Next Article in Journal
Efficacy of Nutritional Strategies on the Improvement of the Performance and Health of the Athlete: A Systematic Review
Next Article in Special Issue
Living Well as a Muslim through the Pandemic Era—A Qualitative Study in Japan
Previous Article in Journal
Body Fat Percentage and Normal-Weight Obesity in the Chinese Population: Development of a Simple Evaluation Indicator Using Anthropometric Measurements
Previous Article in Special Issue
Increased Work from Home and Low Back Pain among Japanese Desk Workers during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Did Children in Single-Parent Households Have a Higher Probability of Emotional Instability during the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Nationwide Cross-Sectional Study in Japan

1
Department of Global Public Health, Karolinska Institutet, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden
2
School of Nutrition and Dietetics, Faculty of Health and Social Services, Kanagawa University of Human Services, Yokosuka 238-8522, Japan
3
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden
4
Department of Psychiatry, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, Sendai 980-8574, Japan
5
School of Health Innovation, Kanagawa University of Human Services, Kawasaki 210-0821, Japan
6
Cancer Control Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka 541-8567, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(7), 4239; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074239
Submission received: 20 February 2022 / Revised: 24 March 2022 / Accepted: 30 March 2022 / Published: 1 April 2022

Abstract

:
The influence of public health measures against COVID-19 in Japan on child mental health by household type is unknown. This study aimed to investigate whether COVID-19 and the declaration of a state of emergency in Japan affected children’s mental health between single-parent and two-parent households disproportionately. A large cross-sectional online survey was conducted from August to September 2020. The study included 3365 parents with children aged 0–14 years old who reported their children’s mental status during the declared state of emergency. Emotional instability was reported dichotomously by parents. As the primary result, the probability of emotional instability was higher in single-parent households compared with that in two-parent households after adjustments for potential covariates; the adjusted prevalence ratio (95% CI) was 1.26 (1.07–1.49). Our findings suggest a disproportionate impact on children’s mental health due to the pandemic.

1. Introduction

Since December 2019, coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has brought about disastrous impacts globally [1]. Several public health measures to deter the pandemic, including lockdowns, mass screening and contact tracing, have been adopted since early 2020 [2]. The lockdown and social distancing measures substantially affected the lives of both adults and children. According to UNESCO, more than 80% of students worldwide experienced school closures in April 2020 [3].
As in other countries and regions, a rapid increase in the number of COVID-19 cases urged the Japanese government to request that all elementary schools, junior high schools, high schools and special needs schools close from 2 March 2020, until the end of March, 2020 [4,5]. On the basis of the request, 99% of the schools in Japan closed until the beginning of April 2020 [6]. However, the government declared a state of emergency in seven populated prefectures, such as Tokyo, on 7 April 2020. The government, in turn, requested schools in the prefectures affected to close again [7]. Subsequently, the declaration of a state of emergency was announced on 16 April 2020. Therefore, all elementary schools, junior high schools, high schools and special needs schools were again encouraged to close [8]. The declaration was lifted on 25 May 2020. Virtually, many students experienced almost a 3-month school closure.
During the school closure, many schools continued to offer learning opportunities for children. However, online classes were not common in the Japanese compulsory educational system before the COVID-19 pandemic. Hence, the online schooling was rather Emergency Remote Teaching (ERT) than planned online education. ERT is defined as “a temporary shift of instructional delivery to an alternate delivery mode due to crisis circumstances” [9]. In Japan, a local educational committee settled in each municipality is responsible for education provision at municipality-owned schools, which account for most of the public education in Japan, and educational corporations are responsible at private schools. A nationwide survey conducted by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology on 16 April 2020, revealed that 60 of 1213 local educational committees (5%) provided real-time interactive education via online platforms, 118 (10%) developed original lecture videos, 353 (29%) took advantage of other digital textbooks and materials and 288 (24%) utilized television programs [10].
Children in single-parent households in Japan faced challenges not only economically but also physically or mentally even before the COVID-19 pandemic. Single mothers in Japan more frequently reported severe living conditions and psychological distress than two-parent mothers [11]. Japanese adolescents in single-parent households are more likely to experience psychological distress than two-parent households [12].
Furthermore, the pandemic possibly affected single-parent and two-parent households disproportionately [13]. A previous study in Hong Kong suggests that single-parent families are more prone to being adversely affected by COVID-19 and related social restrictions [14]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no study examined the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on children’s mental health in Japan by household type.
This study aimed to investigate whether COVID-19 and the first declaration of a state of emergency in Japan affected children’s mental health by comparing single-parent and two-parent households. Second, we explored environmental and behavioral factors associated with children’s emotional instability.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

This cross-sectional study used data on children aged 0–14 years old (information provided by their parents) from the Japan “COVID-19 and Society” Internet Survey (JACSIS) study. The JACSIS study is based on a self-administered web questionnaire survey distributed through a commercial research agency ((Rakuten Insight, Inc., Tokyo, Japan), which had approximately 2.2 million qualified panelists in 2019). Invitation to the survey was sent via e-mail to the panelists. The invitation and the questionnaire were only in Japanese.
The questionnaires were distributed to 224,389 panelists selected by sex, age and residential area (47 prefectures in Japan) based on the random sampling method. Japanese people who lived in other countries were not allowed to participate. The panelists who agreed to answer the survey accessed the designated website and responded to the questions. Options were available to skip some questions or discontinue participation during the survey. Recruitment was started on 25 August 2020 and was completed on 30 September 2020.
When the respondents had multiple children, we asked them to answer the status of only one child among them. The priority among target children was allocated as follows (highest to lowest in order): a child at a higher grade at primary school (10–12 years old); a child at a lower grade at primary school (6–9 years old); a child at junior high school (13–14 years old); and a child before primary school (0–5 years old). With the structural constraint of the web survey, we were restricted to inquiring about only one of the participants’ children. As the JACSIS survey collectively included many questions related to the parent–child relationship, we prioritized the age groups that could be more profoundly influenced by the parent–child relationship. Our assumption was that children younger than teenagers, namely, younger than 12 years old (i.e., younger than the age of junior high school students) would be more substantially affected than teenagers because teenagers are more likely to be independent from their parents. On the other hand, it would be presumably challenging to assess emotional and behavioral changes among infants and pre-school children; therefore, we assigned the lowest priority to infants and pre-school children. In addition to these assumptions, we supposed children of 10–12 years old were likely to independently participate in more extra-curricular activities and leisure activities than 6–9-year-old children. Accordingly, 10–12-year-olds were the ideal age group to be analyzed, and we prioritized this group first.

2.2. Study Population

Among the recruited respondents, we excluded individuals who reported unnatural or inconsistent responses using an algorithm shared in JACSIS studies [15]. We excluded respondents who reported that they had no child, or all their children were over 14 years old. Then, we excluded outliers who were younger than the age at which people are legally allowed to marry in Japan, that is, 18 years old for men and 16 years old for women. Furthermore, respondents with two or more children of the same age were excluded because we could not identify which child the participants answered for in the questions of interest. Moreover, we excluded respondents who reported that they had more than five children because the maximum number of children that the respondents could in the survey was five due to the survey structure as they might have failed to provide the appropriate outcome information about children in the prioritized age groups. Lastly, we excluded respondents who did not provide valid information about the primary outcome variable (i.e., they responded “do not know” or “not relevant” to the statement “my child became emotionally instable”).

2.3. Outcome (Emotional Instability)

As the primary outcome variable, we asked the parents about their children’s emotional instability during the two months from April to May 2020 (i.e., the period of the first state of emergency in Japan, when schools, nurseries and kindergartens were requested to close). The statement “my child became emotionally instable” was responded to with any one of “yes”, “no”, “do not know”, or “not relevant”. “Yes” was defined as the presence of emotional instability, while “no” was defined as no emotional instability.
As secondary outcome measures, we measured child misbehaviors that can be deemed consequences of children’s emotional instability. Child misbehaviors included violence, abusive language towards others, demotivation to study and school absenteeism during the same two months. We asked the parents whether they agreed with the following statements: “my child perpetrated violence”, “my child spoke abusive words”, “my child was demotivated to study” and “my child stopped going to school”; the parents responded any one of “yes”, “no”, “do not know”, or “not relevant”. “Yes” was defined as the perpetration of the behaviors, while “no” was defined as no perpetration.

2.4. Exposure (Single-Parent)

The primary exposure variable was the family type defined by self-reported number of spouses (0 or 1). Respondents who answered no spouse were defined as “single-parent households”, and the other respondents were defined as “two-parent households”.
Children’s environment and extracurricular activities included school and nursery closures, online education in April and May 2020, childcare by others (i.e., relatives and childcare services) due to school and nursery closure, and extracurricular activity participation (i.e., cram schools and sport/hobby lessons), which were reported by either “yes” or “no”.
The daily activities of their children were reported in the following categories: sleeping, studying, physical activity, reading, watching TV and online entertainment and playing games. We asked the parents to select one of the following alternatives that specify the hours spent per day carrying out each of the abovementioned activities: 0, less than 30 min, about 30 min, 1 h, 2 h, 3 h, 4–5 h, 6–7 h, 8–9 h, 10–11 h, ≥12 h and do not know. We then described the proportion of the children in the following groups: 8 h or longer for sleep, 1 h or longer for study, 0.5 h or longer for physical activity, 0.5 h or longer for reading, 2 h or longer for watching TV and online entertainment and 1 h or longer for playing games.

2.5. Covariates

We included the following covariates: parents’ age, socioeconomic status and whether they lived with a grandparent. As a socioeconomic status, we included: educational attainment (graduated from college or higher institutions and high school or lower institutions); household income level (categorized using the tertiles of household equivalent income (“low” = less than 2.5 million JPY; “medium” = 2.5 to 4.3 million JPY; and “high” = more than 4.3 million JPY), and “not responded”); the number of family members (≤3, 4 and ≥5); and employment status (employer, self-employed, regular employee, non-regular employee, and unemployed). The household equivalized income was calculated as the self-reported gross (pre-tax) income in 2019, divided by the square root of the number of household members. Whether they lived with a grandparent was dichotomously categorized (yes/no) based on the self-reported number of grandparents living with them.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

We performed a descriptive analysis of covariates according to family type. We used Chi-square tests to test the differences in categorical variables and the Mann–Whitney test for parents’ age because the distribution of parents’ age was not assumed as normal distribution.
We calculated the prevalence ratio and 95% confidence interval (95% CI) of emotional instability using Poisson regression with robust variance to examine the association between the family type and children’s emotional instability [16]. Adjustment items for multivariable logistic regression models were respondents’ age groups (15–19, 20–29, … and 70–79), educational attainment, household income, employment status and whether they lived with a grandparent. For two-parent families, as one of the two parents reported all the information, we adjusted for the covariates based on the information from the responding parent only. For sensitivity analyses, we conducted stratified analyses of the following items: children’s age, children’s environment (i.e., school/nursery closure, online education and childcare by others), extracurricular activities (i.e., cram schools and sports/hobby lessons) and daily activities (i.e., sleep ≥8 h, study ≥1 h, physical activity ≥30 min, reading ≥30 min, watching TV and online entertainment ≥2 h and playing games ≥1 h). As for the stratified analyses, we excluded the respondents who did not provide valid information necessary for stratification as we could not classify them into either of the two groups. In addition, to test the interaction effects, we performed adjusted generalized linear model analyses with an interaction term of stratification variables (i.e., the child age category x child environment or child extracurricular activities).
We performed the analyses using STATA version 16.1. We considered two-sided p-values < 0.05 as statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics

During the study period, 29,000 respondents (response rate 12.9%) answered the questionnaires. We excluded 2601 individuals who reported unnatural or inconsistent responses using an algorithm we developed, which left 26,399 respondents (91.0% of the total survey respondents). Among the 26,399 respondents, we excluded 21,124 respondents who reported that they had no child (n = 16,899), or that all their children were over 14 years old (n = 4225). Then, we excluded 19 outliers who were younger than the age at which people are allowed to be legally married, that is, 18 years old for men (n = 15) and 16 years old for women (n = 4). Furthermore, 96 respondents with two or more children of the same age were excluded because we could not assume which child the participants answered for in the questions of interest. Moreover, we excluded seven respondents who reported that they had more than five children because the maximum number of children that the respondents could answer for was five due to the survey structure as they might have failed to provide the appropriate outcome variable; we thus included 5153 respondents at this step. Lastly, we excluded 1788 respondents due to invalid primary-outcome information. As a result, we analyzed 3365 participants in the present study (Figure 1).
Among 3365 participants, 762 (22.6%) were single parents (Table 1). The proportion of female respondents was 49.2% in two-parent households and 90.0% in single-parent households. The mean parent age was younger among single-parent households. Educational attainment and household income level tended to be higher among two-parent households. The proportion of non-regular employees was higher among single-parent households. The proportion of the youngest child age group (0–5 years old) was higher among two-parent households.
During the state of emergency declaration, the proportion of childcare by others was higher among single-parent households (Table 1). While more single parents reported school and nursery closure than married parents, educational opportunities and online and cram schools were not significantly different between single-parent and two-parent children.
Overall, children in single-parent households spent a shorter time carrying out physical activities and reading, and spent a longer time studying at home, watching television and online entertainment and playing games than those in two-parent families (Table 1). On the other hand, sleeping hours were not significantly different. The distribution of the daily activities by child age group is shown in Table A1 (Appendix A).
Within two-parent households, the covariates, namely, the respondents’ age, household income level and educational attainment were significantly different according to respondents’ gender, except for the status of living with a grandparent (Table A2).

3.2. Children’s Emotional Instability

As the primary result in the present study, the probability of emotional instability was higher in single-parent households compared with that in two-parent households; the adjusted prevalence ratio (95% CI) was 1.26 (1.07–1.49) (Table 2).
As the results of analyses using secondary outcomes, we observed similar trends, with the main result being that children in single-parent households were more likely to use abusive language towards others, become demotivated to study and be absent from school but not perpetrate violence towards others (Table 2).
When we stratified by children’s age, among the 13–14-year-old group, the prevalence ratio of children’s emotional instability in single-parent households was statistically significantly higher than that of two-parent households (Table 3). However, we did not observe a significant interaction between children’s age and family type (p-interaction = 0.49, 0.74 and 0.54 in 6–9-year-old, 10–12-years-old and 13–14-year-old groups, respectively). The stratification for children’s age of the secondary outcome analyses also showed the trend of increasing prevalence ratios with the increase in the children’s age, except regarding violence towards others (Table A3). We found the interaction between family type and the children’s age of 13–14 years old to be insignificant.

3.3. Stratifying by Environment and Daily Activities

Table 4 shows the results of stratified analyses according to environment based on the association between children’s emotional instability and the p-value of the interaction terms between the family type and environment. Among those who experienced school or nursery closure, single-parent children had a higher prevalence ratio of emotional instability than two-parent children; the adjusted prevalence ratio was 1.24 (1.04–1.48). On the contrary, children who attended school or nursery did not show a significant prevalence ratio of emotional instability between two-parent and single-parent households.
Furthermore, Table 5 shows the results of stratified analyses according to children’s daily activities based on the association between children’s emotional instability and p-values of the interaction terms between the family type and daily activities. Among children who had shorter sleeping hours, a shorter reading time, a longer screen time and who spent a longer time playing games, children living in single-parent households had higher prevalence ratios of emotional instability than those in two-parent households.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the impact of family type (single-parent and two-parent households) on children’s emotional instability during the COVID-19 outbreak in Japan. The probability of emotional instability was higher in single-parent households. Additionally, changes in children’s environments due to the pandemic, such as school or nursery closure, affected children’s emotional stability disproportionately according to family type. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first cross-sectional study to investigate the impact of single-parent households on children’s mental status during the COVID-19 pandemic in Japan.
The association between family type and emotional instability was consistently evident in terms of both parent-reported emotional status and perceived misbehavior. Misbehaviors, namely, violence towards others, abusive language towards others, demotivation to study and absenteeism, reflect children’s mental health problems [17,18,19,20]. The trend was pronounced among children aged 13–14 years old (i.e., junior high school age). If this emotional instability leads to or is a symptom of depression, this trend will be a severe problem because depression is associated with devastating consequences, such as poor academic performance [21] and suicide [22,23] in this life stage. Although the present study did not investigate the severity of children’s mental status, the trajectory in children’s psychosocial conditions, especially in single-parent households, would be exacerbated if the social situation for children remained devastated by the COVID-19 pandemic.
The results of the stratified analyses suggested several considerable effect modifiers regarding the association with children’s emotional instability. For example, the adjusted prevalence ratios of emotional instability among the children who could not go to school or nursery were higher than those among the children who did. On the contrary, we observed a higher adjusted prevalence ratio of emotional instability among single-parent children both with and without online education. This result suggests that the online educational opportunities did not mitigate the risk of a deteriorating emotional status among single-parent children, even though children could communicate with their teachers and keep learning. Moreover, over 30% of the parents reported that their children had opportunities to access online education. However, children who accessed online education showed a higher prevalence of mental instability among both family types. There may be la ack of transferring school functions. Therefore, online classes might have exacerbated children’s anxiety. Of note, the impact of online education, including ERT, reported to date is heterogeneous. Even with remote learning opportunities, school closure contributes to increased anxiety and loneliness among adolescents and emotional instability among children [17]. On the other hand, there was no association between ERT and children’s risk for anxiety, depression and obsessive compulsive disorder during the six months from April 2020 to October 2020 in Florida, USA [24]. These findings support that the opening of schools and nurseries could lessen the negative effect of family type on children’s emotional instability. Schools can serve as mental health care providers for children and adolescents [18]. Without schools or nurseries, children may have lacked holistic support that would have been otherwise provided. According to a national survey, only 20% of the municipalities opened consultation counters designated for children, and 59% secured places in schools for children [10]. The lack of contact with adults other than their family or physical locations apart from their homes could have influenced children’s emotional states. Even without classes or formal education, maintaining occasional connection between teachers and children via telephone or online tools could have a beneficial effect on children’s emotional states, especially among those with less parental support or financial hardship, since they are likely to be at a high risk of mental problems [20]. Additionally, there are some public resources, such as toll-free hotlines, designated specifically for children in Japan [25,26]. These measures should have been more advertised through the educational system. Given the burden on schools, more rigorous, nationwide support for offering comprehensive care is required to safeguard children in terms of mental health in case of school closure.
Some daily activities, namely, shorter sleeping hours, a longer time watching TV and online entertainment and a longer time playing games were also considerable effect modifiers regarding the association with children’s emotional instability. Apart from household types, sleep and playing games are suggested to be associated with psychological problems among children during the COVID-19 pandemic [14,27]. Our findings suggested that children who spent more than 2 h watching TV and online entertainment showed a higher prevalence of emotional instability in both two-parent and single-parent households than children who spent less than 2 h on these activities. Additionally, single-parent households were associated with a higher prevalence ratio of emotional instability only among children who spent a longer time watching TV and online entertainment. This finding was aligned with prior reports that a longer screen time was associated with psychosocial problems [28,29].
There are some limitations of the present study. First, this was a cross-sectional study; hence, we cannot infer any causal links between household types and children’s mental status. Data on the target children’s mental health before or after the pandemic were not available. This made it challenging to assess whether children’s mental instability was associated solely with the pandemic and its related countermeasures. However, we asked parents whether their children “became” mentally instable during the declaration of a state of emergency, instead of “were,” to reduce the possibility of capturing an already worsened mental status since before the declaration. The fact that no prior research has investigated mental instability by family type in Japan might highlight the significance of this study. Second, the misclassification of children’s emotional status is a concern because this status was reported by their parents, not by children themselves. If parental reporting caused non-differential misclassification, our main findings were likely underestimated. Third, the adjustment of multivariable regression analysis was performed according to the covariates reported by only one of the two parents among two-parent households. As shown in Table A2, it is likely that fathers and mothers belonged to different age categories and educational backgrounds. Fourth, there was a limitation in the generalizability of our findings due to the study design. As this survey was commercial- and web-based, participants were likely familiar with internet use and online surveys. Participation was voluntary and compensated with a small benefit (provided via points used as cash in Rakuten service), so that they had spare time for voluntary activities. Prior studies from JACSIS adopted a weighing method to adjust responses to improve generalizability [15]; however, a weighing score was not available in the 1000 single-parent respondents in the present study. Fifth, a limited number of single fathers were recruited in this study. Only 76 out of 762 (10.0%) single parents were males among the eligible sample (n = 3365). Nevertheless, the proportion was not far from the national proportion of fathers among single parents in Japan, 11.2% according to the census in 2017 [30]. Hence, we believe that the gender balance in the present study reflected the current circumstances in Japan. Lastly, the JACSIS questionnaire did not ask for the children’s gender, which might affect their emotional instability. However, children’s gender was not likely to have affected their parents’ participation in this study, and therefore, we can assume that there was no significant gender disproportion among the analyzed children.

5. Conclusions

A large-scale cross-sectional internet survey in Japan indicated that children in single-parent households were more likely to be emotionally instable than those in two-parent households during the declaration of a state of emergency in Japan. Our findings suggest a disproportionate impact of policies on children’s health. Further investigation of the potential causes of youth health inequity is warranted, and policies that support young people under a pandemic or a state of emergency should be revisited.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.N. and T.T.; methodology, T.N., Y.T., T.O., K.T. and T.T.; formal analysis, T.N.; investigation, T.T.; data curation, T.T.; writing—original draft preparation, T.N.; writing—review and editing, Y.T., T.O., K.T. and T.T.; project administration, T.T.; funding acquisition, Y.T. and T.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grants (grant numbers 17H03589, 19K10671, 19K10446, 18H03107, 18H03062, 19H03860 and 21H04856); the JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (grant number 19K19439); the Research Support Program to Apply the Wisdom of the University to tackle COVID-19 Related Emergency Problems, University of Tsukuba, Health Labour Sciences Research Grant (grant numbers 19FA1005, 19FG2001 and 19FA1012); the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED; grant number 2033648); and the Individual Research Allowance at Kanagawa University of Human Services. The findings and conclusions of this article are the sole responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the research funders.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Osaka International Cancer Institute (No. 20084, 19 June 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate valuable feedback from the JACSIS study group researchers.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Daily activity trends of the children stratified by age group.
Table A1. Daily activity trends of the children stratified by age group.
Daily Activities, %Two-ParentSingle-Parentp-Value 1
TotalSleep ≥ 8 h81.979.70.32
Study ≥ 1 h49.550.9<0.01
Physical activity ≥ 30 min53.540.0<0.01
Reading ≥ 30 min39.333.3<0.01
Watching TV/online entertainment ≥ 2 h44.450.8<0.01
Gaming ≥ 1 h43.053.1<0.01
0–5 years oldSleep ≥ 8 h88.783.50.14
Study ≥ 1 h7.45.00.07
Physical activity ≥ 30 min67.054.0<0.01
Reading ≥ 30 min35.424.50.04
Watching TV/online entertainment ≥ 2 h37.230.20.22
Gaming ≥ 1 h9.614.40.13
6–9 years oldSleep ≥ 8 h87.889.20.30
Study ≥ 1 h59.054.90.54
Physical activity ≥ 30 min56.142.1<0.01
Reading ≥ 30 min40.434.40.27
Watching TV/online entertainment ≥ 2 h40.944.60.55
Gaming ≥ 1 h46.249.70.62
10–12 years oldSleep ≥ 8 h80.079.90.23
Study ≥ 1 h75.367.0<0.01
Physical activity ≥ 30 min44.836.90.01
Reading ≥ 30 min45.238.40.04
Watching TV/online entertainment ≥ 2 h50.057.00.07
Gaming ≥ 1 h66.869.90.47
13–14 years oldSleep ≥ 8 h52.763.10.07
Study ≥ 1 h72.058.40.01
Physical activity ≥ 30 min33.030.20.83
Reading ≥ 30 min30.530.90.98
Watching TV/online entertainment ≥ 2 h58.466.40.14
Gaming ≥ 1 h62.762.41.00
1 We performed Chi-square tests.
Table A2. Characteristics by gender among two-parent households.
Table A2. Characteristics by gender among two-parent households.
Respondents’ Gender
MaleFemaleTotal
(n = 1322)(n = 1281)(n = 2603)p
Age, mean (SD)42 (7.5)39.4 (7)40.7 (7.4)<0.01 3
Educational attainment, % <0.01 4
  High school or lower 16.023.119.5
  College or higher 83.876.880.4
Household income level, % 1 <0.01 4
  Higher28.118.423.3
  Intermediate47.839.843.9
  Lower17.526.521.9
  Not answered6.715.210.9
  Number of children, mean (SD)1.88 (0.74)1.82 (0.76)1.85 (0.75)0.02 3
Number of family member, % 2 0.04 4
  ≤3 persons30.134.732.3
  4 persons48.745.146.9
  ≥5 persons21.220.220.7
Employment status, % <0.01 4
  Employer6.30.93.6
  Self-employed5.42.03.8
  Regular employee84.922.854.4
  Non-regular employee2.132.617.1
  Unemployed1.241.621.1
Living with a grandparent, %8.76.77.70.06 4
Notes: 1 As equivalized household income, “low” = less than 2.5 million JPY; “medium” = 2.5 to 4.3 million JPY; and “high” = more than 4.3 million JPY. 2 Including respondents. 3 We performed Mann–Whitney test. 4 We performed Chi-square tests for categorical variables.
Table A3. Family type and misbehaviors: stratified by children’s age (n = 3358).
Table A3. Family type and misbehaviors: stratified by children’s age (n = 3358).
Total NumberPrevalenceCrude PR (95%CI)Adjusted PR (95%CI) 1p-Interaction 2
Violence towards others
Children’s age
0–5 years old Reference
Two-parent15574.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2716.3%1.37 (0.83–2.25)1.43 (0.79–2.60)
6–9 years old 0.37
Two-parent10286.9%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2756.5%0.99 (0.61–1.62)0.99 (0.59–1.68)
10–12 years old 0.71
Two-parent10544.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent3825.8%1.21 (0.75–1.97)1.01 (0.60–1.70)
13–14 years old 0.32
Two-parent3773.4%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2132.8%0.83 (0.32–2.15)0.88 (0.36–2.18)
Abusive language towards others
Children’s age
0–5 years old Reference
Two-parent15575.8%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2718.5%1.50 (0.98–2.28)1.29 (0.80–2.09)
6–9 years old 0.29
Two-parent102814.8%ReferenceReference
Single-parent27516.0%1.12 (0.83–1.51)0.99 (0.71–1.37)
10–12 years old 0.70
Two-parent105414.6%ReferenceReference
Single-parent38219.6%1.35 (1.06–1.71)1.21 (0.93–1.57)
13–14 years old 0.82
Two-parent37710.1%ReferenceReference
Single-parent21313.6%1.38 (0.89–2.15)1.45 (0.92–2.29)
Demotivation to study
Children’s age
0–5 years old Reference
Two-parent15572.8%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2712.6%0.96 (0.44–2.07)0.85 (0.39–1.85)
6–9 years old 0.76
Two-parent102825.8%ReferenceReference
Single-parent27527.6%1.08 (0.88–1.32)1.02 (0.82–1.27)
10–12 years old 0.51
Two-parent105433.5%ReferenceReference
Single-parent38242.1%1.25 (1.09–1.42)1.25 (1.08–1.44)
13–14 years old 0.47
Two-parent37734.0%ReferenceReference
Single-parent21342.3%1.26 (1.05–1.52)1.34 (1.10–1.62)
Absenteeism
Children’s age
0–5 years old Reference
Two-parent15571.3%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2712.2%1.66 (0.68–4.04)1.28 (0.56–2.91)
6–9 years old 0.71
Two-parent10283.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2754.7%1.33 (0.73–2.46)1.20 (0.60–2.41)
10–12 years old 0.94
Two-parent10543.6%ReferenceReference
Single-parent3826.0%1.66 (1.00–2.73)1.51 (0.79–2.91)
13–14 years old 0.23
Two-parent3772.9%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2139.4%3.20 (1.57–6.50)3.12 (1.44–6.78)
Notes: PR = prevalence ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval. 1 Adjusted for respondents’ age group, educational attainment, household income and whether they lived with a grandparent. 2 Two-parent and 0–5 years were used as the reference to assess p values of the interaction between family type and children’s age group.

References

  1. World Health Organization. WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. Available online: https://covid19.who.int/ (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  2. Johanna, N.; Citrawijaya, H.; Wangge, G. Mass screening vs lockdown vs combination of both to control COVID-19: A systematic review. J. Public Health Res. 2020, 9, 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. UNESCO. COVID-19 Impact on Education. Available online: https://en.unesco.org/covid19/educationresponse (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  4. Cabinet Office. Prime Minister Abe’s Press Conference on 29 February 2020. Available online: https://www.kantei.go.jp/jp/98_abe/statement/2020/0229kaiken.html (accessed on 21 March 2022).
  5. Liu, Y.; Morgenstern, C.; Kelly, J.; Lowe, R.; Jit, M. The impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions on SARS-CoV-2 transmission across 130 countries and territories. BMC Med. 2021, 19, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Takaku, R.; Yokoyama, I. What the COVID-19 school closure left in its wake: Evidence from a regression discontinuity analysis in Japan. J. Public Econ. 2021, 195, 104364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology. School and Community Activities after the Declaration of the Emergency State. 2020. Available online: https://www.mext.go.jp/content/20200409-mxt_kouhou01-000004520_1.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2022).
  8. Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology. The Guideline for School Closure Implementation in Response to the SARS-CoV2 Pandemic (Revision as of 7 April 2020). 2020. Available online: https://www.mext.go.jp/content/20200408-mxt_kouhou01-000006156_2.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2022).
  9. Hodges, C.; Moore, S.; Lockee, B.; Trust, T.; Bond, A. The Difference between Emergency Remote Teaching and Online Learning. EDUCASE Review. 2020. Available online: https://er.educause.edu/articles/2020/3/the-difference-between-emergency-remote-teaching-and-online-learning (accessed on 21 March 2022).
  10. Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology. Educational Measures among Public Schools Related to School Closure for the SARS-CoV2 Pandemic. 2020. Available online: https://www.mext.go.jp/content/20200421-mxt_kouhou01-000006590_1.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2022).
  11. Kato, T.; Takehara, K.; Suto, M.; Sampei, M.; Urayama, K.Y. Psychological distress and living conditions among Japanese single-mothers with preschool-age children: An analysis of 2016 Comprehensive Survey of Living Conditions. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 286, 142–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kachi, Y.; Abe, A.; Ando, E.; Kawada, T. Socioeconomic disparities in psychological distress in a nationally representative sample of Japanese adolescents: A time trend study. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2016, 51, 278–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Bouye, K.; Truman, B.I.; Hutchins, S.; Richard, R.; Brown, C.; Guillory, J.A.; Rashid, J. Pandemic Influenza Preparedness and Response among Public-Housing Residents, Single-Parent Families, and Low-Income Populations. Am. J. Public Health 2009, 99, S287–S293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Tso, W.W.Y.; Wong, R.S.; Tung, K.T.S.; Rao, N.; Fu, K.W.; Yam, J.C.S.; Chua, G.T.; Chen, E.Y.H.; Lee, T.M.C.; Chan, S.K.W.; et al. Vulnerability and resilience in children during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 31, 161–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Miyawaki, A.; Tabuchi, T.; Tomata, Y.; Tsugawa, Y. Association between participation in the government subsidy programme for domestic travel and symptoms indicative of COVID-19 infection in Japan: Cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2021, 11, e049069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Naimi, A.I.; Whitcomb, B.W. Estimating Risk Ratios and Risk Differences Using Regression. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2020, 189, 508–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Chaabane, S.; Doraiswamy, S.; Chaabna, K.; Mamtani, R.; Cheema, S. The Impact of COVID-19 School Closure on Child and Adolescent Health: A Rapid Systematic Review. Children 2021, 8, 415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Meherali, S.; Punjani, N.; Louie-Poon, S.; Abdul Rahim, K.; Das, J.K.; Salam, R.A.; Lassi, Z.S. Mental Health of Children and Adolescents Amidst COVID-19 and Past Pandemics: A Rapid Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Di Giorgio, E.; Di Riso, D.; Mioni, G.; Cellini, N. The interplay between mothers’ and children behavioral and psychological factors during COVID-19: An Italian study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 30, 1401–1412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Fegert, J.M.; Vitiello, B.; Plener, P.L.; Clemens, V. Challenges and burden of the Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic for child and adolescent mental health: A narrative review to highlight clinical and research needs in the acute phase and the long return to normality. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2020, 14, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Awadalla, S.; Davies, E.B.; Glazebrook, C. A longitudinal cohort study to explore the relationship between depression, anxiety and academic performance among Emirati university students. BMC Psychiatry 2020, 20, 448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Velasco, A.A.; Cruz, I.S.S.; Billings, J.; Jimenez, M.; Rowe, S. What are the barriers, facilitators and interventions targeting help-seeking behaviours for common mental health problems in adolescents? A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 2020, 20, 293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. World Health Organization. Adolescent Mental Health. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  24. Acosta, D.; Fujii, Y.; Joyce-Beaulieu, D.; Jacobs, K.D.; Maurelli, A.T.; Nelson, E.J.; McKune, S.L. Psychosocial Health of K-12 Students Engaged in Emergency Remote Education and In-Person Schooling: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology. SOS Contacts for Children. Available online: https://www.mext.go.jp/a_menu/shotou/seitoshidou/06112210.htm (accessed on 24 March 2022).
  26. Childline Support Center Japan. ChildLine for Child in Japan. Available online: https://childline.or.jp/en/ (accessed on 24 March 2022).
  27. Sharma, M.; Aggarwal, S.; Madaan, P.; Saini, L.; Bhutani, M. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on sleep in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 2021, 84, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Cerniglia, L.; Cimino, S.; Ammaniti, M. What are the effects of screen time on emotion regulation and academic achievements? A three-wave longitudinal study on children from 4 to 8 years of age. J. Early Child. Res. 2021, 19, 145–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cartanyà-Hueso, À.; Lidón-Moyano, C.; González-Marrón, A.; Martín-Sánchez, J.C.; Amigo, F.; Martínez-Sánchez, J.M. Association between Leisure Screen Time and Emotional and Behavioral Problems in Spanish Children. J. Pediatr. 2021, 241, 188–195.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Ministry of Health Labor and Welfare. Support Policies for Single Parent Households in 2017. Available online: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/content/11920000/000363014.pdf (accessed on 21 March 2022).
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study population.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study population.
Ijerph 19 04239 g001
Table 1. Parent and child characteristics by family type (n = 3365).
Table 1. Parent and child characteristics by family type (n = 3365).
Family Type
Two-ParentSingle-ParentTotal
(n = 2603)(n = 762)(n = 3365)p
Response from mother, %49.290.058.5<0.01 3
Age, mean (SD)40.7 (7.4)39.7 (6.7)40.5 (7.2)<0.01 4
Educational attainment, % <0.01 3
   High school or lower 19.532.022.3
   College or higher 80.467.677.5
Household income level, % 1 <0.01 3
   Higher23.38.820.0
   Intermediate43.917.237.8
   Lower21.960.630.7
   Not answered10.913.411.4
Number of children, mean (SD)1.85 (0.75)1.57 (0.75)1.79 (0.76)<0.01 4
Number of family member, % 2 <0.01 3
   ≤3 persons32.365.439.8
   4 persons46.918.040.4
   ≥5 persons20.716.719.8
Employment status, % <0.01 3
   Employer3.62.93.5
   Self-employed3.84.33.9
   Regular employee54.452.253.9
   Non-regular employee17.132.820.7
   Unemployed21.17.718.1
Living with a grandparent, %7.732.413.3<0.01 3
Children’s age group, % 5 <0.01 3
   0–5 years old30.718.227.9
   6–9 years old28.125.627.5
   10–12 years old30.536.631.9
   13–14 years old10.719.612.7
School/nursery closure, % 0.07 3
   Open12.313.512.6
   Closed76.077.776.3
Not answered11.78.811.1
Online education 0.41 3
   No65.867.566.2
   Yes31.830.831.6
   Not answered2.41.72.3
Childcare by others, % <0.01 3
   No20.227.221.8
   Yes1.81.61.8
   Not answered7.15.46.7
Extracurricular activity participation
cram school, % 0.56 3
   No17.617.517.6
   Yes1.61.01.5
   Not answered6.44.25.9
Sport/hobby lessons, % 0.37 3
   No81.183.181.5
   Yes17.916.317.6
   Not answered1.00.70.9
Daily activities, %
   Sleep: ≥8 h81.979.781.40.32 3
   Study: ≥1 h49.550.949.8<0.01 3
   Physical activity: ≥30 min53.540.050.5<0.01 3
   Reading: ≥30 min39.333.337.9<0.01 3
   Watching TV/online entertainment: ≥2 h44.450.845.9<0.01 3
   Game: ≥1 h43.053.145.3<0.01 3
Notes: SD = standard deviation. 1 As equivalized household income, “low” = less than 2.5 million JPY; “medium” = 2.5 to 4.3 million JPY; and “high” = more than 4.3 million JPY. 2 Including respondents. 3 We performed Chi-square tests for categorical variables. 4 We performed a Mann–Whitney test. 5 The proportion of the target child age group is shown.
Table 2. Association between family type and children’s outcome (emotional instability and misbehaviors) (n = 3358).
Table 2. Association between family type and children’s outcome (emotional instability and misbehaviors) (n = 3358).
Total NumberPrevalenceCrude PR (95%CI) Adjusted PR (95% CI) 1
Emotional instability
Two-parent260317.8%ReferenceReference
Single-parent76223.1%1.30 (1.11–1.51)1.26 (1.07–1.49)
Violence towards others
Two-parent26036.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent7627.1%1.06 (0.79–1.42)1.01 (0.74–1.38)
Abusive language towards others
Two-parent260314.3%ReferenceReference
Single-parent76219.0%1.35 (1.13–1.60)1.23 (1.02–1.49)
Demotivation to study
Two-parent260326.2%ReferenceReference
Single-parent76235.8%1.35 (1.21–1.51)1.37 (1.22–1.55)
Absenteeism
Two-parent26033.6%ReferenceReference
Single-parent7626.8%1.83 (1.32–2.54)1.72 (1.17–2.53)
Notes: PR = prevalence ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval. 1 Adjusted for respondents’ age group, educational attainment, household income and whether they lived with a grandparent.
Table 3. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s age (n = 3358).
Table 3. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s age (n = 3358).
Children’s Age GroupTotal NumberPrevalence of Emotional InstabilityCrude PR (95%CI)Adjusted PR (95% CI) 1p-Interaction 2
0–5 years old Reference
Two-parent15576.9%ReferenceReference
Single-parent2718.5%1.22 (0.81–1.85)1.13 (0.72–1.77)
6–9 years old 0.49
Two-parent102815.1%ReferenceReference
Single-parent27515.3%1.02 (0.75–1.37)0.93 (0.67–1.30)
10–12 years old 0.74
Two-parent105414.3%ReferenceReference
Single-parent38218.6%1.34 (1.04–1.71)1.20 (0.91–1.57)
13–14 years old 0.54
Two-parent37713.5%ReferenceReference
Single-parent21318.8%1.47 (1.02–2.11)1.68 (1.13–2.49)
Notes: PR = prevalence ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval. 1 Adjusted for respondents’ age group, educational attainment, household income and whether they lived with a grandparent. 2 Two-parent and 0–5 years were used as the reference to assess p values of the interaction between family type and children’s age group.
Table 4. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s environment and extracurricular activity.
Table 4. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s environment and extracurricular activity.
Total NumberPrevalence of Emotional InstabilityCrude PR
(95%CI)
Adjusted PR
(95%CI) 1
p-Interaction 2
School/nursery 0.87
Open
Two-parent3917.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent12010.0%1.25 (0.66–2.35)1.03 (0.51–2.08)
Closed
Two-parent251815.6%ReferenceReference
Single-parent78119.5%1.29 (1.10–1.52)1.24 (1.04–1.48)
Online education 0.83
No
Two-parent28449.3%ReferenceReference
Single-parent81612.6%1.30 (1.06–1.59)1.25 (1.00–1.56)
Yes
Two-parent102718.2%ReferenceReference
Single-parent29524.4%1.36 (1.08–1.71)1.31 (1.02–1.69)
Childcare by others 0.42
No
Two-parent31949.9%ReferenceReference
Single-parent83313.1%1.29 (1.06–1.57)1.28 (1.04–1.58)
Yes
Two-parent67620.7%ReferenceReference
Single-parent27323.1%1.14 (0.89–1.47)1.03 (0.78–1.37)
Cram school 0.89
No
Two-parent330310.6%ReferenceReference
Single-parent94013.9%1.27 (1.06–1.52)1.27 (1.04–1.55)
Yes
Two-parent58419.2%ReferenceReference
Single-parent17225.0%1.32 (0.99–1.78)1.14 (0.83–1.56)
Sport/hobby lessons 0.98
No
Two-parent333810.4%ReferenceReference
Single-parent97213.9%1.30 (1.09–1.55)1.28 (1.05–1.55)
Yes
Two-parent57620.0%ReferenceReference
Single-parent14427.1%1.28 (0.94–1.73)1.21 (0.87–1.67)
Notes: PR = prevalence ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval. 1 Adjusted for respondents’ age group, educational attainment, household income and whether they lived with a grandparent. 2 Two-parent and open school or nursery and “No” to each activity were used as the reference of the interaction terms.
Table 5. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s daily activities.
Table 5. Family type and children’s emotional instability: stratified by children’s daily activities.
Emotional InstabilityTotal NumberPrevalence of Emotional InstabilityAdjusted
PR (95%CI) 1
p-Interaction 2
Sleep 0.07
<8 h
Two-parent42218.2%Reference
Single-parent13730.7%1.66 (1.15–2.40)
≥8 h
Two-parent213317.9%Reference
Single-parent60721.4%1.16 (0.96–1.40)
Study 0.47
<1 h
Two-parent80319.2%Reference
Single-parent28825.7%1.38 (0.98–1.94)
≥1 h
Two-parent128919.2%Reference
Single-parent38823.5%1.21 (0.90–1.62)
Physical activity 0.80
<30 min
Two-parent110120.0%Reference
Single-parent43024.4%1.31 (0.98–1.74)
≥30 min
Two-parent139316.8%Reference
Single-parent30521.6%1.27 (0.91–1.78)
Reading 0.53
<30 min
Two-parent148617.7%Reference
Single-parent48222.2%1.36 (1.03–1.78)
≥30 min
Two-parent102218.8%Reference
Single-parent25426.0%1.36 (0.96–1.92)
Watching TV/online entertainment 0.15
<2 h
Two-parent136415.2%Reference
Single-parent35116.5%1.11 (0.79–1.57)
≥2 h
Two-parent115621.4%Reference
Single-parent38728.9%1.43 (1.07–1.89)
Game 0.18
<1 h
Two-parent139615.8%Reference
Single-parent33117.2%1.08 (0.77–1.52)
≥1 h
Two-parent112020.8%Reference
Single-parent40527.9%1.42 (1.06–1.89)
Notes: PR = prevalence ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval. 1 Adjusted for respondents’ age group, educational attainment, household income and whether they lived with a grandparent. 2 Two-parent and shorter time of each activity were used as the reference of the interaction terms.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Naito, T.; Tomata, Y.; Otsuka, T.; Tsuno, K.; Tabuchi, T. Did Children in Single-Parent Households Have a Higher Probability of Emotional Instability during the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Nationwide Cross-Sectional Study in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 4239. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074239

AMA Style

Naito T, Tomata Y, Otsuka T, Tsuno K, Tabuchi T. Did Children in Single-Parent Households Have a Higher Probability of Emotional Instability during the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Nationwide Cross-Sectional Study in Japan. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(7):4239. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074239

Chicago/Turabian Style

Naito, Takuto, Yasutake Tomata, Tatsui Otsuka, Kanami Tsuno, and Takahiro Tabuchi. 2022. "Did Children in Single-Parent Households Have a Higher Probability of Emotional Instability during the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Nationwide Cross-Sectional Study in Japan" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 7: 4239. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074239

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop