanink and Snyder (2014). These findings provide strong support for TCP.
As one may notice, none of the aforementioned corpus studies examined adjectival resultatives. This exclusion is due to their infrequent occurrence in spontaneous speech, both in child and adult production, making corpus analyses less reliable for studying their acquisition. For instance, Wang et al. (2020) examined longitudinal corpora from 20 English-acquiring children but found that only 10 of them produced at least one clear instance of an adjectival resultative. Furthermore, Wang et al. (2022) analyzed maternal input and found that adjectival resultatives occurred at low frequencies, ranging from 2 to 568 per 100,000 utterances, with a mean frequency of 30. Given this low frequency in spontaneous production, corpus-based methods may be unreliable for determining their age of acquisition (AoA), and behavioral experiments provide a more reliable approach to studying the acquisition of resultatives. Wang et al. (2022) conducted a Truth Value Judgment task to examine English-acquiring children’s comprehension of adjectival resultatives. The study tested children as early as experimentally feasible—shortly after their third birthday. The results showed that even at this early age, children were able to interpret adjectival resultatives, aligning with previous findings on the acquisition of novel N-N compounds and other complex predicates. Another experimental study supporting TCP comes from Sugisaki and Isobe (2000), who investigated Japanese-speaking children’s acquisition of resultatives and novel N-N compounds. Using a comprehension task, they assessed children’s understanding of resultatives, while an elicitation task was used to test their production of novel N-N compounds. Their findings revealed a strong association between the acquisition of resultatives and N-N compounding, with both structures emerging around age 3;06. (For a discussion of factors contributing to AoA differences between Japanese and English, see Wang et al. (2022).)

2.3. Summary and Remaining Question

In sum, previous acquisitional studies revealed that the acquisition of V-NP-particles and adjectival resultatives is closely correlated with the acquisition of novel N-N compounds, which supports TCP.
Compared to the observed correlation between V-NP-particles and resultatives with N-N compounds, the question of whether different complex predicates are interrelated in the way predicted by the SCP remains unresolved. Although Snyder and Stromswold (1997) reported that complex predicates tend to be acquired as a group, consistent with what the SCP predicts, their findings are potentially confounded by general developmental factors. While Snyder (2001) controlled for this confound, he did not examine the correlations among complex predicts themselves.7 As a result, it remains unclear whether these constructions are genuinely linked in terms of their acquisition, particularly after controlling for general developmental factors (such as “grammatical explosion”). In the next section, we present a new corpus study examining the acquisition of novel N-N compounds alongside various complex predicates, including V-NP-particles, put-locatives, DO datives, make-causatives and perceptual reports. Our main goal is to assess potential relations between the acquisition of these structures.
In addition, some prior corpus studies have been limited to 10 or 12 English-speaking children, raising concerns about generalizability. To address this concern, we conducted a larger-scale investigation to determine whether previous findings can be replicated and extended to a larger sample.

3. Corpus Study

3.1. Method

We analyzed the spontaneous production data of 23 children, whose longitudinal corpora are available from the English-North America and English-UK repositories of the CHILDES database (MacWhinney, 2000). These children’s ages fall within the range of 1;0 to 3;0 at the time of the initial transcript recording (see Table 2). This age range is determined based on the findings of Snyder and Stromswold (1997).
Our primary goal was to investigate potential links in children’s AoA across several constructions. Specifically, we examined (i) the association between novel N-N compounds and V-NP-particles, as predicted by TCP; and (ii) the interrelations among complex predicates (V-NP-particles, DO datives, put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports), as predicted by the SCP. (iii) Although put-locatives, DO datives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports (1c–f) are not directly tied to TCP, their acquisition may nonetheless correlate with that of N-N compounds if the triggers that English-acquiring children use to set TCP also contribute to setting SCP. Therefore, we also examined the relation between novel N-N compounds and these complex predicates.
To determine the AoA of a construction, we used the measure of First Regular Use (FRU), defined as the first use followed within a month by additional uses involving non-identical lexical items (Snyder, 2007). In cases where FRU could not be determined due to the low frequency of a construction in the corpus, we also noted the First Clear Use (FCU) for each child.
We first used a Python version 3.10 program together with Garber’s (2019) CHA file Python parser to identify, for each child, potential candidates for the following constructions: (a) novel endocentric N-N compounds; (b) V-NP-particles in lines containing a verb and a potential particle; (c) put-locatives in lines containing the verb put, place, or set along with a preposition; (d) DO datives; and (e) make-causatives in lines containing any form of the verb make followed by a bare infinitival verb with its NP “subject”; (f) perceptual reports in lines containing any form of the verbs hear, see, or watch followed by a bare infinitival verb with its NP “subject”. Next, manual searches were conducted. For constructions (b) through (f), two research assistants manually examined the Python output data and identified the first five occurrences of each construction. One of the authors then reviewed their annotations and determined the FRU/FCUs for each child. For (a) N-N compounds, the authors assessed each potential compound in the context of the transcript to determine whether it was truly novel based on two criteria: first, it could not be a lexicalized form (e.g., toothbrush, orange juice); second, the context supports the interpretation of the compound that the child created on the spot, meaning that it made sense within the context and had not been previously used by an adult in the corpus. The authors identified the first three to five clear uses of novel compounds before determining the FRU/FCUs for each child. Across all the constructions examined, we excluded imitations, routines, repetitions, and otherwise unclear utterances.

