1. Introduction
In 2019, 2593 fatalities and 19619 injuries of pedestrians under the age of 15 years were recorded across the country by the National Bureau of Statistics of China (
Xinhua, 2021). Fatal collisions and injuries in adolescents are frequent because their cognitive and perceptive skills have not reached their full development (
Adminaite et al., 2018). Adolescents, characterized by limited cognitive abilities, high impulsivity and inaccurate assessments of danger, are more likely to engage in risky behaviours such as running red lights and abruptly accelerating across roads (e.g.,
Biassoni et al., 2018;
Wang et al., 2022). To reduce the occurrence of such traffic accidents among young pedestrians, many studies have examined the characteristics of pedestrians’ crossing behaviours and their influencing factors (e.g.,
Salducco et al., 2022;
Wang et al., 2022;
Zhou & Horrey, 2010). Although many studies have demonstrated that safety attitudes and peer and parental influences have important impacts on the safety of pedestrian behaviours (e.g.,
Liu et al., 2021;
Papadimitriou et al., 2013;
Soole et al., 2011), few studies have examined the interactions between safety attitudes, social norms and the family climate on the crossing behaviour of young pedestrians aged 10–18 years.
1.1. Pedestrian Behaviour
Typically, self-reported risk behaviours as a pedestrian are commonly examined in the literature. In this context, several self-reported instruments have been developed.
Moyano-Díaz (
1997) developed the Pedestrian Behaviour Questionnaire (PBQ) in Chile. The PBQ contains 26 items divided into four factors: violation, error, lapses, and aggressive behaviour. Studies using the PBQ have revealed that male pedestrians commit more violations than female pedestrians do (
Moyano-Díaz, 1997), and more errors are observed among young pedestrians (18–24 years) than among older pedestrians (
Liu et al., 2021).
Granié et al. (
2013) constructed the Pedestrian Behaviour Scale (PBS). The PBS has 32 items and is divided into 4 factors. Transgression behaviour is defined as making decisions that put pedestrians at risk, whether they violate the law or not. Lapses are defined as maladaptive behaviours associated with a lack of focus on a task. Positive behaviours are defined as those that soothe social interactions. Aggressive behaviour is defined as conflicting behaviour with other road users. The PBS by
Granié et al. (
2013) focuses on changes in the street crossing behaviour of pedestrians with ages ranging from 15 years to 78 years. The PBS was later validated in other countries (
Vandroux et al., 2022), such as Serbia (
Antić et al., 2016) and the USA (
Deb et al., 2017).
Granié et al. (
2013) reported that individuals whose age is less than 35 years tend to commit more transgressions, offences and errors than those whose age is greater than 45 years. However, whether the original PBS can be used for younger people (aged 10–18 years) still needs more investigation.
Risk-taking behaviours during adolescence are well known, and numerous studies have focused on young teens’ risky behaviours as pedestrians and/or driver licence candidates (
Biassoni et al., 2018;
Elliott & Baughan, 2004;
Salducco et al., 2022;
Zhou & Horrey, 2010).
Salducco et al. (
2022) examined the relationships between crossing behaviours during early adolescence (ages 10~14) and risk perception and reported that risky behaviours while travelling as a pedestrian were more widespread among ninth-grade students and those who were more independent. Similarly,
Liu et al. (
2021) reported that younger people aged 18–24 years reported a greater risk of pedestrian behaviour than older adults aged over 34 years. To our knowledge, few studies have examined the characteristics of the crossing behaviour of young pedestrians aged 10–18 years and their influencing factors in China.
1.2. Theoretical Framework
It is widely believed that the formation of all behaviour is a process wherein external factors interact with personally held dispositions (
Green & Kreuter, 2005). Following this theoretical logic, several studies have examined the interactions of personal variables (internal factors) and situational variables (external factors) in the field of driving behaviour and reported that personal and external variables jointly affect driving behaviour (e.g.,
Carpentier et al., 2014;
Yang et al., 2022). For instance,
Carpentier et al. (
2014) explored the moderating role of attitudes in the relationship between a family safety climate (i.e., noncommitment) and risky driving behaviours and reported that greater noncommitment was associated with more risky driving when young drivers had risk-supportive attitudes.
The present study adopted the same logic as
Carpentier et al. (
2014) and
Yang et al. (
2022), and we focused on safety attitudes and their interaction with social norms and the family climate on young pedestrians’ risky and positive behaviours. Attitudes are tendencies to evaluate an entity with some degree of favour or disfavour, ordinarily expressed in cognitive, affective and behavioural responses (
Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Numerous studies have shown that a risk-supportive attitude results in more risky driving behaviour (
Carpentier et al., 2014;
Iversen, 2004) or unsafe crossing behaviour (
Evans & Norman, 1998;
Liu et al., 2021;
Mirzaei-Alavijeh et al., 2019;
Zhou & Horrey, 2010). For example,
Zhou and Horrey (
2010) reported that students aged 12–19 years have a more positive attitude towards crossing behaviour, experience more social approval from significant others and consider this behaviour to be more consistent with moral norms. Although some studies have shown that safety attitudes significantly affect individuals’ willingness and intention to cross the street safely (
Liu et al., 2021;
Mirzaei-Alavijeh et al., 2019), other studies have reported that safety attitudes are poor predictors of safety behaviours in countries that are prominently ‘collectivistic’ (
Nordfjærn & Şimşekoğlu, 2013). This inconsistency suggests that the effect of safety attitudes on pedestrian crossing behaviour still needs more investigation.
With respect to the external variables, we use social norms and family climate as potential moderators of the effect of safety attitudes on pedestrians’ crossing behaviour. The family climate for road safety in this study refers to the perceptions of young teens regarding the values, perceptions, priorities and practices of their parents with respect to safe crossing. Social learning and socialization theories stress familial processes of behaviour transmission and focus more particularly on parents as offspring role models (
Taubman-Ben-Ari & Katz-Ben-Ami, 2013). For example,
Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami (
2013) reported that a lower level of positive parental model and feedback leads to more reckless driving. Other studies have shown that young drivers who imitate their parents’ model for risky driving and parents tend not to invest time in safety education and ignore young drivers’ risky driving behaviour are associated with more self-reported reckless driving behaviours (e.g.,
Carpentier et al., 2014;
Ma et al., 2024). In this context, this study takes the first step to explore parental modelling and positive feedback on their offspring’s risky and safe crossing behaviours.
Social norms can be seen as a person’s beliefs or understandings of a group about how members of this group should behave in certain situations. In this study, social norms refer to peer influence on young pedestrians. Studies have reported consistent results regarding the effects of social norms elicited from peers on young pedestrians’ crossing behaviours (
Morrongiello et al., 2019;
O’Neal et al., 2019;
Pfeffer & Hunter, 2013;
Wang et al., 2024). For instance,
Pfeffer and Hunter (
2013) reported that participants aged 16–18 years least often identified safe road-crossing sites when accompanied by a negative peer and more frequently identified dangerous road-crossing sites when accompanied by a positive peer. However, to our knowledge, the effects of social norms on young pedestrians’ crossing behaviour have largely not been investigated in China. Given that Chinese people tend to cross the street dangerously if enough people are waiting on the roadside, exploring whether the effects of social norms enhance or reduce young teens’ risky crossing behaviour can be highly valuable.
1.3. Objective and Hypotheses
The primary objective of the present study was to examine the effects of safety attitudes on pedestrian behaviour, and the moderating roles of social norms and the family climate in the relationships between safety attitudes and pedestrian behaviour were also examined. As portrayed in the hypothesized model (
Figure 1), the independent variable is safety attitudes, the dependent variable is PBS factors and the moderating variables are social norms and family climate. Three hypotheses are formulated from these variables.
Hypothesis 1. Safety attitudes affect both young pedestrians’ risky and positive behaviours.
Hypothesis 2. Social norms and the family climate directly influence pedestrian behaviour.
Hypothesis 3. Social norms and family climate moderate the effects of safety attitudes on pedestrians’ behaviour.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
A total of 400 participants agreed to participate in this study. Participants were recruited from Dalian, Zhangzhou and Chengdu. After the participants were informed of the purpose of the study, 16 participants were unwilling to participate, and the remaining 384 signed a consent form and finished the paper and pen survey. The survey included items related to pedestrian behaviour, social norms, the family climate and safety attitudes. Participants first completed a self-reported demographic questionnaire in which they provided information such as sex, age, class, walking frequency and weekly walking time. Participants were then required to finish the survey within 30 min. Data were collected from 1 to 20 October 2024.
Finally, 370 valid data were obtained and 14 data were discarded because some answers were missing. Each participant received 5 RMB upon finishing the survey. The sample included 181 males (48.9%) and 189 females (51.1%). Participants ranged in age from 10–18 years (M = 13.99, SD = 2.53). Participants’ walking frequency ranged from 1 (less than once per day) to 4 (5~6 times per day) (M = 2.22, SD = 1.05), and their mean weekly walking time ranged from 1 (less than 150 min) to 3 (more than 300 min) (M = 2.06, SD = 0.81). Most of the participants (86.5%) were unaccompanied by adults, particularly when travelling to and from school as pedestrians or on public transport.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. PBS Scale
The original PBS contains 32 items. Participants were asked to rate the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The PBS has 4 factors: transgression (e.g., I cross between vehicles stopped on the roadway in traffic jams), lapses (e.g., I become angry with another user and insult him), positive behaviour (e.g., I stop to let the pedestrians I meet by) and aggressive behaviour (e.g., I cross without looking, following other people who are crossing). Given that the age of the participants in the original study ranged from 15–78 years, the factorial structure of the 32-item scale was first examined via exploratory factor analysis to assess its reliability in assessing the crossing behaviours of pedestrians aged 10–18 years.
2.2.2. Social Norms
The social norm scale contains 6 items that address both the implicit and the explicit influences that respondents experience from their peers (
Carpentier et al., 2014). The items were modified to better capture social norms among pedestrians. Both implicitly expressed peer norms (e.g.,
my friend often uses his cell phone while crossing the street) and explicitly expressed peer norms (e.g.,
my friends will not mind if I do not look at the traffic lights occasionally) were assessed. Participants were asked to rate the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (agree very much) to 5 (agree not at all). The total score was calculated by averaging the scores of the 6 items, with a higher score indicating a greater presence of risk-supportive peer norms. In this study, the internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) of this scale was 0.74.
2.2.3. Family Climate
The family climate scale is derived from an existing scale, namely the Family Climate for Road Safety Scale (FCRSS) developed by
Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami (
2013). This 6-item scale has been widely used in previous Chinese studies (e.g.,
Ma et al., 2024). Five items (e.g.,
my parents are concerned about whether I cross the street safely) from the FCRSS scale were used in this study, and all the items were modified to better capture the family climate perceived by young pedestrians. Participants were asked to rate the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). The total score was calculated by averaging the 5 items, with a higher score indicating a higher level of parental monitoring and modelling. In this study, the internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) for this scale is 0.85.
2.2.4. Safety Attitudes
The safety attitudes scale was derived from an existing scale developed by
Iversen (
2004). The scale includes 6 items, which come from the rule violation factor of the original scale. The scale has been widely used in previous Chinese studies (e.g.,
Liu et al., 2021). The items are modified to better capture safety attitudes among young pedestrians (e.g.,
to feel the destination in time, it is possible to occasionally break traffic rules). Participants were asked to rate the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The total score was calculated by averaging the scores of the 6 items, with a higher score indicating a higher level of rule violation. In this study, the internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) for this scale was 0.74.
2.3. Data Analysis
The data were analysed via SPSS 26.0. First, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted to test the factorial structure of the PBS in young pedestrians. Second, the correlations between social norms, the family climate, safety attitudes and the PBS factors were calculated. Third, the predictive role of social norms, the family climate and safety attitudes and their interaction with the PBS factors were analysed via hierarchical regression analysis. The interaction terms that were found to be significant in the regression analysis were further analysed via simple slope analysis to determine the nature of the interaction effects via the Hayes PROCESS macro Model 1 suggested by
Hayes et al. (
2012).
2.4. Common Method Bias
Harman’s single-factor method was used to assess common method bias (
Podsakoff et al., 2003). The first factor accounted for 33.25% of the variance, and factors for which the eigenvalues exceeded 1.0 accounted for 58.86% of the variance. Thus, common method bias does not appear to be a serious problem in this study.
3. Results
3.1. Exploratory Factor Analysis
The original 32 items were subjected to an EFA via principal component analysis with oblique rotation. The sampling adequacy measure of Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin was 0.962, and Bartlett’s sphericity test was significant (chi-square = 7540.378, p < 0.001). The results revealed that four factors (eigenvalues > 1) explained 61.11% of the total variance. However, six items (e.g., I realized I could not remember the route I just took) that loaded on more than one factor were deleted.
The remaining 26 items are submitted to a second EFA again. The sampling adequacy measure of Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin was 0.951, and Bartlett’s sphericity test was significant (chi-square = 5619.987,
p < 0.001). The results revealed that four factors (eigenvalues > 1) explained 61.85% of the total variance. Factor 1 (11 items), labelled transgression behaviour, explained 42.41% of the variance. Factor 2 (5 items), labelled positive behaviour, explained 9.82% of the variance. Factor 3 (5 items), labelled aggressive behaviour, explained 5.24% of the variance. Factor 4 (5 items), labelled lapses, explained 4.38% of the variance. The factor loadings are shown in
Table 1. The summary statistics of the PBS factors are shown in
Table 2. The factorial structure was comparable to that obtained in the original PBS (
Granié et al., 2013).
3.2. Correlation Analysis
The correlations between the PBS factors and social norms, family climate and safety attitudes are shown in
Table 3.
Table 3 shows that social norms and safety attitudes are positively correlated with transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour and negatively correlated with positive behaviour. The family climate is negatively correlated with transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour.
3.3. Hierarchical Regression Analyses
Several hierarchical regression analyses were conducted with transgression, lapses, and aggressive and positive behaviour as the dependent variables. In each regression, sociodemographic variables (e.g., sex, age and class) were entered in step 1. Social norms, safety attitudes and the family climate were entered in step 2. The interaction of the three independent variables was entered in step 3, and only the effects of those interactions that reached significance are shown in
Table 4.
Table 3 shows that social norms, safety attitudes and family climate can significantly predict transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour and explain 54.2%, 44.3% and 49.3% of the variance, respectively, whereas only social norms can predict positive behaviour and explain 2.6% of the variance. The interactions between social norms and safety attitudes, between family climate and safety attitudes and the three variables contributed to 4.6% of the variance in transgression behaviours. The interactions between social norms and safety attitudes and between family climate and safety attitudes contributed to 2.8% of the variance in lapses and 2.3% of the variance in aggressive behaviour.
3.4. Simple Slope Analyses
Significant interactions were further examined via the Hayes PROCESS macro-Model 1 suggested by
Hayes et al. (
2012).
Figure 2a shows that social norms moderated the relationship between safety attitudes and transgression behaviour. For high social norms (
b = 0.59,
t = 9.53,
p < 0.01), a higher score on safety attitudes was associated with more transgression behaviour. For low social norms (
b = −0.02,
t = −0.37,
p = 0.71), a higher score for safety attitudes did not increase transgression behaviour.
Figure 2b,c revealed similar results: for high social norms, a higher score on safety attitudes was associated with more lapse behaviour (
b = 0.60,
t = 8.97,
p < 0.01) and aggressive behaviour (
b = 0.58,
t = 9.20,
p < 0.01). For low social norms, a higher score on safety attitudes did not increase lapse behaviour (
b = 0.09,
t = 1.43,
p = 0.15) or aggressive behaviour (
b = 0.02,
t = 0.33,
p = 0.74).
Figure 2d shows that the family climate moderated the relationship between safety attitudes and transgression behaviour. For both a low family climate (
b = 0.51,
t = 13.58,
p < 0.01) and a high family climate (
b = 0.15,
t = 2.97,
p < 0.05), a higher score on safety attitudes was associated with more transgression behaviour.
Figure 2e shows that for both a low family climate (
b = 0.55,
t = 13.73,
p < 0.01) and a high family climate (
b = 0.20,
t = 3.61,
p < 0.01), a higher score on safety attitudes was associated with more aggressive behaviour. Similarly,
Figure 2f shows that for both a low family climate (
b = 0.63,
t = 13.98,
p < 0.01) and a high family climate (
b = 0.23,
t = 3.64,
p < 0.01), a higher score on safety attitudes was associated with more lapse behaviour.
We conducted a simple slope analysis to examine the three-way interaction effects of social norms, safety attitudes and the family climate on transgression behaviours.
Figure 2g shows that when social norms are high, a higher score on safety attitudes leads to more transgression behaviour when both family climate are high (
b = 0.31,
t = 3.83,
p < 0.01) and lower (
b = 0.42,
t = 5.17,
p < 0.01).
Figure 2h shows that when social norms are low, a higher score on safety attitudes leads to more transgression behaviour when the family climate is low (
b = 0.43,
t = 4.88,
p < 0.01) but not when the family climate is high (
b = −0.05,
t = −0.75,
p = 0.45).
4. Discussion
This study took the first step in investigating the relationships between young pedestrians’ risky and positive crossing behaviours, safety attitudes, family climate and social norms. Our results revealed significant associations between the studied variables and the moderating role of family climate and social norms in relation to safety attitudes and crossing behaviours. The findings not only support the theoretical model of interactions between external factors and personal variables on crossing behaviours but also provide a theoretical framework for developing interventions to ensure the crossing safety of younger pedestrians.
Safety attitudes and social norms are important determinants of pedestrian crossing behaviour. This study also confirmed that perceived parental influence (family climate) played an important role in determining risky and positive crossing behaviour of young pedestrians. Our results show that safety attitudes, social norms and family climate can significantly predict transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour, whereas only social norms and family climate can significantly predict positive behaviour, which supports research Hypotheses 1 and 2. The present study revealed that social norms moderate the effect of safety attitudes on three risky crossing behaviours (i.e., transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour). Only when the level of risk-supportive peer norms is high (social norm) is a stronger rule violation attitude associated with more transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour. These results are consistent with the findings of several previous studies showing that a risk-supportive attitude and negative peer influence are associated with more risky crossing behaviours (e.g.,
Liu et al., 2021;
Morrongiello et al., 2019;
O’Neal et al., 2019;
Pfeffer & Hunter, 2013;
Wang et al., 2024;
Zhou & Horrey, 2010).
More importantly, family climate moderates the effect of safety attitudes on the three risky crossing behaviours (i.e., transgression, lapses and aggressive). A higher rule violation attitude leads to more transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour, regardless of whether the family climate is high or low. Consistent findings in driving safety have demonstrated that a lack of parental commitment to road safety leads to an increase in risky behaviours among young drivers (
Carpentier et al., 2014;
Ma et al., 2024;
Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2014). These results indicated that positive communication and consensus between parents and offspring on road safety could help reduce risky crossing behaviours of young pedestrians aged 10–18 years.
The present study contributed to the literature in that we found significant interactions between safety attitudes, social norms and the family climate in terms of transgression behaviour. Our results show that regardless of whether the level of risk-supportive peer norms is low or high (social norms), a higher rule violation attitude results in more transgression behaviours if parents do not give much attention to monitoring their offspring’s crossing behaviour on the road or setting a model for their offspring by obeying traffic rules and crossing the street safely. However, a higher score for family climate did not increase transgression behaviours as much as a lower score for family climate. These findings suggest that a higher score for family climate can serve as a protective factor in reducing transgression behaviour. This finding was further confirmed by our results, which indicated that a stronger rule violation attitude did not increase their transgression behaviours under low risk-supportive peer norms. Even though young pedestrians might confront the negative influence of their friends (
Morrongiello et al., 2019;
O’Neal et al., 2019;
Wang et al., 2024), parents can still minimize these adverse effects by teaching their children to obey traffic rules and set examples in safe crossing behaviour.
5. Implications and Limitations
These research results have certain practical applications. First, the relationships among social norms, safety attitudes, the family climate and various crossing behaviours provide a theoretical approach for better understanding both risky and positive crossing behaviours of young pedestrians aged 10–18 years. Second, our findings highlight that safety education programs should be tailored to address both individual attitudes and peer influences. Given that a low score on social norms does not increase transgression, lapses or aggressive behaviours, it might be of great value in cultivating a shared commitment to safe crossing among young pedestrians. For policy makers and practitioners, safety education and interventions that jointly include young pedestrians and their peers might be more effective. Third, the present study contributes to our understanding of how familial, peer and individual factors interact to influence risky crossing behaviours. Our findings suggest that school and community-based initiatives that engage both young pedestrians and their parents could be more effective in reinforcing safe crossing behaviours. For instance, education and counselling should be provided to young pedestrians and their peers to change their safety attitudes, whereas guidance should be provided to parents to help foster a positive family climate for road safety.
This study has several limitations. The first limitation is that the study is based on a small sample, and the sample might not represent the whole population in China. Future studies with larger samples should be conducted to further examine the generalizability of the results obtained in this study. Although we recruited participants from three cities in China, the differences in the environmental factors (e.g., speed limit, road type, population density and weather conditions) of the three cities could affect pedestrians’ crossing behaviours. Another limitation is that, as in most previous studies (
Carpentier et al., 2014;
Yang et al., 2022), the family climate was used as a moderator. In this study, the items related to family climate were limited to parental monitoring and modelling. Given that the family climate for road safety is a multidimensional structure, future studies are recommended to explore the effects of other factors of the family climate on pedestrians’ crossing behaviour. A third limitation is that the relationships among the studied variables may be inaccurate due to social desirability. In the future, more objective indicators of crossing behaviours should be included.
6. Conclusions
This study examined the relationships among the safety attitudes, family climate, social norms and crossing behaviours of pedestrians aged 10–18 years. Our results showed that social norms, family climate and safety attitudes can significantly predict risky crossing behaviours (transgression, lapses and aggressive behaviour). More importantly, we found that the effects of safety attitudes on risky crossing behaviours were moderated by social norms and the family climate. The findings may provide a theoretical framework to understand young pedestrians’ behaviour and assist in developing safety education and interventions. However, the framework itself cannot directly reduce the number of risk-taking children. Instead, it offers valuable insights for creating more comprehensive safety strategies.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, L.S.; methodology, L.S.; formal analysis, Q.Z. and S.Y.; investigation, Q.Z. and S.Y.; data curation, Q.Z. and S.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, Q.Z., S.Y. and L.S.; writing—review and editing, Q.Z., S.Y. and L.S.; funding acquisition, L.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded by the Social Science Foundation of Liaoning Province, grant number L22CSH006.
Institutional Review Board Statement
This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of Liaoning Normal University (protocol code LL2024156 and date of approval 27 September 2024).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The original data presented in the study are openly available in [FigShare] at [10.6084/m9.figshare.28340681].
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- Adminaite, D., Jost, G., Stipdonk, H., Ward, H., & Calinescu, T. (2018). Reducing child deaths on European roads, ETSC PIN Flash Report 34, February 2018. Available online: https://etsc.eu/reducing-child-deaths-on-european-roads-pin-flash-34/ (accessed on 26 January 2025).
- Antić, B., Pešić, D., Milutinović, N., & Maslać, M. (2016). Pedestrian behaviours: Validation of the Serbian version of the pedestrian behaviour scale. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 41, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biassoni, F., Bina, M., Confalonieri, F., & Ciceri, R. (2018). Visual exploration of pedestrian crossings by adults and children: Comparison of strategies. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 56, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpentier, A., Brijs, K., Declercq, K., Brijs, T., Daniels, S., & Wets, G. (2014). The effect of family climate on risky driving of young novices: The moderating role of attitude and locus of control. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 73, 53–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deb, S., Strawderman, L., DuBien, J., Smith, B., Carruth, D. W., & Garrison, T. M. (2017). Evaluating pedestrian behavior at crosswalks: Validation of a pedestrian behavior questionnaire for the US population. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 106, 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliott, M. A., & Baughan, C. J. (2004). Developing a self-report method for investigating adolescent road user behaviour. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(6), 373–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, D., & Norman, P. (1998). Understanding pedestrians’ road crossing decisions: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Health Education Research, 13(4), 481–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granié, M. A., Pannetier, M., & Gueho, L. (2013). Developing a self-reporting method to measure pedestrian behaviors at all ages. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 830–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, L. W., & Kreuter, M. W. (2005). Health program planning: An educational and ecological approach. McGraw-Hill Humanities. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Iversen, H. (2004). Risk-taking attitudes and risky driving behaviour. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(3), 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, M., Wu, J., Yousaf, A., Wang, L., Hu, K., Plant, K. L., McIlroy, R. C., & Stanton, N. A. (2021). Exploring the relationship between attitudes, risk perceptions, fatalistic beliefs, and pedestrian behaviors in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(7), 3378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, Y., Qiu, Y., & Sun, L. (2024). Chinese adaptation of the Reckless Driving Behaviour Scale: Testing its psychometric properties and links with safe driving climate among family and peers. Heliyon, 10(2024), e34446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirzaei-Alavijeh, M., Babakhani, M., Jalilian, F., Vaezi, M., Jalilian, F., Khashij, S., & Hamzeh, B. (2019). Socio-cognitive determinants of safe road-crossing behaviors: An application of the prototype willingness model. Journal of Injury and Violence Research, 11(1), 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morrongiello, B. A., Corbett, M., Koutsoulianos, S., & Foster, A. (2019). Effects of peers on child pedestrian behaviors in a virtual traffic context. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 44(4), 509–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moyano-Díaz, E. (1997). Evaluation of traffic violation behaviors and the causal attribution of accidents in Chile. Environment and Behavior, 29(2), 264–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nordfjærn, T., & Şimşekoğlu, Ö. (2013). The role of cultural factors and attitudes for pedestrian behaviour in an urban Turkish sample. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 21, 181–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Neal, E. E., Jiang, Y., Brown, K., Kearney, J. K., & Plumert, J. M. (2019). How does crossing roads with friends impact risk taking in young adolescents and adults? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 44(6), 726–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papadimitriou, E., Theofilatos, A., & Yannis, G. (2013). Patterns of pedestrian attitudes, perceptions and behaviour in Europe. Safety Science, 53, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfeffer, K., & Hunter, E. (2013). The effects of peer influence on adolescent pedestrian road-crossing decisions. Traffic Injury Prevention, 14(4), 434–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Podsakoff, P. M., Mackenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salducco, A., Abati, D., Bina, M., Bertani, B., Calarco, S., Balzarotti, S., & Biassoni, F. (2022). Young pedestrians’ behaviours and risk perception: A pilot study with Italian early adolescents. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 90, 512–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soole, D. W., Lennon, A., & Haworth, N. (2011). Parental beliefs about supervising children when crossing roads and cycling. International Journal of Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 18(1), 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taubman-Ben-Ari, O. (2014). The parental factor in adolescent reckless driving: The road ahead. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 69, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., & Katz-Ben-Ami, L. (2013). Family climate for road safety: A new concept and measure. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 54, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandroux, R., Granié, M., Jay, M., Sueur, C., & Pelé, M. (2022). The pedestrian behaviour scale: A systematic review of its validation around the world. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 165, 106509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, H., Su, X., Fan, M., & Schwebel, D. C. (2024). The more peers are present, the more adventurous? How peer presence influences adolescent pedestrian safety. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 102, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, H., Wang, A., Su, F., & Schwebel, D. C. (2022). The effect of age and sensation seeking on pedestrian crossing safety in a virtual reality street. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 88, 99–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xinhua. (2021). Annual statistical report on road traffic accidents in China. Available online: https://www.xinhuanet.com/video/sjxw/2021-12/02/c_1211471017.htm (accessed on 26 January 2025).
- Yang, Z., Sun, L., & Ren, G. (2022). Effects of safe driving climate among friends on prosocial and aggressive driving behaviors of young drivers: The moderating role of traffic locus of control. Journal of Safety Rresearch, 81, 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, R., & Horrey, W. J. (2010). Predicting adolescent pedestrians’ behavioral intentions to follow the masses in risky crossing situations. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13(3), 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
| Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).