Assessing the Welfare of Captive Group-Housed Cockroaches, Gromphadorhina oblongonota
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Subjects
2.2. Experimental Design
2.3. Welfare Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. The Impact of Environmental Factors on Welfare Score
4.2. Other Factors That Impacted Welfare Score
4.3. Limitations
5. Conclusions
- Adapt the AWAG scoring criteria to the species, based on thorough research.
- Improve the representation of individual welfare by combining the percentage of impacted individuals and severity of the impact in the scoring criteria.
- Standardise the time of day the assessment takes place, which should include the species’ most active period.
- Assess welfare regularly.
- Utilise the assessment results to drive changes that will improve animal welfare.
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Cooper, J.E. Invertebrate Care. Vet. Clin. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2004, 7, 473–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boppré, M.; Vane-Wright, R.I. Welfare Dilemmas Created by Keeping Insects in Captivity. In The Welfare of Invertebrate Animals; Animal Welfare, Carere, C., Mather, J., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; Volume 18, pp. 23–67. ISBN 978-3-030-13946-9. [Google Scholar]
- van Huis, A. Welfare of Farmed Insects. J. Insects Food Feed. 2019, 5, 159–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delvendahl, N.; Rumpold, B.A.; Langen, N. Edible Insects as Food–Insect Welfare and Ethical Aspects from a Consumer Perspective. Insects 2022, 13, 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pippinato, L.; Gasco, L.; Di Vita, G.; Mancuso, T. Current Scenario in the European Edible-Insect Industry: A Preliminary Study. J. Insects Food Feed. 2020, 6, 371–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- WWF-UK. The Future of Feed: A WWF Roadmap to Accelerating Insect Protein in UK Feeds. 2022. Available online: https://www.wwf.org.uk/sites/default/files/2022-06/future_of_feed_full_report.pdf (accessed on 12 September 2023).
- The Member States. Treaty of Lisbon Amending the Treaty on European Union and the Treaty Establishing the European Community, Signed at Lisbon, 13 December 2007; Proactis: Aberdeen, UK, 2007; Volume 306. [Google Scholar]
- Birch, J.; Burn, C.; Schnell, A.; Browning, H.; Crump, A. Review of the Evidence of Sentience in Cephalopod Molluscs and Decapod Crustaceans; London School of Economics and Political Science: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Jinman, P. Animal Sentience 2018. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5f0f05db3a6f400394d55225/FAWC_letter_on_animal_sentience_16_March_2018.pdf (accessed on 12 September 2023).
- Lambert, H.; Elwin, A.; D’Cruze, N. Wouldn’t Hurt a Fly? A Review of Insect Cognition and Sentience in Relation to Their Use as Food and Feed. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 243, 105432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherwin, C.M. Can Invertebrates Suffer? Or, How Robust Is Argument-By-Analogy? Anim. Welf. 2001, 10, S103–S118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knutsson, S.; Munthe, C. A Virtue of Precaution Regarding the Moral Status of Animals with Uncertain Sentience. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 2017, 30, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrett, M.; Chia, S.Y.; Fischer, B.; Tomberlin, J. Welfare Considerations for Farming Black Soldier Flies, Hermetia Illucens (Diptera: Stratiomyidae): A Model for the Insects as Food and Feed Industry. J. Insects Food Feed. 2022, 9, 119–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saul-Gershenz, L.S.; Arnold, R.A.; Scriber, J.M. Design of Captive Environments for Endangered Invertebrates. In Conservation of Endangered Species in Captivity: An Interdisciplinary Approach; Gibbons, E.F., Durrant, B.S., Demarest, J., Eds.; State University of New York Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 51–71. ISBN 978-0-7914-1911-3. [Google Scholar]
- Arbuckle, K. Folklore Husbandry and a Philosophical Model for the Design of Captive Management Regimes. Herpetol. Rev. 2013, 44, 448–452. [Google Scholar]
- WAZA. Our Approach to Animal Welfare. 2023. Available online: https://www.waza.org/ (accessed on 12 September 2023).
- Arndt, S.S.; Goerlich, V.C.; van der Staay, F.J. A Dynamic Concept of Animal Welfare: The Role of Appetitive and Adverse Internal and External Factors and the Animal’s Ability to Adapt to Them. Front. Anim. Sci. 2022, 3, 908513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, I.J.H. The Changing Concept of Animal Sentience. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, I.J.H. Is Sentience Only a Nonessential Component of Animal Welfare? Anim. Sentience 2016, 1, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cooper, J.E.; Williams, D.L. The Feeding of Live Food to Exotic Pets: Issues of Welfare and Ethics. J. Exot. Pet Med. 2014, 23, 244–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, M. Feeding Live Invertebrate Prey in Zoos and Aquaria: Are There Welfare Concerns? Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 316–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kagan, R.; Allard, S.; Carter, S. What Is the Future for Zoos and Aquariums? J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2018, 21, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Collins, S.; Burn, C.C.; Wathes, C.M.; Cardwell, J.M.; Chang, Y.-M.; Bell, N.J. Time-Consuming, but Necessary: A Wide Range of Measures Should Be Included in Welfare Assessments for Dairy Herds. Front. Anim. Sci. 2021, 2, 703380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiVincenti, L.; McDowell, A.; Herrelko, E.S. Integrating Individual Animal and Population Welfare in Zoos and Aquariums. Animals 2023, 13, 1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Narshi, T.M.; Free, D.; Justice, W.S.M.; Smith, S.J.; Wolfensohn, S. Welfare Assessment of Invertebrates: Adapting the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG) for Zoo Decapods and Cephalopods. Animals 2022, 12, 1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reuben Digital Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Available online: https://awag.org.uk/ (accessed on 29 September 2023).
- Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J.; Littlewood, K.E.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D.; Jones, B.; Wilkins, C. The 2020 Five Domains Model: Including Human–Animal Interactions in Assessments of Animal Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Honess, P.; Wolfensohn, S. The Extended Welfare Assessment Grid: A Matrix for the Assessment of Welfare and Cumulative Suffering in Experimental Animals. Altern. Lab. Anim. 2010, 38, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Justice, W.S.M.; O’Brien, M.F.; Szyszka, O.; Shotton, J.; Gilmour, J.E.M.; Riordan, P.; Wolfensohn, S. Adaptation of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG) for Monitoring Animal Welfare in Zoological Collections. Vet. Rec. 2017, 181, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfensohn, S.; Shotton, J.; Bowley, H.; Davies, S.; Thompson, S.; Justice, W. Assessment of Welfare in Zoo Animals: Towards Optimum Quality of Life. Animals 2018, 8, 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ryan, M.; Waters, R.; Wolfensohn, S. Assessment of the Welfare of Experimental Cattle and Pigs Using the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Animals 2021, 11, 999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malkani, R.; Paramasivam, S.; Wolfensohn, S. Preliminary Validation of a Novel Tool to Assess Dog Welfare: The Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 940017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Free, D.; Justice, W.S.M.; Smith, S.J.; Howard, V.; Wolfensohn, S. An Approach to Assessing Zoo Animal Welfare in a Rarely Studied Species, the Common Cusimanse Crossarchus obscurus. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2022, 3, 420–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, M.C.; Fraser, J. Sound Production in the Cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa: Evidence for Communication by Hissing. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1980, 6, 305–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schal, C.; Gautier, J.-Y.; Bell, W.J. Behavioural Ecology of Cockroaches. Biol. Rev. 1984, 59, 209–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monahan, C.F.; Bogan, J.E.; LaDouceur, E.E.B. Histological Findings in Captive Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches (Gromphadorhina portentosa) and a Literature Review. Vet. Pathol. 2023, 03009858231166659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bell, W.J.; Roth, L.M.; Nalepa, C.A. Cockroaches: Ecology, Behavior, and Natural History; JHU Press: Baltimore, MA, USA, 2007; ISBN 978-0-8018-8616-4. [Google Scholar]
- Brereton, J. The Behavioural Biology of Invertebrates. In The Behavioural Biology of Zoo Animals; Rose, P., Ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2022; pp. 269–282. ISBN 978-1-00-077657-7. [Google Scholar]
- Kandilian, K. Roach Crossing’s Cockroach Husbandry Guide; Roach Crossing LLC: Royal Oak, MI, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Yoder, J.A.; Barcelona, J.C. Food and Water Resources Used by the Madagascan Hissing-Cockroach Mite, Gromphadorholaelaps Schaeferi. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 1995, 19, 259–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chua, J.; Fisher, N.A.; Falcinelli, S.D.; DeShazer, D.; Friedlander, A.M. The Madagascar Hissing Cockroach as an Alternative Non-Mammalian Animal Model to Investigate Virulence, Pathogenesis, and Drug Efficacy. J. Vis. Exp. 2017, 24, 56491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Retardo-Agua, J.; Crausos, K.; Sasam, J.M. Growth Rate and Thigmotactic Behavior of Turkestan Cockroach (Blatta lateralis) under Different Illumination Conditions. IJRIAS 2023, 8, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michel, M.; Lyons, L.C. Unraveling the Complexities of Circadian and Sleep Interactions with Memory Formation through Invertebrate Research. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stephenson, R.; Chu, K.M.; Lee, J. Prolonged Deprivation of Sleep-like Rest Raises Metabolic Rate in the Pacific Beetle Cockroach, Diploptera punctata (Eschscholtz). J. Exp. Biol. 2007, 210, 2540–2547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mishra, M.; Meyer-Rochow, V.B. Fine structural description of the compound eye of the Madagascar ‘hissing cockroach’Gromphadorhina portentosa (Dictyoptera: Blaberidae). Insect Sci. 2008, 15, 179–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Warrant, E.J. The remarkable visual capacities of nocturnal insects: Vision at the limits with small eyes and tiny brains. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2017, 372, 20160063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clark, D.C.; Moore, A.J. Social Interactions and Aggression among Male Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches (Gromphadorhina portentosa) in Groups (Dictyoptera: Blaberidae). J. Insect Behav. 1994, 7, 199–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, D.C.; Moore, A.J. Variation and Repeatability of Male Agonistic Hiss Characteristics and Their Relationship to Social Rank in Gromphadorhina portentosa. Anim. Behav. 1995, 50, 719–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, D.; Moore, A. Social Communication in the Madagascar Hissing Cockroach: Features of Male Courtship Hisses and a Comparison of Courtship and Agonistic Hisses. Behaviour 1995, 132, 401–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mack, C. Winner and Loser Effects in Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches (Gromphadorhina portentosa); Bucknell University: Lewisburg, PA, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Brouwers, S.; Duchateau, M.J. Feasibility and Validity of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid to Monitor the Welfare of Zoo-Housed Gorillas Gorilla Gorilla Gorilla. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2021, 9, 208–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Analog Devices Inc. Thermochron iButton; Analog Devices Inc.: Wilmington, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Mulder, P.; Shufran, A. Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches: Information and Care; Oklahoma State University: Stillwater, OK, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Bradt, D.L.; Hoback, W.W.; Kard, B.M. American Cockroach Response to Cold Temperatures. Southwest. Entomol. 2018, 43, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halsey, L.G.; Matthews, P.G.D.; Rezende, E.L.; Chauvaud, L.; Robson, A.A. The interactions between temperature and activity levels in driving metabolic rate: Theory, with empirical validation from contrasting ectotherms. Oecologia 2015, 177, 1117–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arrant, A.E.; Schramm-Sapyta, N.L.; Kuhn, C.M. Use of the Light/Dark Test for Anxiety in Adult and Adolescent Male Rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2013, 256, 119–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhukovskaya, M.; Novikova, E.; Saari, P.; Frolov, R.V. Behavioral Responses to Visual Overstimulation in the Cockroach Periplaneta americana L. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2017, 203, 1007–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zewde, A.M.; Yu, F.; Nayak, S.; Tallarida, C.; Reitz, A.B.; Kirby, L.G.; Rawls, S.M. PLDT (Planarian Light/Dark Test): An Invertebrate Assay to Quantify Defensive Responding and Study Anxiety-like Effects. J. Neurosci. Methods 2018, 293, 284–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laurent Salazar, M.-O.; Planas-Sitjà, I.; Sempo, G.; Deneubourg, J.-L. Individual Thigmotactic Preference Affects the Fleeing Behavior of the American Cockroach (Blattodea: Blattidae). J. Insect Sci. 2018, 18, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.-R.; Li, D.-W.; Wang, H.-P.; Lin, S.-S.; Yang, E.-C. The Impact of Thigmotaxis Deprivation on the Development of the German Cockroach (Blattella germanica). iScience 2022, 25, 104802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoder, K. Food Resource Based Agonistic Interactions in Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches (Gromphadorhina portentosa). 2014. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/269410065_Food_Resource_Based_Agonistic_Interactions_in_Madagascar_Hissing_Cockroaches_Gromphadorhina_portentosa#fullTextFileContent (accessed on 29 August 2023).
- Varadínová, Z.; Stejskal, V.; Frynta, D. Patterns of Aggregation Behaviour in Six Species of Cockroach: Comparing Two Experimental Approaches: Aggregation Behaviour of Cockroaches. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2010, 136, 184–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherwen, S.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. The Visitor Effect on Zoo Animals: Implications and Opportunities for Zoo Animal Welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Casteren, A.; Codd, J.R. Foot Morphology and Substrate Adhesion in the Madagascan Hissing Cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa. J. Insect Sci. 2010, 10, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, D.; Shanklin, D. Madagascar Hissing Cockroaches (Gromphadorhina portentosa); University of Kentucky: Lexington, KY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Broom, D.M. Indicators of Poor Welfare. Br. Vet. J. 1986, 142, 524–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hill, S.P.; Broom, D.M. Measuring Zoo Animal Welfare: Theory and Practice. Zoo Biol. 2009, 28, 531–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Manteca, X.; Amat, M.; Salas, M.; Temple, D. Animal-Based Indicators to Assess Welfare in Zoo Animals. CABI Rev. 2016, 2016, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veasey, J.S.; Waran, N.K.; Young, R.J. On Comparing the Behaviour of Zoo Housed Animals with Wild Conspecifics as a Welfare Indicator. Anim. Welf. 1996, 5, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howell, C.P.; Cheyne, S.M. Complexities of Using Wild versus Captive Activity Budget Comparisons for Assessing Captive Primate Welfare. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2019, 22, 78–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Score | General Condition | Presence of Injury | Activity Level |
---|---|---|---|
Reduction in general condition would include: dull carapace, difficulty moulting, abnormal body morphology (abdomen not shrivelled/swollen), high density of mites. | Presence of injury including: damaged or missing limbs, tarsi or antennae, lameness/abnormal locomotion, prolapse. | Activity level, e.g., lethargy or hyperactivity. Consider circadian rhythm, which is normally high at night. | |
1 | All of group demonstrated ideal physical condition. | No observable signs of injury. | All of the group demonstrated normal activity levels. |
2 | 1–10% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 1–10% had observable signs of injury. | 1–10% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
3 | 11–20% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 11–20% had observable signs of injury. | 11–20% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
4 | 21–30% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 21–30% had observable signs of injury. | 21–30% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
5 | 31–40% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 31–40% had observable signs of injury. | 31–40% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
6 | 41–50% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 41–50% had observable signs of injury. | 41–50% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
7 | 51–60% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 51–60% had observable signs of injury. | 51–60% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
8 | 61–70% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 61–70% had observable signs of injury. | 61–70% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
9 | 71–80% had worse than optimum physical condition. | 71–80% had observable signs of injury. | 71–80% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
10 | >80% had worse than optimum physical condition. | >80% had observable signs of injury. | >80% did not exhibit normal activity levels. |
Score | Abnormal Behaviour | Response to Guest Presence | Social Interaction |
---|---|---|---|
Abnormal behaviour such as moving fast and excessive hissing. Remember to consider normal circadian rhythm when making the assessment. | Reaction to people approaching glass or sudden elevations in noise level. Reactions include: increased startling and hissing. | Evidence of aggression (threat displays or combative) or defensive/submissive behaviours or, e.g., abdominal flick, push, butt or lunge, abdominal extension, abdominal thrash, agonistic hiss and stilt stance [47]. | |
1 | No abnormal behaviours observed in any of the group. | None of the group reacted to guest presence. | No negative social interactions witnessed, either aggressive or defensive. |
2 | 1–10% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 1–10% reacted to guest presence. | 1 mild incident witnessed. |
3 | 11–20% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 11–20% reacted to guest presence. | Multiple mild incidences between 2 individuals. |
4 | 21–30% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 21–30% reacted to guest presence. | 1 moderate incidence witnessed. |
5 | 31–40% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 31–40% reacted to guest presence. | Multiple mild incidences between >2 individuals. |
6 | 41–50% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 41–50% reacted to guest presence. | Multiple moderate incidences between 2 individuals |
7 | 51–60% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 51–60% reacted to guest presence. | 1 severe incidence witnessed |
8 | 61–70% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 61–70% reacted to guest presence. | Multiple severe incidences between 2 individuals |
9 | 71–80% exhibited abnormal behaviours. | 71–80% reacted to guest presence. | >80% of individuals involved in at least 1 mild incident. |
10 | >80% or more of the group exhibited abnormal behaviours. | >80% reacted to guest presence. | >50% of individuals involved in at least 1 moderate-severe incident. |
Score | Environment | Group Size and Structure | Enclosure Complexity | Nutrition | Contingent Events |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Suitability of the environment for the species, e.g., location in the zoo, guest viewing, temperature (22–28 °C plus gradient), humidity (60–80%), space, lighting and light cycle, ventilation (no drafts), clean (no mould), drainage, low noise levels. | Considering the number of individuals, group structure, and density in the enclosure. | The enclosure simulates the natural habitat, including branches, bark, leaf litter, rock crevices, among other dark, damp locations, with various heights. Suitable substrate is offered, such as organic soil and leaf litter and natural forage options. | Refers to diet and forage, and presentation. | Contingent events include: enclosure changes, building works, guest events/educational aids, bin collection, deliveries. | |
1 | The enclosure is ideal for the species. | Group size in accordance with natural group size; group structure is appropriate; suitable density for the enclosure size. | Ability to demonstrate all natural behaviours. | Nutrition is optimally suited to the species and individual. (nutritional, physiological and behavioural). | None. |
2 | One factor is below average. | Group structure is marginally different from appropriate group structure. | The ability to demonstrate natural behaviours, but the available options for this are not ideal. | Nutrition available has a marginally decreased appropriateness to accommodate species-specific needs. | Event outside of the enclosure (e.g., continuing construction work) taking place with little disruption |
3 | Two/three factors are below average. | An increased or decreased number of animals present in comparison to the natural group size range, no overstocking. | The ability to demonstrate natural behaviours, but there are few possibilities for this. | Nutrition available has a moderately decreased appropriateness for species-specific needs. | Event outside of the enclosure (e.g., continuing construction work) with slight disruption, e.g., noise or vibrations |
4 | Four/five factors are below average. | Group structure is moderately different from appropriate group structure. | The ability to demonstrate one form of natural behaviour is constrained due to the enclosure design. | Nutrition available is largely inappropriate to accommodate species-specific needs. | An external event that causes some disruption OR a change in the enclosure’s contents without any other events occurring. |
5 | Six factors below average. | A somewhat greater animal density than suitable for enclosure size (many young present without a decrease in the adult population) | A certain type of natural behaviour is unable to be demonstrated because the option is not offered. | Nutrition available is inadequate to fulfil behavioural needs of the species. | External incident that causes a visible interruption OR movement into a familiar environment without any other events occurring. |
6 | Seven factors below average. | An increased or decreased number of animals present in comparison to the natural group size range, with marginal overstocking. | The options to demonstrate natural behaviours are limited, preventing the demonstration of particular natural behaviours associated with enclosure design. | Nutrition available is inadequate to fulfil physiological needs of the species. | External incident that causes a visible interruption AND movement into a familiar environment. |
7 | Eight factors below average. | Group structure is greatly different from appropriate group structure. | The options to demonstrate natural behaviours are limited, preventing the demonstration of multiple natural behaviours associated with enclosure design. | Nutrition available is inadequate to fulfil the behavioural and physiological needs of the species. | Movement into unfamiliar enclosure OR addition of unfamiliar animal. |
8 | Nine factors below average. | Animal density is moderately greater than suitable for enclosure size. | The options to demonstrate natural behaviours are limited, preventing the demonstration of the majority of natural behaviours associated with enclosure design. | Available nutrition is inadequate to fulfil behavioural, physiological and nutritional needs of the species. | Movement into unfamiliar enclosure AND addition of unfamiliar animal. |
9 | Ten factors below average. | Animal density is much greater than enclosure can accommodate. | The options to demonstrate natural behaviours are very limited, preventing the demonstration of virtually all of the natural behaviours associated with enclosure design. | No nutrition is available. | External incident that causes a definite interruption AND movement into unfamiliar enclosure |
10 | All factors scored are inadequate—the enclosure is inappropriate for the species being monitored. | Group size greatly differed from natural group size or a significant level of overstocking. | The animal is unable to demonstrate natural behaviours associated with enclosure design because the options are not offered. | Nutrition is dangerous for the species. | Mixture of events: extended external incident, movement into an unfamiliar enclosure, introduction of unfamiliar animals. Extreme detrimental levels of disruption. |
Score | Effect of Intervention |
---|---|
For example, removing old food or dead individuals, changes to environmental complexity, etc. Stress-related behaviours include: hissing, burrowing or fast movement away from the disturbance | |
1 | No intervention. |
2 | 1–10% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
3 | 11–20% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
4 | 21–30% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
5 | 31–40 reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
6 | 41–50% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
7 | 51–60% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
8 | 61–70% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
9 | 71–80% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
10 | >80% reacted to the intervention by exhibiting stress-related behaviours. |
Parameter/Factor | Score | Parameter/Factor | Score |
---|---|---|---|
Physical | Environmental | ||
General condition | 2 | Environment | 5 |
Presence of injury | 3 | Group size and structure | 6 |
Activity level | 3 | Enclosure complexity | 6 |
Psychological | Nutrition | 2 | |
Response to guests | 1 | Contingent events | 1 |
Social interaction | 5 | Procedural | |
Effect of intervention | 1 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Free, D.; Wolfensohn, S. Assessing the Welfare of Captive Group-Housed Cockroaches, Gromphadorhina oblongonota. Animals 2023, 13, 3351. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213351
Free D, Wolfensohn S. Assessing the Welfare of Captive Group-Housed Cockroaches, Gromphadorhina oblongonota. Animals. 2023; 13(21):3351. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213351
Chicago/Turabian StyleFree, Danielle, and Sarah Wolfensohn. 2023. "Assessing the Welfare of Captive Group-Housed Cockroaches, Gromphadorhina oblongonota" Animals 13, no. 21: 3351. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213351