3.2. Data Analysis and Results

Table 3 reports the ages of FRU/FCU (in years) for the constructions under investigation. As noted in Section 3.1, we used the age of FRU as a measure for AoA. However, when FRU could not be determined from the corpus for a given child, FCU was used instead. For make-causatives and perceptual reports, FRU could be determined for only 5 out of 23 children (less than one-fourth), due to the low frequency of these two constructions in the corpora. Moreover, for 4 of these 5 children, the FRU and FCU ages were identical. Therefore, only FCU was reported for make-causatives and perceptual reports.
Given the limited data for make-causatives and perceptual reports, we set these constructions aside for now and focus on the others. We will return to make-causatives and perceptual reports later in the paper.

3.2.1. N-N Compounds, V-NP-Particles, Put-Locatives, and DO Datives

TCP predicts that the AoA of V-NP-particles should correlate with that of N-N compounds. SCP predicts that the AoAs of complex predicates (V-NP-particles, put-locatives, and DO datives) should be correlated with one another. To test these predictions, we conducted a series of correlation analyses examining the relationship between different construction pairs. Table 4 summarizes the correlation coefficients (r), t-values, degrees of freedom (df), and p-values.
The results revealed significant associations across all construction pairs, with some pairs exhibiting particularly strong correlations, such as N-N compounds and V-NP-particles, V-NP-particles and put-locatives, as well as put-locatives and DO datives. These results support the idea that N-N compounds and V-NP-particles are closely related (both requiring GM). Furthermore, the results suggest that the acquisition of these complex predicates relies on shared prerequisite(s), such as allowing the verb to take a small clause complement (i.e., the positive setting of the SCP).
However, alternative explanations for these results exist. As pointed out by Snyder (2001, 2007), the co-emergence of these structures could stem from a broader “grammatical explosion” phase, during which children’s processing capacity crosses a critical threshold, enabling these constructions to naturally enter their spoken repertoire. In other words, the grammatical prerequisites for these constructions might be entirely irrelevant.
To examine this possibility, we followed Snyder’s (2007) approach, using morpheme-based mean length of utterance (MLUm) as an index of children’s processing capacity. Moreover, we took the age at which a child’s MLUm first reaches a threshold as an estimate of when they attained the corresponding level of processing ability. Specifically, we calculated the average MLUm from each child’s transcript that contained the FRU of V-NP-particles. This construction is frequent in the corpora, making its FRU reliably determinable for each child. The resulting threshold, 2.134, closely aligns with the value reported in Snyder (2007), 1.919, despite differences in sample selection. For each child, we then calculated the age that their MLUm first reached this threshold, which we subsequently controlled for in our analyses.
We conducted partial correlation tests for each construction pair, using the age at which a child’s MLUm first reached 2.134 as a covariate. As shown in Table 5, most correlations remained statistically significant even after controlling for this developmental measure, suggesting that the observed AoA correlations among these constructions cannot be fully explained by general developmental effects. More specifically, the significant partial correlation between N-N compounds and V-NP-particles supports the hypothesis that both constructions are linked to TCP. In addition, the significant partial correlations between V-NP-particles and put-locatives, as well as between put-locatives and DO datives, provided support for the hypothesis that their acquisition shares some common prerequisite, such as the positive setting of the SCP.
Two additional patterns do not follow directly from the predictions of TCP and the SCP. The first is the observed correlation between N-N compounds and put-locatives; the second is the lack of correlation between DO datives and V-NP-particles, which we discuss next.
First, although put-locatives are not hypothesized to depend on the positive setting of TCP, their AoA was significantly correlated with that of N-N compounds, a pattern also observed in Snyder (2001). One possible explanation for this finding is that N-N compounds and put-locatives are indirectly linked through their shared association with V-NP-particles, even though their acquisition is governed by distinct parameters (TCP for N-N compounds, SCP for put-locatives). Here we propose that for English, the same set of linguistic triggers may be responsible for setting the values of both TCP and SCP.8 Two plausible candidates for these triggers are V-NP-particles and strong adjectival resultatives, as these structures are subsumed under both TCP and SCP. Comparing the two candidates, strong adjectival resultatives are less likely to serve as triggers for these parameters, because they are infrequent in spontaneous speech, as discussed in Section 2.2. Wang et al. (2022) found that resultatives occur at a very low frequency in English maternal input, averaging only 30 instances per 100,000 utterances. In contrast, they found that V-NP-particles are much more frequent in English maternal input, with an average of 3570 instances per 100,000 utterances. Given this disparity in input frequency, V-NP-particles are a more plausible candidate for serving as the trigger for both TCP and SCP. Their associated structures, N-N compounds which are subsumed under TCP, and put-locatives, which are subsumed under the SCP, are thus indirectly correlated.
Second, DO datives exhibit no statistically significant correlation with N-N compounds, and their correlation with V-NP-particles is only marginally significant. The lack of correlation with N-N compounds, also reported in Snyder (2001), is unsurprising, given that the acquisition of DO datives is not predicted to depend on the positive setting of TCP. The latter pattern, however, is unexpected, given that both DO datives and V-NP-particles are hypothesized to require the positive setting of the SCP. We consider two possible explanations for this finding: First, it is possible that DO datives do not actually depend on the positive setting of the SCP. If this is the case, there would be no expectation that DO datives and V-NP-particles should be related to one another in their timing of acquisition. A second possibility is that DO datives do depend on the positive setting of the SCP, but also require some additional prerequisite, which is acquired late by at least some children. In this case, the positive setting of the SCP (evidenced by V-NP-particles becoming available in child’s speech) alone would be a relatively weak predictor of when DO datives became available in a child’s speech. This explanation further predicts that children’s ages of acquisition for DO datives will be either the same as, or later than, the corresponding ages for V-NP-particles. To test this, we conducted a pairwise t-test comparing children’s age of FRU for V-NP-particles and DO datives. The results showed that DOs are acquired significantly later than V-NP-particles (t(22) = 5.865, p < 0.001). Thus, despite the relative “noisiness” of the data for DO datives (and without yet identifying the proposed late-acquired prerequisite), the available evidence is still consistent with the hypothesis that DO has the positive setting of the SCP as one of its prerequisites.
Along similar lines of reasoning, if V-NP-particles serve as the trigger for TCP and the SCP, we make a further prediction that their associated constructions, N-N compounds and put-locatives, should not be acquired prior to V-NP-particles; rather they should emerge around the same time as or later than V-NP-particles. To test this prediction, we conducted pairwise t-tests for each construction pair to investigate potential ordering effects, including the previously reported ordering between DO datives and V-NP-particles. The results, summarized in Table 6, are consistent with our prediction: N-N compounds are acquired significantly later than V-NP-particles, and put-locatives are acquired significantly later than V-NP-particles. However, N-N compounds and put-locatives show no consistent ordering effect, because the acquisition of the two constructions is governed by different parameters. DO datives are acquired significantly later than other constructions subsumed under the SCP, V-NP-particle and put-locatives, suggesting an additional prerequisite for acquisition, which we will discuss in Section 4.

3.2.2. Make-Causative Constructions

We next examine make-causative constructions. First, we excluded children for whom the FRU or FCU of make-causatives was not found in the corpora, resulting in a dataset of 16 children. We then conducted a series of correlation tests to examine the relationship between make-causatives and the other constructions under investigation. If the acquisition of make-causatives requires the positive setting of the SCP, we predict that it should correlate with that of some other complex predicates subsumed under the same parameter. In contrast, we do not predict such a correlation with N-N compounds, as make-causatives do not depend on the positive setting of TCP.
As shown in Table 7, while all correlations were positive, none reached statistical significance. We propose two possible explanations for the lack of significance correlation. One possibility is that the acquisition of make-causatives does not require the positive setting of SCP, but instead depends on distinct linguistic prerequisites. Another possibility is that the low frequency of this construction inherently produces “noisier” data, making it harder to detect correlations in spontaneous production. This is supported by the lack of FRU in most children (18 out of 23) and the absence of both FRU and FCU in about one-third of children (7 out of 23) in our sample.

3.2.3. Perceptual Reports

In this section, we examine perceptual reports, another low-frequency construction, as evidenced by the absence of FRU/FCU in several children in our sample. Following the approach used for other constructions, we first excluded children for whom the FRU or FCU for perceptual reports was not present in the corpora, resulting in a dataset of 16 children. We then conducted a series of correlation tests to examine the relationship between perceptual reports and other constructions under investigation. If perceptual reports require the positive setting of the SCP, their acquisition should correlate with that of other complex predicates. On the other hand, since they do not require the positive setting of TCP, no such correlation is expected with N-N compounding.
The results, summarized in Table 8, showed that perceptual report constructions were correlated with other complex predicates but not with N-N compounds, suggesting that their acquisition requires the positive setting of the SCP but not TCP.
Next, we controlled for developmental factors using the same approach as in Section 3.2.1. Specifically, we calculated the average MLUm from transcripts where children’s FRU of V-NP-particles was observed, which was 2.219. We then calculated the age at which each child’s MLUm first exceeded this threshold. Partial correlation analyses were conducted while controlling for this covariate. The results are summarized in Table 9.
The results revealed a significant correlation between perceptual reports and DO datives, suggesting that their acquisition shares a common prerequisite, such as the positive setting of the SCP. However, only marginally significant correlations were observed between perceptual reports and V-NP-particles or put-locatives, and (as expected) no significant correlations were found between perceptual reports and N-N compounds.
Similar to our analysis of DO datives, we conducted pairwise t-tests to examine whether there is an ordering effect between perceptual reports and other complex predicates, particularly V-NP-particles, which may serve as a trigger for the SCP. This analysis allowed us to assess whether the acquisition of perceptual reports is truly independent from the acquisition of other complex predicates. The results are summarized in Table 10, where we also included results on DO datives to verify previous results regarding the ordering relationship between DO datives, V-NP-particles, and put-locatives.
The results showed that, like DO datives, perceptual reports are acquired significantly later than V-NP-particles and put-locatives, suggesting that their acquisition may depend on the positive setting of SCP, along with additional prerequisites.9 At the same time, we emphasize that, similar to make-causatives, the low frequency of perceptual reports in children’s spontaneous speech may introduce noise, making it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about their relationship to other complex predicates. As such, the results should be interpreted with caution.

4. Discussion

In Section 3, we analyzed data from 23 children to explore potential associations among six constructions: novel N-N compounds, V-NP-particles, put-locatives, DO datives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports. We summarize the key findings below: First, children’s acquisition of V-NP-particles correlates with that of N-N compounds, supporting the hypothesis that both depend on the positive setting of TCP. Second, we found associations between V-NP-particles and put-locatives, suggesting that these constructions share common linguistic underpinnings, such as the positive setting of the SCP. Third, DO datives and perceptual reports showed no significant correlations with V-NP-particles but emerged later in children’s spontaneous speech, suggesting that they may involve not only the positive setting of the SCP but also additional prerequisites. In contrast, make-causatives did not show correlation with any other constructions, suggesting that their acquisition may not rely on either TCP or SCP. However, results for make-causatives and perceptual reports should be interpreted with caution due to their low frequency in children’s spontaneous speech. For such low-frequency constructions, behavioral experiments may provide a more reliable means for investigating children’s acquisitional trajectories, an avenue we leave for future research. Overall, our findings align with Snyder’s (1995 et seq.) parametric approach to complex word formation and complex predicates.
We next discuss the broader implications of these findings and outline some remaining questions for future research.

4.1. TCP and SCP Setting Across Languages: Focusing on English and Japanese

According to Snyder (2001), TCP has an initial, unmarked setting: “−“. This means that the child initially has a grammar that does not permit GM. At some point, if the child encounters evidence that her language allows structures requiring the application of GM (such as V-NP-particles), she will set the parameter to its marked value “+”.
In the same spirit, and assuming a system of “subset parameters” in the sense of Wexler and Manzini (1987), the SCP also has an initial, unmarked value “−”. That is, a child starts with a grammar that disallows verbs from taking small clause complements. If she later encounters structures that requires small clause complementation, she will switch the setting of SCP from “−” to “+”.
For children acquiring English, we have proposed that V-NP-particles may serve as the trigger for setting both TCP and SCP. This proposal is based on two findings: First, the acquisition of N-N compounds is significantly correlated with that of put-locatives, although the two constructions fall under different parameters; and second, V-NP-particles are acquired significantly earlier than the other constructions under discussion. These observations suggest that V-NP-particles may play a central role in setting both parameters.
A related question arises for languages like Japanese, which are [+TCP, −SCP] and lack V-NP-particle constructions: How do children acquiring such languages determine the parameter values?
Snyder (2016) and Wang et al. (2022) argue that children acquiring different languages may rely on different structures and traverse distinct paths to reach the same parametric setting for TCP. They propose two candidate structures that might enable Japanese-acquiring children to determine that their target language is [+TCP]. One candidate is (weak) resultatives. If learners can recognize a resultative construction encodes both a primary activity and a secondary change of state within a single VP, this could signal that the target language allows GM. Another candidate is recursive compounding,10 based on Namiki’s (1994) generalization that a language allows bare-stem endocentric compounding if and only if it also allows recursive compounding (e.g., peanut butter sandwich in English or gakusee eiga kenkyuukai “student film club” in Japanese). Exposure to a robust number of recursive endocentric compounds may allow learners to infer that the language permits endocentric compounding as a creative process, leading to a [+TCP] setting. Wang et al. (2022) further suggest that, for Japanese, recursive compounding is a more plausible trigger for [+TCP] than resultatives, given their frequency in child-directed speech: on average, resultatives occur only about 2 times per 100,000 utterances, whereas recursive compounds appear around 210 times per 100,000 utterances.11
Before ending this subsection, we add a brief note on the setting of SCP and the acquisition of resultatives. As discussed earlier, children acquiring Japanese are assumed to begin with the unmarked [−SCP] setting and to shift to [+SCP] only when they encounter supporting evidence. This raises the question of whether exposure to weak resultatives (despite their rarity) might lead Japanese-acquiring children to mistakenly identify their language as [+SCP], given that weak resultatives superficially resemble strong resultatives and could be analyzed as involving a small clause structure. The absence of small clause structures in adult Japanese (e.g., V-NP-particles, strong resultatives, perceptual reports) suggests two possible acquisition paths: One possibility is that children temporarily adopt the incorrect [+SCP] setting upon encountering weak resultatives, but later backtrack when they fail to find further evidence that would support the positive setting. Another possibility, which we find more plausible, is that children are “grammatically conservative” (see Snyder 2007, 2011, 2021 for other independent evidence): they retain the unmarked [−SCP] setting unless and until they encounter robust, unambiguous positive evidence for [+SCP]. Only then do they commit to the marked value. The main reason to favor this conservative acquisition path is that weak resultatives do not necessitate a small clause analysis. They are compatible with alternative syntactic structures that lack a small clause (e.g., (5)) and therefore fail to provide unambiguous triggering evidence for [+SCP]. This raises a further question: Given the surface similarity between strong and week resultatives, do English-acquiring children distinguish between them and, if so, how? We leave this question for future research.

4.2. Prerequisite for Acquiring DO Datives

In Section 3, we proposed that the acquisition of DO datives requires not only the positive setting of SCP, but also some additional prerequisite. Here, we speculate on what this prerequisite might be, in light of the delayed emergence of DO datives compared to V-NP-particles and put-locatives. One possibility is that children need to recognize the connection between DO datives and the syntax of possession. According to Harley (2002), a language will not permit surface structures in which a “possessor” NP c-commands a “possession” NP, such as DO datives, unless it has PHAVE, a prepositional element expressing possession. In fact, researchers have argued that English DO datives involve a small clause headed by an element like PHAVE (Green, 1974; a.o.). On this view, a sentence like John gave Mary the book expresses a caused-possession meaning: John’s giving of the book caused Mary to have the book. If this analysis is correct, acquiring DO datives requires more than recognizing the presence of a small clause. English-acquiring children must also posit an abstract functional head like PHAVE, and determine that it heads the lower VP shell. This process may also involve understanding how possession is grammatically encoded in English, and adding PHAVE to the inventory of abstract functional elements that are allowed to occur in a syntactic numeration. Developing these may take extra time, beyond what is needed to set the SCP.

4.3. On the Relation Between English To-Datives and Other Complex Predicates

Another open question concerns the relation between English to-datives (e.g., give a book to John) and the complex predicates discussed in this paper. According to Snyder and Stromswold (1997), to-datives share some features with other complex predicates, particularly DO datives, suggesting that their acquisition should be related.
Previous studies have consistently found that to-datives are acquired significantly later than DO datives (e.g., Campbell & Tomasello, 2001; Snyder & Stromswold, 1997; Viau, 2007). Campbell and Tomasello (2001) attributed this ordering effect to an asymmetry in linguistic input, arguing that the earlier acquisition of DO datives results from their greater frequency in child-directed speech. In contrast, Snyder and Stromswold (1997) proposed that while to-datives share some common property with DO datives (and other complex predicates), to-datives require an additional prerequisite that DO datives lack, which explains their later acquisition. Specifically, they suggested that this extra requirement could be children’s understanding of the dative use of to. Supporting this hypothesis, they found that children’s acquisition of to-datives was significantly correlated with their acquisition of dative to in dyadic constructions (e.g., This bag belongs to John; Something happened to Mary).
Following Snyder and Stromswold’s (1997) proposal, the acquisition of to-datives should be related with the acquisition of other complex predicates. Additionally, their account predicts a developmental ordering effect, where to-datives should be acquired later than other complex predicates. This issue requires further investigation, and future research may consider including to-datives as part of their evaluation of the SCP.

5. Conclusions

To conclude, in this study we conducted a larger-scale corpus analysis to examine the parametric accounts for the acquisition of complex predicates (V-NP-particles, put-locatives, DO datives, causatives, and perceptual reports) and complex word formation (creative endocentric N-N compounds). By examining the partial correlation between their acquisition and ordering effects, we aimed to determine whether there are genuine parametric dependencies among these constructions. The results provide supporting evidence for TCP and SCP, shedding light on the potential triggers (i.e., V-NP-particles) that English-acquiring children use to set the values of these two parameters. We hope this work advances our understanding of how parametric variation influences language acquisition and elucidates the developmental interplay among complex predicates and endocentric N-N compounding.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.X. and S.W.; methodology, T.X. and S.W.; formal analysis, T.X. and S.W.; data curation, T.X. and S.W.; writing—original draft, T.X. and S.W.; writing—review and editing, T.X. and S.W.; project administration, T.X. and S.W.; funding acquisition, T.X. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by National Social Science Fund of China, grant number 22BYY076.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All data were obtained from the CHILDES database (https://childes.talkbank.org/), accessed on 23 February 2024.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our sincere gratitude to William Snyder for his invaluable guidance, feedback, and support throughout the development of this project. We are also grateful to the reviewers and editor of Languages for their comments and suggestions. Special thanks go to Zeyuan Yang for developing and executing the Python script used in our analysis. We also thank Yujie Cai, Tianchu Han, Boyao Wang, Shiyang Wu, Jingyun Zhang, and Yanxi Zhu for their assistance in annotating children’s spontaneous speech data.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this article.

Notes

1
We primarily use the term “V-NP-particle” in this paper because it is more structurally transparent and concise, though it is interchangeable with “separable verb-particle”. The latter is used only when contrasting with inseparable verb-particle constructions (e.g., lift up the table), where no NP intervenes between the verb and the particle. Although we use NP, we do not intend to commit to any particular analysis regarding the NP vs. DP distinction.
2
Note that in Snyder’s earlier works (e.g., Snyder, 1995, 2001), he attempted to link all complex predicates in (1) to TCP. However, more recently, he has revised his proposal, suggesting that only V-NP-particles and adjectival resultatives require a positive TCP setting, whereas the other complex predicates depend on the SCP (to be elaborated later) and not on TCP (Snyder, 2025, personal communication). In this paper, we will follow his most recent proposal.
3
Over the years, Snyder has proposed several possibilities regarding what underlies the formation of novel N-N compounding. Accordingly, the formulation of TCP has evolved. In this paper, we adopt his most recent proposal.
4
A reviewer asked how head-directionality fits within GM, given that (2) does not specify the linear order between α and β. In English, the head of compounds is typically the right-hand element (cf. the Righthand Head Rule proposed by Williams (1981)). However, some [+TCP] languages (e.g., Thai) exhibit left-headed compounding. This suggests that while TCP regulates the availability of creative N-N compounding, head directionality, which is subject to cross-linguistic variation, is determined by independent principles.
5
Note that we focus on separable verb-particle constructions because, in inseparable verb-particle combinations, it can be difficult to determine which part serves as the primary predicate. For example, Russian, as a [−TCP] language, has inseparable “prepositional prefixes” that can function as the primary predicate rather than a secondary predicate. However, particles that can be separated from the verb are less likely to function as the primary predicate.
6
There are at least two types of adjectival resultatives: weak and strong resultatives (Washio, 1997). A weak resultative involves a non-arbitrary relation between the verb and the adjective phrase, where the verb is necessarily transitive and its meaning entails that if the direct object undergoes a change of state, this change occurs in one of a very small number of expected ways. For instance, in a table-wiping event, the action of wiping is expected to affect the table in a predictable manner, typically making it cleaner or drier, rather than dirtier or wetter. Thus, wipe the table clean is a weak resultative. In contrast, hammer the metal flat exemplifies a strong resultative, where the verb’s meaning does not inherently entail any expectation of the resulting flatness. Unlike weak resultatives, the connection between the verb and the resulting state is not predictable from the verb’s meaning alone. According to Snyder (2012) and Wang et al. (2022), [+TCP] is a sufficient prerequisite for the availability of weak resultatives but not for strong resultatives. The latter also requires the positive setting of a different parameter, the SCP, to be fully licensed in a language.
  The two types of adjectival resultatives are structurally distinct. In weak resultatives, the verb combines with a full AP (as in (5), repeated in (i-a)). In such cases, GM succeeds only if the meaning of the AP corresponds to an outcome expected from the verb’s semantics. In contrast, in strong resultatives, the verb combines with a small clause (as in (ii-a)), which by itself establishes a subject-predicate relation that holds at the culmination point of the event. As a result, unlike weak resultatives, strong resultatives do not exhibit the obligatory, non-arbitrary connection between the verb and the AP. In other words, the verb’s meaning does not entail the directions of the possible change of state.
(i) Weak resultative:(Wang et al., 2022, p. 7, (15))
a. John [vP v [VP [the table] [V’ wiped [AP clean]]]](before head movement)
b. John [vP [wiped v] [VP [the table] [V’ <wiped> [AP clean]]]]
(ii) Strong resultative:(adapted from Wang et al., 2022, p. 7, (16))
a. John [vP v [VP [V’ hammered [SC the metal flat]]]] (before head movement)
b. John [vP [hammered v] [VP [V’ <hammered> [SC the metal flat]]]]
7
Instead, he focused on whether the acquisition of different complex predicates correlates with that of novel N-N compounds, as he originally proposed that all these complex predicates in English require the positive setting of TCP. He found significant correlations between novel N-N compounding and put-locatives, make-causatives, and perceptual reports, although the correlation between DO datives and N-N compounds was only marginally significant after controlling for potential confounds. We will return to some of these findings in the next section.
8
Note that here, our discussion of the trigger for parameter setting is limited to English. We assume that the trigger for a parameter (e.g., [+TCP]) may differ for children acquiring different languages. In Section 4, we will consider what might trigger the setting of TCP in a language like Japanese, which lacks V-NP-particles but is [+TCP].
9
We speculate that one such prerequisite may stem from the complexity in determining what form a verbal predicate will take in a small-clause or small-clause-like complement, such as those found in perceptual reports. In English, both bare infinitives (e.g., John saw Mary leave) and present participles (e.g., John saw Mary leaving) are allowed, with a subtle semantic difference between the two. However, this is not cross-linguistically uniform. For example, while German allows bare infinites in perceptual report complements (Ich sah Maria gehen, literally “I saw Mary leave”), it disallows present participles (*Ich sah Maria gehend, literally “I saw Mary leaving”). As such, children must acquire language-specific aspectual marking and determine which forms are permissible in perceptual reports in their target language.
10
In principle, creative N-N compounds could serve as a potential trigger for [+TCP]: children might recognize such compounds in their input and conclude that their target language allows GM. However, a general issue with this approach is that learners have no reliable way of determining whether a compound they encounter is truly novel or lexicalized. Since [−TCP] languages (e.g., Spanish) permit lexicalized compounds, the mere presence of N-N compounds in the input is not a reliable cue for identifying a language as [+TCP].
11
Wang et al.’s (2022) findings on input frequency support the idea that English-acquiring children primarily rely on V-NP-particles to determine that their target language is [+TCP], given their substantially higher frequency in child-directed speech (averaging 3570 per 100,000 utterances), compared to recursive N-N compounds (217 per 100,000 utterances) and adjectival resultatives (30 per 100,000 utterances).

References

  1. Braunwald, S. R. (1971). Mother-child communication: The function of maternal-language input. Word, 27(1–3), 28–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of English dative constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 253–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Chierchia, G. (1998). Reference to kinds across languages. Natural Language Semantics, 6, 339–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Demuth, K., & McCullough, E. (2009). The prosodic (re)organization of children’s early English articles. Journal of Child Language, 36, 173–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Den Dikken, M. (1995). Particles: On the syntax of verb-particle, triadic, and causative constructions. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Garber, L. (2019). CHA file python parser. Zenodo. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Green, G. M. (1974). Semantics and syntactic regularity. Indiana University Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Hanink, E. A., & Snyder, W. (2014). Particles and compounds in German: Evidence for the compounding parameter. Language Acquisition, 21(2), 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Harley, H. (2002). Possession and the double object construction. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 2, 29–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hoekstra, T. (1988). Small clause results. Lingua, 74(2–3), 101–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jones, G., & Rowland, C. F. (2017). Diversity not quantity in caregiver speech: Using computational modeling to isolate the effects of the quantity and the diversity of the input on vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology, 98, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Kayne, R. (1985). Principles of particle constructions. In J. Guéron, H.-G. Obenauer, & J.-Y. Pollock (Eds.), Grammatical representation (pp. 101–140). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar]
  14. MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: The database (Vol. 2). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  15. Namiki, T. (1994). Subheads of compounds. In S. Chiba, & T. Nakao (Eds.), Synchronic and diachronic approaches to language: A festschrift for Toshio Nakao on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday (pp. 269–285). Liber Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Snyder, W. (1995). Language acquisition and language variation: The role of morphology [Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology]. [Google Scholar]
  18. Snyder, W. (2001). On the nature of syntactic variation: Evidence from complex predicates and complex word-formation. Language, 77(2), 324–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Snyder, W. (2007). Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  20. Snyder, W. (2011). Children’s grammatical conservatism: Implications for syntactic theory. In N. Danis, K. Mesh, & H. Sung (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual Boston university conference on language development (Vol. 1, pp. 1–20). Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Snyder, W. (2012). Parameter theory and motion predicates. In V. Demonte, & L. McNally (Eds.), Telicity, change, and state: A cross-categorial view of event structure (OSTL 39) (pp. 279–299). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Snyder, W. (2016). How to set the compounding parameter. In L. Perkins, R. Dudley, J. Gerard, & K. Hitczenko (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th conference on generative approaches to language acquisition North America (GALANA 2015) (pp. 122–130). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
  23. Snyder, W. (2021). A parametric approach to the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 48(5), 862–887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Snyder, W., & Stromswold, K. (1997). The structure and acquisition of English dative constructions. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 281–317. [Google Scholar]
  25. Son, M. (2007). Directionality and resultativity: The cross-linguistic correlation revisited. In M. Bašić, M. Pantcheva, M. Son, & P. Svenonius (Eds.), Nordlyd, Tromsø working papers in language & linguistics 34 (2), special issue on space, motion, and result (pp. 126–164). University of Tromsø. [Google Scholar]
  26. Sugisaki, K., & Isobe, M. (2000). Resultatives result from the compounding parameter: On the acquisitional correlation between resultatives and N-N compounds in Japanese. In R. Billerey, & B. D. Lillehaugen (Eds.), Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics (Vol. 19, pp. 493–506). Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Suppes, P. (1974). The semantics of children’s language. American Psychologist, 29, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Theakston, A., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2001). The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Viau, J. (2007). Possession and Spatial Motion in the Acquisition of Ditransitives [Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University]. [Google Scholar]
  30. Wang, S., Kido, Y., & Snyder, W. (2020). Acquisition of English resultatives: Support for the compounding parameter. In M. M. Brown, & A. Kohut (Eds.), Proceedings of the 44th annual Boston university conference on child language development [BUCLD 44] (pp. 717–723). Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wang, S., Kido, Y., & Snyder, W. (2022). Acquisition of English adjectival resultatives: Support for the compounding parameter. Language Acquisition, 29(3), 229–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Washio, R. (1997). Resultatives, compositionality and language variation. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 6, 1–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wexler, K., & Manzini, M. R. (1987). Parameters and learnability in binding theory. In T. Roeper, & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter Setting (pp. 41–76). Reidel. [Google Scholar]
  34. Williams, E. (1981). On the notions” lexically related” and “head of a word”. Linguistic Inquiry, 12(2), 245–274. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Constructions requiring the positive setting of TCP or SCP.
Table 1. Constructions requiring the positive setting of TCP or SCP.
Require Positive Setting of TCP?Require Positive Setting of the SCP?
N-N compoundYes
Weak adjectival resultativeYes
Strong adjectival resultativeYesYes
V-NP-particleYesYes
DO dative Yes
Put-locative Yes
Make-causative Yes
Perceptual report Yes
Collection NameCorpusChild NameAge Range (Year; Month)No. of
Transcripts
No. of Child Utterances
Eng-UKLaraLara1;09–3;0312060,441
Eng-UKManchesterAnne1;10–3;083522,687
Eng-UKManchester Aran1;11–2;103318,293
Eng-UKManchesterCarl1;08–2;083326,280
Eng-UKManchesterDominic1;10–2;103524,239
Eng-UKManchesterGail1;11–2;113418,462
Eng-UKManchesterJoel1;11–2;1035 21,374
Eng-UKManchesterJohn1;11–2;1032 14,449
Eng-UKManchesterLiz1;11–2;1033 16,457
Eng-UKManchesterRuth1;11–2;1133 21,433
Eng-UKManchesterWarren1;10–2;0936 17,813
Eng-NABraunwaldLaura1;05–7;0020040,860
Eng-NABrownAdam2;03–5;025546,743
Eng-NABrownEve1;06–2;032012,113
Eng-NABrownSarah2;03–5;0113938,096
Eng-NANelsonEmily1;07–2;08544893
Eng-NA SuppesNina1;11–3;035233,181
Eng-NAProvidenceAlex1;04–3;055129,252
Eng-NAProvidenceEthan0;11–2;1150 21,920
Eng-NA ProvidenceLily1;01–4;0080 40,027
Eng-NA ProvidenceNaima0;11–3;1088 43,500
Eng-NA ProvidenceViolet1;02–3;1151 17,296
Eng-NA ProvidenceWilliam1;04–3;0444 21,291
Table 3. Ages of FRU/FCU (in years) for each construction type and child. For N-N compounds, V-NP-particles, put-locatives, and DO datives, FRU was reported unless otherwise marked. An asterisk (*) indicates cases where FCU was used as an approximation for AoA. For make-causatives and perceptual reports, FRU could not be determined for most children due to their low frequency, so only FCU was reported. A slash (/) indicates that neither FRU nor FCU was available.
Table 3. Ages of FRU/FCU (in years) for each construction type and child. For N-N compounds, V-NP-particles, put-locatives, and DO datives, FRU was reported unless otherwise marked. An asterisk (*) indicates cases where FCU was used as an approximation for AoA. For make-causatives and perceptual reports, FRU could not be determined for most children due to their low frequency, so only FCU was reported. A slash (/) indicates that neither FRU nor FCU was available.
ChildN-N CompoundV-NP-ParticlePut-LocativeDO DativeCausative *Perceptual
Report *
Lara2.151.881.932.522.562.66
Anne2.051.932.052.33//
Aran1.992.012.082.392.722.62
Carl1.921.732.002.242.591.73
Dominic2.212.172.252.342.67/
Gail2.121.992.112.182.112.95
Joel2.09 *2.072.392.572.32/
John2.001.962.242.05 *//
Liz2.041.962.042.882.322.86
Ruth2.34 *2.262.612.492.612.72
Warren1.851.852.182.49/2.60
Laura2.221.761.761.573.281.56
Emily1.961.821.821.821.862.59
Nina1.962.032.052.102.07/
Alex2.442.322.522.522.322.86
Ethan1.291.461.551.941.82/
Lily1.781.992.21 *2.382.512.53
Naima1.381.551.721.77/1.91
Violet2.421.821.912.262.28/
William2.302.272.422.73/2.61
Adam2.262.262.262.26/2.34
Eve1.921.751.751.83/1.67
Sarah2.60 *2.552.852.832.852.96
Table 4. Correlation between different constructions pairs (n = 23).
Table 4. Correlation between different constructions pairs (n = 23).
Comparisonrtdfp
N-N compound vs. V-NP-particle0.7966.01921<0.001
N-N compound vs. put-locative0.6854.31421<0.001
N-N compound vs. DO dative0.4902.577210.018
V-NP-particle vs. put-locative0.93111.69321<0.001
V-NP-particle vs. DO dative0.6744.18521<0.001
put-locative vs. DO dative0.7254.81921<0.001
Table 5. Partial correlation between different constructions pairs (n = 23).
Table 5. Partial correlation between different constructions pairs (n = 23).
Comparisonrtdfp
N-N compound vs. V-NP-particle0.7264.72020<0.001
N-N compound vs. put-locative0.5512.950200.008
N-N compound vs. DO dative0.2621.212200.240
V-NP-particle vs. put-locative0.8717.91620<0.001
V-NP-particle vs. DO dative0.4122.021200.057
put-locative vs. DO dative0.5162.693200.014
Table 6. Pairwise t-tests between different construction pairs (n = 23).
Table 6. Pairwise t-tests between different construction pairs (n = 23).
Comparisontdfp
N-N compound vs. V-NP-particle2.143220.043
N-N compound vs. Put-locative−1.190220.247
N-N compound vs. DO dative−3.317220.003
V-NP-particle vs. Put-locative−5.84422<0.001
V-NP-particle vs. DO dative−5.86522<0.001
Put-locative vs. DO dative−3.279220.003
Table 7. Correlation between make-causatives and other constructions (n = 16).
Table 7. Correlation between make-causatives and other constructions (n = 16).
Comparisonrtdfp
N-N compound vs. make-causative0.4912.110140.053
DO vs. make-causative0.0980.370140.717
V-NP-particle vs. make-causative0.3181.253140.231
Put-locative vs. make-causative0.3081.210140.246
Table 8. Correlation between perceptual reports and other constructions (n = 16).
Table 8. Correlation between perceptual reports and other constructions (n = 16).
Comparisonrtdfp
N-N compound vs. perceptual report0.4461.866140.083
DO dative vs. perceptual report0.7434.158140.001
V-NP-particle vs. perceptual report0.6653.332140.005
put-locative vs. perceptual report0.6523.214140.006
Table 9. Partial correlation between perceptual reports and other constructions (n = 16).
Table 9. Partial correlation between perceptual reports and other constructions (n = 16).
Comparisonrtdfp
N-N compound vs. perceptual report0.1600.583130.570
DO dative vs. perceptual report0.5942.661130.020
V-NP-particle vs. perceptual report0.4591.862130.085
put-locative vs. perceptual report0.4511.821130.092
Table 10. Pairwise t-tests between perceptual reports and other complex predicates (n = 16).
Table 10. Pairwise t-tests between perceptual reports and other complex predicates (n = 16).
Comparisontdfp
DO dative vs. V-NP-particle4.392150.001
DO dative vs. put-locative2.514150.024
DO dative vs. perceptual report−1.788150.094
V-NP-particle vs. put-locative−4.671150.000
V-NP-particle vs. perceptual report−5.105150.000
put-locative vs. perceptual report−3.451150.004
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Xu, T.; Wang, S. On the Acquisition of English Complex Predicates and Complex Word Formation: Revisiting the Parametric Approach. Languages 2025, 10, 201. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080201

AMA Style

Xu T, Wang S. On the Acquisition of English Complex Predicates and Complex Word Formation: Revisiting the Parametric Approach. Languages. 2025; 10(8):201. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080201

Chicago/Turabian Style

Xu, Ting, and Shuyan Wang. 2025. "On the Acquisition of English Complex Predicates and Complex Word Formation: Revisiting the Parametric Approach" Languages 10, no. 8: 201. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080201

APA Style

Xu, T., & Wang, S. (2025). On the Acquisition of English Complex Predicates and Complex Word Formation: Revisiting the Parametric Approach. Languages, 10(8), 201. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080201

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop