The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. NC and EFs: Are They Linked?
- shifting may be involved in the generation of complete episodes within a narrative discourse, in the selection of informative words, and in the ability to monitor the communicative flow;
- updating of working memory may be required to generate and understand sentences as well as recall episodic contents for an accurate organization of a story;
- inhibition processes may be critical for monitoring the production of extraneous comments and derailments while telling a story and for the ability to inhibit the semantic competitors while producing words;
- planning and more complex EFs may be recruited to the extent of coordinating all the processes involved, as well as for the planning and goal setting of the story (e.g., retelling a narrative containing all of the story elements in the correct sequence [51]).
1.2. Aims of the Study
- Determine the overall strength of the relationship between narrative competence (NC) and executive functions (EFs) across childhood and adolescence (3–18 years)
- Determine if the strength of this relationship changes across childhood and when it changes across development.
- Examine potential moderators to understand if the strength of the relation changes:
- between typically vs. atypically developing children (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), specific language impairment (SLI)).
- by different EF domains (working memory capacity and updating, behavioural inhibition, interference control, shifting, planning, and problem-solving);
- by different narrative types (oral vs. written) and levels (micro vs. macrostructural levels).
2. Methods
2.1. Operational Definitions
2.2. Search Strategy
2.3. Inclusion Criteria
- at least one performance-based test related to EFs and one related to the micro- or macrostructural level of NC;
- correlational study with a cross-sectional or longitudinal design;
- monolingual participants aged between 3 and 18 years old;
- paper is written in English, Italian, or Spanish.
2.4. Exclusion Criteria
2.5. Coding
2.6. Meta-Analytic Procedures
- r ≈ 0.10 [z ≈ 0.10]: small effect;
- r ≈ 0.30 [z ≈ 0.31]: moderate effect;
- r ≈ 0.50 [z ≈ 0.54]: large effect.
3. Results
3.1. Selected Studies
3.2. Inspection for Publication Bias
3.3. Research Question 1: Exploring the Overall Association between EFs and NC
3.4. Research Question 2: Exploring If and When the Association between EFs and NC Changes over Development
3.5. Research Question 3: Potential Moderators of the Relationship between EFs and NC before and after 7 Years Old
- Typically vs. atypically developing population. We categorized the sample in typically developing and atypically developing participants based on the presence of a diagnosis (i.e., deafness, SLI, learning disorders, ADHD, and ASD). The studies involving children younger than 7 years old (n = 795) indicated that the effect sizes differed between the groups, F(1, 83) = 4.400, p = 0.039. The association between EFs and NC was almost twice in atypically developing children (z = 0.436) than in typical peers (z = 0.249), unless both effects are significant.Conversely, in the subsample of studies involving children older than 8 years old (n = 2615), the analysis indicated that the effect size was the same for typically (z = 0.211) and atypically (z = 0.196) developing populations, F(1, 180) = 0.132, p = 0.715.The number of studies involving atypically developing populations of children, however, was relatively small in both subsamples: we found only four studies with a total of eight different effects and 143 atypically developing children younger than 7 years old; and only five studies with a total of seventy-six different effects and 203 atypically developing children older than 8 years old.
- EF domains. Looking at EFs, we investigated if, before and after 7 years old, effect size differs on the type of EF domains taken into consideration (i.e., interference control, behavioural inhibition, working memory capacity, updating of working memory, shifting, planning). Results showed that before 7 years, the effect size did not statistically differ based on the type of EF domains, F(5, 77) = 2.069, p = 0.109. At this stage, EF domains are equally significantly associated with NC. However, in the subsample of studies involving participants older than 8 years old, variance in the effect size was significantly explained by EF domains, F(5, 162) = 3.399, p = 0.006. In line with the age effect previously discovered, the relationship between NC and the majority of the EF processes decreased, with the exception of behavioural inhibition. The effect size of the association between behavioural inhibition and more general NC was larger than those observed in younger children.
- Narrative Competence. Looking at the characteristics of NC, we next compared studies on children before and after 7 years old, analysing if micro versus macrostructural levels of narratives moderated the effect size of the relationship between EFs and NC. Results referring to studies on participants younger than 7 years old indicated that the effect size was higher for macrostructural (z = 0.329) than microstructural (z = 0.208) competences, F(1, 75) = 12.23, p < 0.001, unless both the effects were significant (p < 0.001). After 7 years old, however, no significant difference emerged for the comparison between micro and macrostructural aspects, F(1, 180) = 0.074, p = 0.784.
4. Discussion
Study Limitations
5. Conclusions
- Previous research has focused more on these domains taken singly than on their relationship. However, to understand human development and support it with effective intervention, we should also focus on connecting its parts. NC and EFs are promising domains because they predict many life outcomes and seem trainable. We should know much about their relationship, especially in atypically developing people and in longitudinal ways. This is to understand when and how it is better to intervene to be effective.
- Previous research on EFs and NC focused mainly on two age bands (i.e., 3–6 and 9–12) and considered large age ranges. This makes it hard to understand the development of the relationship between EFs and NC across time. Even if results provided by single studies are frequently controlled by age differences, it would be insightful to observe the correlation in more homogeneous age groups. Furthermore, since the strength of the relationship seems to decrease over time, and a turning point in this sense may be represented by the first two grades of elementary school, studies focused on this particular time window—which is been more neglected—should be encouraged to better understand what happens at this specific stage and if we can use it to support child development.
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- Narrat* AND Executive Function [OR working memory OR Inhibit* OR flexibility OR shifting OR planning OR problem solving] (filtered by age: > 18 years excluded; by type of document: NOT review)
- Storytelling AND Executive Function [OR working memory OR Inhibit* OR flexibility OR shifting OR planning OR problem solving] (filtered by age: > 18 years excluded; by type of document: NOT review)
References
- Schraeder, T.; Quinn, M.; Stockman, I.J.; Miller, J. Authentic assessment as an approach to preschool speech-language screening. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 1999, 8, 195–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fazio, B.B.; Naremore, R.C.; Connell, P.J. Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: A 3-year longitudinal study. J. Speech Hear. Res. 1996, 39, 611–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Neill, D.K.; Pearce, M.J.; Pick, J.L. Preschool Children’s Narratives and Performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test—Revised: Evidence of a Relation between Early Narrative and Later Mathematical Ability. First Lang. 2004, 24, 149–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickinson, D.K.; McCabe, A. Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2001, 16, 186–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, T.M.; Hemphill, L.; Camp, L.; Wolf, D.P. Oral Discourse in the Preschool Years and Later Literacy Skills. First Lang. 2004, 24, 123–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnston, J.R. Narratives: Twenty-five years later. Top. Lang. Disord. 2008, 28, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, N.L.; Glenn, C.G. An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In New Directions in Discourse Processing; Frredle, R.O., Ed.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Pinto, G. Il Suono, il Segno, il Significato. Psicologia dei Processi di Alfabetizzazione; Carocci Editore: Roma, Italy, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Stein, N.L. The development of children’s storytelling skill. In Child Language; Franklin, M., Barten, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1988; pp. 282–287. [Google Scholar]
- Berman, R.A.; Slobin, D.I. (Eds.) Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Van den Broek, P.; Lorch, E.P.; Thurlow, R. Children’s and adult’s memory for television stories: The role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 3010–3028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abbott, R.D.; Berninger, V.W. Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary-and intermediate-grade writers. J. Educ. Psychol. 1993, 85, 478–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berninger, V.; Abbott, R.D.; Abbott, S.P.; Graham, S.; Richards, T. Writing and reading connections between language by hand and language by eye. J. Learn. Disabil. 2002, 35, 39–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fitzgerald, J.; Shanahan, T. Reading and writing relations and their development. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 35, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olinghouse, N.G. Student- and instruction-level predictors of narrative writing in third-grade students. Read. Writ. 2008, 21, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bigozzi, L.; Vettori, G. To tell a story, to write it: Developmental patterns of narrative skills from preschool to first grade. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2016, 31, 461–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Justice, L.M.; Bowles, R.; Pence, K.; Gosse, C. A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Child. Res. Q. 2010, 25, 218–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mäkinen, L.; Loukusa, S.; Nieminen, L.; Leinonen, E.; Kunnari, S. The development of narrative productivity, syntactic complexity, referential cohesion and event content in four-to eight-year-old Finnish children. First Lang. 2014, 34, 24–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castilla-Earls, A.; Petersen, D.; Spencer, T.; Hammer, K. Narrative Development in Monolingual Spanish-Speaking Preschool Children. Early Educ. Dev. 2015, 26, 1166–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roch, M.; Florit, E.; Levorato, C. Narrative competence of Italian–English bilingual children between 5 and 7 years. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2016, 37, 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zanchi, P.; Zampini, L. The Narrative Competence task: A standardised test to assess children’s narrative skills. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2021, 37, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shonkoff, J.; Phillips, D. From Neurons to Neighbourhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development; National Academy Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt, T.E.; Arseneault, L.; Belsky, D.; Dickson, N.; Hancox, R.J.; Harrington, H.; Houts, R.; Poulton, R.; Roberts, B.W.; Ross, S.; et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 2693–2698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H.; Jones, L.L. Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 2009, 29, 180–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Müller, U.; Lieberman, D.; Frye, D.; Zelazo, P.D. Executive function, school readiness, and school achievement. In Applied Cognitive Research in K–3 Classrooms; Thurman, S.K., Fiorello, C.A., Eds.; Routledge; Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 41–83. [Google Scholar]
- Wiebe, S.A.; Sheffield, T.; Nelson, J.M.; Clark, C.A.; Chevalier, N.; Espy, K.A. The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011, 108, 436–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lehto, J.E.; Juujärvi, P.; Kooistra, L.; Pulkkinen, L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 21, 59–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monette, S.; Bigras, M.; Lafrenière, M.A. Structure of executive functions in typically developing kindergarteners. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 140, 120–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scionti, N.; Marzocchi, G.M. The dimensionality of early executive functions in young preschoolers: Comparing unidimensional versus bidimensional models and their ecological validity. Child Neuropsychol. 2021, 27, 491–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Usai, M.C.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; De Franchis, V. Latent structure of executive function in five-and six-year-old children: A longitudinal study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 447–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 1641–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carriedo, N.; Corral, A.; Montoro, P.R.; Herrero, L.; Rucián, M. Development of the updating executive function: From 7-year-olds to young adults. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 666–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morra, S.; Panesi, S.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews (2017). J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 167, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garon, N.; Bryson, S.E.; Smith, I.M. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davidson, K.; Norrie, J.; Tyrer, P.; Gumley, A.; Tata, P.; Murray, H.; Palmer, S. The effectiveness of cognitive behavior therapy for borderline personality disorder: Results from the borderline personality disorder study of cognitive therapy (BOSCOT) trial. J. Personal. Disord. 2006, 20, 450–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nigg, J.T. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 361–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gandolfi, E.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Inhibitory processes in toddlers: A latent-variable approach. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Anderson, V.A.; Anderson, P.; Northam, E.; Jacobs, R.; Catroppa, C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 385–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Welsh, M.C.; Pennington, B.F. Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1988, 4, 199–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCormack, T.; Atance, C.M. Planning in young children: A review and synthesis. Dev. Rev. 2011, 31, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Odato, C.V. The development of children’s use of discourse like in peer interaction. Speech 2013, 88, 117–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, S.; Su, M.; Kang, C.; Liu, H.; Zhang, Y.; McBride-Chang, C.; Tardif, T.; Li, H.; Liang, W.; Zhang, Z. Tracing children’s vocabulary development from preschool through the school-age years: An 8-year longitudinal study. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 119–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 2000, 74, 209–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gooch, D.; Thompson, P.; Nash, H.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Hulme, C. The development of executive function and language skills in the early school years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016, 57, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mar, R.A. The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1414–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coelho, C.; Liles, B.; Duffy, R. Impairments of discourse abilities and executive functions in traumatically brain-injured adults. Brain Inj. 1995, 9, 471–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fletcher, P.C.; Happe, F.; Frith, U.; Baker, S.C.; Dolan, R.J.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Frith, C.D. Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition 1995, 57, 109–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mozeiko, J.; Le, K.; Coelho, C.; Krueger, F.; Grafman, J. The relationship of story grammar and executive function following TBI. Aphasiology 2011, 25, 826–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khan, K.S. The Relation between Cognitive Skills and Language Skills in Typically Developing 3 ½ to 6 Year Olds. Ph.D. Thesis, The Pennsylvania State University, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Tonér, S.; Nilsson Gerholm, T. Links between language and executive functions in Swedish preschool children: A pilot study. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2021, 42, 207–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balaban, H.O.; Hohenberger, A. The development of narrative skills in Turkish-speaking children: A complexity approach. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0232579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drijbooms, E.; Groen, M.A.; Verhoeven, L. How executive functions predict development in syntactic complexity of narrative writing in the upper elementary grades. Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 2017, 30, 209–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McNiven, C.L. The Relation Between Cognitive Skills and Language Skills in Typically Developing 3 1/2 to 6 Year Olds. Master Thesis, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Balioussis, C.; Johnson, J.; Pascual-Leone, J. Fluency and complexity in children’s writing: The role of mental attention and executive function. Riv. Psicolinguist. Appl. 2012, 12, 33–45. [Google Scholar]
- Puranik, C.S. Expository Writing Skills in Elementary School Children from Third Through Sixth Grades and Contributions of Short—Term and Working Memory. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Swanson, H.L.; Berninger, V.W. Individual differences in children’s writing: A function of working memory or reading or both processes? Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 1996, 8, 357–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dodwell, K.; Bavin, E.L. Children with specific language impairment: An investigation of their narratives and memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2008, 43, 201–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ketelaars, M.P.; Jansonius, K.; Cuperus, J.; Verhoeven, L. Narrative competence and underlying mechanisms in children with pragmatic language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2012, 33, 281–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Drijbooms, E.; Groen, M.A.; Verhoeven, L. The contribution of executive functions to narrative writing in fourth grade children. Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 2015, 28, 989–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Salas, N.; Silvente, S. The role of executive functions and transcription skills in writing: A cross-sectional study across 7 years of schooling. Read. Writ. 2020, 33, 877–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swanson, H.L.; Berninger, V.W. Individual differences in children’s working memory and writing skill. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1996, 63, 358–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Artico, M.; Penge, R. Uno studio trasversale sul contributo delle Funzioni Esecutive nella narrazione scritta in bambini con Disturbo Specifico di Apprendimento [A transversal study on the contribution of Executive Functions on written narratives in children with learning disabilities]. G. Neuropsichiatr. Dell’età Evol. 2016, 36, 14–23. [Google Scholar]
- Friend, M.; Phoenix-Bates, R. The union of narrative and executive function: Different but complementary. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fernández, A.Y.; Miranda, B.R.; Casas, A.M. Funciones ejecutivas, comprensión de historias y coherencia narrativa en niños con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. Rev. Logop. Foniatría Audiol. 2010, 30, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duinmeijer, I.; de Jong, J.; Scheper, A. Narrative abilities, memory and attention in children with a specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 542–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abel, C.; Nerren, J.; Wilson, H. Leaping the language gap: Strategies for preschool and head start teachers. Int. J. Child Care Educ. Policy 2015, 9, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Diamond, A.; Lee, K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science 2011, 333, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scionti, N.; Cavallero, M.; Zogmaister, C.; Marzocchi, G.M. Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10, 2812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Petersen, D.B. A systematic review of narrative-based language intervention with children who have language impairment. Commun. Disord. Q. 2011, 32, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spencer, T.D.; Weddle, S.A.; Petersen, D.B.; Adams, J.A. Multi-tiered narrative intervention for preschoolers: A Head Start implementation study. NHSA Dialog 2018, 20, 1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Henry, L.A.; Bettenay, C. The assessment of executive functioning in children. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2010, 15, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van der Sluis, S.; De Jong, P.F.; Van der Leij, A. Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence 2007, 35, 427–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alloway, T.P.; Gathercole, S.E.; Pickering, S.J. Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Dev. 2006, 77, 1698–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCormack, T.; Hanley, M. Children’s reasoning about the temporal order of past and future events. Cogn. Dev. 2011, 26, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, M.E.; West, R.F.; Stanovich, K.E. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peristeri, E.; Baldimtsi, E.; Andreou, M.; Tsimpli, I.M. The impact of bilingualism on the narrative ability and the executive functions of children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Commun. Disord. 2020, 85, 105999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, H. Language Skills, Oral Narrative Production, and Executive Functions of Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, TN, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 8 April 2022).
- RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Rstudio; PBC: Boston, MA, USA, 2020; Available online: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed on 8 April 2022).
- Viechtbauer, W.; Viechtbauer, M.W. Package ‘metafor’. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. Package ‘Metafor’. 2015. Available online: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/metafor/metafor.Pdf (accessed on 6 April 2022).
- Assink, M.; Wibbelink, C.J. Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2016, 12, 154–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Olkin, I.; Finn, J.D. Correlations redux. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 118, 155–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, R.A.; Brown, S.P. On the Use of Beta Coefficients in Meta-Analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 2005, 90, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Borenstein, M. Effect sizes for continuous data. In The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis; Cooper, H., Hedges, L.V., Valentine, J.C., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 221–235. [Google Scholar]
- Knapp, G.; Hartung, J. Improved tests for a random effects meta-regression with a single covariate. Stat. Med. 2003, 22, 2693–2710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cochran, W.G. Some methods for strengthening the common χ2 tests. Biometrics 1954, 10, 417–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sacchetti, S.A. Lo Sviluppo delle Competenze Narrative e Funzioni Esecutive in Età Prescolare [The Development of Narrative Competence and Executive Functions in Preschool Age]. Master Thesis, University of Milan Bicocca, Milano, Italy, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Arán Filippetti, V.; Richaud, M.C. Do Executive Functions Predict Written Composition? Effects beyond Age, Verbal Intelligence and Reading Comprehension. Acta Neuropsychol. 2015, 13, 331–349. [Google Scholar]
- Veraksa, A.; Bukhalenkova, D.; Kartushina, N.; Oshchepkova, E. The relationship between executive functions and language production in 5–6-year-old children: Insights from working memory and storytelling. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vanderberg, R.; Lee Swanson, H. Which components of working memory are important in the writing process? Read. Writ. 2007, 20, 721–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunter, J.E.; Schmidt, F.L. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings; Sage Publications, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Marini, A.; Piccolo, B.; Taverna, L.; Berginc, M.; Ozbič, M. The complex relation between executive functions and language in preschoolers with developmental language disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schneider, P.; Hayward, D.; Vis Dubé, R. Storytelling from pictures using the Edmonton narrative norms instrument. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Audiol. 2006, 30, 224–238. [Google Scholar]
- Seymour, P.H.; Aro, M.; Erskine, J.M. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 143–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nanson, A. Storytelling and Ecology: Empathy, Enchantment and Emergence in the Use of Oral Narratives; Bloomsbury Publishing: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, P.J.; Reidy, N. Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012, 22, 345–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vallotton, C.; Ayoub, C. Use your words: The role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Child. Res. Q. 2011, 26, 169–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zelazo, P.D.; Jacques, S. Children’s Rule Use: Representation, Reflection, and Cognitive Control. In Annals of Child Development: A Research Annual; Jessica Kingsley Publishers: London, UK, 1996; Volume 12, pp. 119–176. [Google Scholar]
- Botting, N.; Morgan, G.; Jones, A.; Marshall, C.; Denmark, T.; Atkinson, J. Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: A study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1689–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craig, F.; Margari, F.; Legrottaglie, A.R.; Palumbi, R.; De Giambattista, C.; Margari, L. A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 1191–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thorell, L.B.; Lindqvist, S.; Bergman Nutley, S.; Bohlin, G.; Klingberg, T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 270–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wass, S.V.; Scerif, G.; Johnson, M.H. Training attentional control and working memory–Is younger, better? Dev. Rev. 2012, 32, 360–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Why Act Early if You’re Concerned about Development? Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/actearly/whyActEarly.html (accessed on 11 April 2022).
- Shapiro, L.R.; Hudson, J.A. Tell me a make-believe story: Coherence and cohesion in young children’s picture-elicited narratives. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 960–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piantadosi, S.; Byar, D.P.; Green, S.B. The ecological fallacy. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1988, 127, 893–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, S.G.; Higgins, J.P. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1559–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perner, J.; Lang, B. Development of Theory of Mind and Executive Control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 9, 337–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Effect | No. Outcomes | No. Studies | No. Participants | Estimated z | SE | 95% CI | p-Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children’s age (years) | 267 | 29 | 3410 | −0.014 | 0.005 | −0.025 | −0.003 | 0.009 |
Developmental time windows | ||||||||
Before 7 years | 85 | 13 | 795 | 0.274 | 0.029 | 0.216 | 0.333 | <0.001 |
After 7 years | 182 | 16 | 2615 | 0.212 | 0.021 | 0.170 | 0.254 | <0.001 |
Effect | No. Outcomes | No. Studies | No. Participants | Estimated z | SE | 95% CI | p-Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4–7 years: Population | ||||||||
Typically developing | 77 | 9 | 652 | 0.248 | 0.230 | 0.202 | 0.294 | <0.001 |
Atypically developing | 8 | 4 | 143 | 0.436 | 0.086 | 0.264 | 0.607 | <0.001 |
8–18 years: Population | ||||||||
Typically developing | 106 | 11 | 2412 | 0.221 | 0.026 | 0.169 | 0.273 | <0.001 |
Atypically developing | 76 | 5 | 203 | 0.199 | 0.040 | 0.119 | 0.279 | <0.001 |
4–7 years: EF Domain | ||||||||
Working memory capacity | 36 | 7 | 459 | 0.259 | 0.035 | 0.188 | 0.330 | <0.001 |
Working memory updating | 2 | 1 | 37 | 0.344 | 0.144 | 0.057 | 0.632 | 0.019 |
Interference control | 8 | 2 | 63 | 0.309 | 0.074 | 0.160 | 0.458 | <0.001 |
Behavioural Inhibition | 18 | 5 | 185 | 0.153 | 0.049 | 0.055 | 0.251 | 0.002 |
Shifting | 12 | 4 | 211 | 0.292 | 0.054 | 0.183 | 0.400 | <0.001 |
Planning | 7 | 2 | 122 | 0.372 | 0.075 | 0.222 | 0.522 | <0.001 |
8–18 years: EF Domain | ||||||||
Working memory capacity | 39 | 11 | 2248 | 0.232 | 0.032 | 0.168 | 0.297 | <0.001 |
Working memory updating | 12 | 1 | 40 | 0.135 | 0.087 | −0.036 | 0.307 | 0.120 |
Interference control | 52 | 4 | 1295 | 0.228 | 0.044 | 0.139 | 0.317 | <0.001 |
Behavioural Inhibition | 14 | 4 | 269 | 0.292 | 0.048 | 0.197 | 0.387 | <0.001 |
Shifting | 30 | 6 | 339 | 0.205 | 0.043 | 0.119 | 0.291 | <0.001 |
Planning | 17 | 4 | 177 | 0.204 | 0.052 | 0.101 | 0.307 | <0.001 |
8–18 years: Narrative form | ||||||||
Oral | 86 | 6 | 266 | 0.252 | 0.044 | 0.165 | 0.340 | <0.001 |
Written | 96 | 10 | 2349 | 0.200 | 0.026 | 0.148 | 0.252 | <0.001 |
4–7 years: Narrative Competence | ||||||||
Micro-structural | 45 | 8 | 578 | 0.209 | 0.023 | 0.163 | 0.0255 | <0.001 |
Macro-structural | 32 | 8 | 527 | 0.329 | 0.025 | 0.278 | 0.380 | <0.001 |
8–18 years: Narrative Competence | ||||||||
Micro-structural | 105 | 12 | 2476 | 0.213 | 0.024 | 0.164 | 0.261 | <0.001 |
Macro-structural | 77 | 14 | 1208 | 0.216 | 0.026 | 0.164 | 0.268 | <0.001 |
References | Location | Clinical Risk Status of the Sample | Mean Age (Years) | Age Range | EF Domain | EF Task | Narrative Form | Narrative Competence | NC Indicator | Fisher’s Z, [95% CI] | SE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Balaban et al., 2020 [53] | Turkie | Typically developing (n = 18) | 4.42 | 4–5 | Behavioural Inhibition | Emotional Stroop Task | Oral | Macro-structural | Story Content–plot complexity | 0.2554 [−0.2506, 0.7615] | 0.2583 |
Emotional Stroop Task | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.4847 [−0.0214, 0.9908] | 0.2583 | |||||||
Dodwell and Bavin, 2008 [59] | Australia | Specific Language Impairment (n = 16) | 6.70 | 6–7 | Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Oral | Macro-structural | Information | 0.182 [0.3616, 0.7256] | 0.2773 |
Working Memory capacity | Word Span | Information | 0.3205 [0.2231, 0.8641] | 0.2773 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Recalling Sentences | Information | 0.4059 [0.1377, 0.9495] | 0.2773 | |||||||
Duinmeijer et al., 2012 [67] | The Netherlands | Specific Language Impairment (n = 34) | 7.35 | 6–9 | Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Oral | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.6416 [0.2896, 0.9936] | 0.1797 |
Friend and Phoenix-Bates, 2014 [65] | USA | Typically developing (n = 38) | 5.00 | 4–5 | Shifting | ANT-executive attention subtest | Oral | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.2693 [−0.062, 0.6006] | 0.1691 |
Shifting | ANT-executive attention subtest (latency) | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.3062 [−0.0251, 0.6375] | 0.1691 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Tapping | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.1861 [−0.1452, 0.5174] | 0.1691 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Tapping (latency) | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.2059 [−0.1254, 0.5372] | 0.1691 | ||||||
USA | Typically developing (n = 42) | 4.42 | 4–5 | Behavioural Inhibition | Tapping | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.1748 [−0.1391, 0.4886] | 0.1600 | ||
Behavioural Inhibition | Tapping (latency) | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.3172 [0.0034, 0.6311] | 0.1600 | ||||||
Shifting | ANT-executive attention subtest | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.3406 [0.0267, 0.6544] | 0.1600 | ||||||
Shifting | ANT-executive attention subtest (latency) | - | Story content, lexicon and syntax | 0.009 [−0.3048, 0.3228] | 0.1600 | ||||||
Ketelaars et al., 2011 [60] | The Netherlands | Specific Language Impairment (n = 77) | 5.60 | 4–6 | - | Nepsy subtests | Oral | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.3884 [0.1606, 0.6163] | 0.1162 |
The Netherlands | Typically developing (n = 77) | 5.60 | 4–6 | - | Nepsy subtests | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.3095 [0.0817, 0.5374] | 0.1162 | ||
Khan, 2013 (dissertation) [51] | USA | Typically developing (n = 84) | 4.50 | 3.5–5 | Shifting | Verbal Fluency | Oral | Macro-structural | Story Content | 0.2132 [−0.0046, 0.4309] | 0.1109 |
Planning | Tower of Hanoi | Story Content | 0.2769 [0.0591, 0.4946] | 0.1109 | |||||||
Shifting | Card Sorting | Story Content | 0.3316 [0.1139, 0.5494] | 0.1109 | |||||||
Marini et al., 2020 [96] | Italy | Developmental Language Disorder (n = 16) | 5.17 | 5 | Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Oral | Macro-structural | Information | 0.3294 [−0.2142, 0.873] | 0.2773 |
Interference Control | Square/Circle | Micro-structural | Number Of Utterance | 0.5101 [−0.335, 1.0537] | 0.2773 | ||||||
Square/Circle | Macro-structural | Information | 0.6169 [0.0734, 1.1605] | 0.2773 | |||||||
McNiven, 2010 [55] | Canada | Typically developing (n = 37) | 6.95 | 5–8 | Updating of Working Memory | Keep Track | Oral | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness-Referential accuracy | 0.3462 [0.0101, 0.6823] | 0.1715 |
Updating of Working Memory | N-back | Cohesiveness-Referential accuracy | 0.362 [0.0259, 0.6982] | 0.1715 | |||||||
Updating of Working Memory | Sound monitoring task | Cohesiveness-Referential accuracy | 0.4784 [0.1423, 0.8146] | 0.1715 | |||||||
Sacchetti, 2018 (dissertation) [91] | Italy | Typically developing (n = 38–40) | 4.92 | 3–5 | Planning | Non-Narrative Sequences | Oral | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.4392 [0.1079, 0.7705] | 0.1691 |
Non-Narrative Sequences | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1186 [−0.2127, 0.4498] | 0.1691 | |||||||
Non-Narrative Sequences | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3417 [0.0104, 0.673] | 0.1691 | |||||||
Non-Narrative Sequences | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.2247 [−0.1066, 0.556] | 0.1691 | |||||||
Non-Narrative Sequences | Macro-structural | Story Content | 0.5037 [0.1724, 0.835] | 0.1691 | |||||||
Non-Narrative Sequences | Macro-structural | Coherence of structure | 0.5191 [0.1878, 0.8504] | 0.1691 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.008 [−0.3142, 0.3302] | 0.1643 | ||||||
Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.006 [−0.3162, 0.3282] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1034 [−0.2188, 0.4256] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.1409 [−0.1813, 0.4631] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Go/NoGo | Macro-structural | Story Content | 0.1419 [−0.1803, 0.4642] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Go/NoGo | Macro-structural | Coherence of structure | 0.044 [−0.2782, 0.3662] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Vocal Span | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1522 [−0.1701, 0.4744] | 0.1643 | ||||||
Vocal Span | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1624 [−0.1598, 0.4846] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Vocal Span | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.051 [−0.2712, 0.3733] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Vocal Span | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.043 [−0.2792, 0.3652] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Vocal Span | Macro-structural | Information and Story Content | 0.0832 [−0.239, 0.4054] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Vocal Span | Macro-structural | Coherence of structure | 0.0852 [−0.237, 0.4074] | 0.1643 | |||||||
Tonér and Nilsson Gerholm, 2021 [52] | Sweden | Typically developing (n = 47) | 5.30 | 4–6 | Interference Control | Flanker | Oral | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1409 [−0.1546, 0.4364] | 0.1507 |
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Total Lexical Production | 0.0701 [−0.2254, 0.3656] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Total Lexical Production | 0.01 [−0.2855, 0.3055] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Total Lexical Production | 0.01 [−0.2855, 0.3055] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Interference Control | Flanker | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.3654 [0.0700, 0.6609] | 0.1507 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Lexical Variety | 0.2132 [−0.0823, 0.5086] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Lexical Variety | 0.2554 [−0.041, 0.5509] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Lexical Variety | 0.4847 [0.1892, 0.7802] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Interference Control | Flanker | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.4356 [0.1401, 0.7311] | 0.1507 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.1206 [−0.1749, 0.4161] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.2877 [0.0078, 0.5832] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.2554 [−0.0401, 0.5509] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Interference Control | Flanker | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1614 [−0.1341, 0.4569] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.05 [−0.2454, 0.3455] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2448 [−0.0507, 0.5402] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3428 [0.0474, 0.6383] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Interference Control | Flanker | Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates | 0.1717 [−0.1238, 0.4671] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates | 0.03 [−0.2655, 0.3255] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates | 0.1206 [−0.1749, 0.4161] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates | 0.3316 [0.0362, 0.6271] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Interference Control | Flanker | Macro-structural | Information | 0.2877 [−0.0078, 0.5832] | 0.1507 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders | Information | 0.1104 [−0.185, 0.4059] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Information | 0.3095 [0.0140, 0.6050] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Shifting | Dimensional Change Card Sorting | Information | 0.4722 [0.1768, 0.7677] | 0.1507 | |||||||
Veraksa et al., 2020 [93] | Russia | Typically developing (n = 269) | 5.58 | 5–6 | Working Memory capacity | Memory Design | Oral | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.1206 [0.0004, 0.2408] | 0.0616 |
Memory Design | Micro-structural | Number Of Syntagmas | 0.1511 [0.0310, 0.2713] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Memory Design | Micro-structural | Number Of Simple Utterance | 0.1511 [0.0310, 0.2713] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Memory Design | Macro-structural | Coherence–Semantic adequacy | 0.1614 [0.0412, 0.2816] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Memory Design | Micro-structural | Lexical Production | 0.1614 [0.412, 0.2816] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Memory Design | Macro-structural | Coherence–programming | 0.182 [0.0618, 0.3022] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Sentence Repetition | Micro-structural | Number Of Simple Utterance | 0.2027 [0.0826, 0.3229] | 0.0616 | ||||||
Sentence Repetition | Number Of Syntagmas | 0.2237 [0.1035, 0.3438] | 0.0616 | ||||||||
Working Memory capacity | Memory Design | Macro-structural | Coherence–Semantic completeness | 0.2342 [0.114, 0.3544] | 0.0616 | ||||||
Memory Design | Coherence of structure | 0.2554 [0.1352, 0.3756] | 0.0616 | ||||||||
Working Memory capacity | Sentence Repetition | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2554 [0.1352, 0.3756] | 0.0616 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Memory Design | Macro-structural | Coherence–narrative structure | 0.2661 [0.1459, 0.3863] | 0.0616 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Sentence Repetition | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Accuracy | 0.3205 [0.2004, 0.4407] | 0.0616 | ||||||
Sentence Repetition | Macro-structural | Coherence–Semantic adequacy | 0.4356 [0.3154, 0.5558] | 0.0616 | |||||||
Sentence Repetition | Coherence–narrative structure | 0.4599 [0.3397, 0.5801] | 0.0616 | ||||||||
Sentence Repetition | Coherence–programming | 0.4847 [0.3645, 0.6049] | 0.0616 | ||||||||
Sentence Repetition | Coherence–narrative type (complete, simplified, distorted) | 0.5361 [0.4159, 0.6562] | 0.0616 | ||||||||
Sentence Repetition | Coherence–Semantic completeness | 0.5493 [0.4291, 0.6695] | 0.0616 |
References | Location | Clinical Risk Status of the Sample | Mean Age (Years) | Age Range | EF Domain | EF Task | Narrative Form | Narrative Competence | NC Indicator | Fisher’s Z [95% CI] | SE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Artico and Penge, 2016 [64] | Italy | Dyslexia and Dysgraphia (n = 54) | 9.87 | 8–12 | Shifting | Verbal Fluency | Written | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1003 [0.1741, 0.3748] | 0.1400 |
Verbal Fluency | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness | 0.1003 [−0.1741, 0.3748] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1307 [−0.1437, 0.4052] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Response set (NEPSY II) | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1409 [−0.1335, 0.4154] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Total Lexical Production | 0.1409 [−0.1335, 0.4154] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Shifting | Verbal Fluency | Total Lexical Production | 0.1717 [−0.1028, 0.4461] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1820 [−0.0925, 0.4564] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Response set (NEPSY II) | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.1820 [−0.0925, 0.4564] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Switching NEPSY II | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness | 0.1923 [−0.0821, 0.4668] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Cohesiveness | 0.1923 [−0.0821, 0.4668] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Shifting | Switching NEPSY II | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2027 [−0.0717, 0.4772] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Response set (NEPSY II) | Micro-structural | LexicalVariety | 0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Verbal Fluency | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Coherence | 0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Planning | Clocks | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness | 0.2342 [0.4030, 0.5086] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Switching NEPSY II | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.2342 [0.4030, 0.5086] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Response set (NEPSY II) | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness | 0.2448 [−0.0297, 0.5192] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Go/NoGo | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.2448 [−0.0297, 0.5192] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Planning | Clocks | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2877 [0.0132, 0.5621] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Switching NEPSY II | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.2986 [0.0241, 0.5730] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Response set (NEPSY II) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2986 [0.0241, 0.5730] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.3428 [0.0684, 0.6173] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Shifting | Verbal Fluency | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3541 [0.0796, 0.6285] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Go/NoGo | Macro-structural | Cohesiveness | 0.3541 [0.0796, 0.6285] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Go/NoGo | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.3654 [0.0910, 0.6399] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Planning | Clocks | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3884 [0.1140, 0.6629] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Clocks | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.4001 [0.1256, 0.6745] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Clocks | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.4236 [0.1492, 0.6981] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Shifting | Switching NEPSY II | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.4599 [0.1854, 0.7343] | 0.1400 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Go/NoGo | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.5230 [0.2485, 0.7974] | 0.1400 | |||||||
Balaban et al., 2020 [53] | Turkia | Typically Developing (n = 87) | 8.17 | 7–11 | Behavioural Inhibition | Emotional Stroop Task | Oral | Micro-structural | Syntactic Complexity | 0.1717 [−0.0422, 0.3855] | 0.1091 |
Emotional Stroop Task | Macro-structural | Plot Complexity | 0.3316 [0.1178, 0.5455] | 0.1091 | |||||||
Balioussis et al., 2012 [56] | Canada | Typically Developing (n = 70) | 9.83 | 8–9 | Working Memory capacity | Letter Memory Task | Written | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3541 [0.1146, 0.5935] | 0.1221 |
Shifting | Contingency Naming Task | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.4599 [0.2204, 0.6993] | 0.1221 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Letter Memory Task | Total Lexical Production | 0.3316 [0.0922, 0.5711] | 0.1221 | |||||||
Shifting | Contingency Naming Task | Micro-structural | Syntactic Complexity | 0.3428 [0.1034, 0.5823] | 0.1221 | ||||||
Drijbooms et al., 2017 [54] | The Netherlands | Typically Developing (n = 93) | 11.08 | - | - | Trail Making Test; Tower of London | Written | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.03 [−0.1766, 0.2366] | 0.1054 |
- | Trail Making Test; Tower of London | Macro-structural | Story content | 0.03 [−0.1766, 0.2366] | 0.1054 | ||||||
- | Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva | Story content | 0.0601 [−0.1465, 0.2667] | 0.1054 | |||||||
- | Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0701 [−1365, 0.2767] | 0.1054 | ||||||
- | Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.0701 [−0.1365, 0.2767] | 0.1054 | ||||||
- | Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution | Total Lexical Production | 0.1717 [−0.0349, 0.3783] | 0.1054 | |||||||
- | Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution | Macro-structural | Story content | 0.2027 [−0.0039, 0.4093] | 0.1054 | ||||||
- | Trail Making Test; Tower of London | Micro-structural | Morphosintactic Complexity | 0.2237 [0.0171, 0.4303] | 0.1054 | ||||||
- | Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution | Morphosintactic Complexity | 0.2554 [0.0488, 0.462] | 0.1054 | |||||||
Drijbooms et al., 2015 [61] | The Netherlands | Typically Developing (n = 102) | 9.58 | 8–11 | Planning | Tower of London | Written | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.05 [−0.1469, 0.247] | 0.1005 |
Shifting | Trail Making Test | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.0701 [−0.1269, 0.2671] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Total Lexical Production | 0.0701 [−0.1269, 0.2671] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Opposite words | Macro-structural | Story content | 0.1003 [−0.0966, 0.2973] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Shifting | Trail Making Test | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1104 [−0.0865, 0.3074] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Macro-structural | StoryContent | 0.1409 [−0.0561, 0.3379] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Digit Span | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1511 [−0.0458, 0.3481] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Macro-structural | Story content | 0.1511 [−0.0458, 0.3481] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Walk don’t Walk | Story content | 0.1717 [−0.0253, 0.3687] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Shifting | Trail Making Test | Story content | 0.1717 [−0.0253, 0.3687] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Walk don’t Walk | Micro-structural | Morphosintactic Complexity | 0.182 [−0.015, 0.379] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Opposite words | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2132 [0.0162, 0.4102] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2237 [0.0267, 0.4206] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Opposite words | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2448 [0.0478, 0.4418] | 0.1005 | ||||||
Behavioural Inhibition | Walk don’t Walk | Total Lexical Production | 0.2554 [0.0584, 0.4524] | 0.1005 | |||||||
Fisher et al., 2019 [97] | USA | Dyslexia (n = 92) | 9.25 | - | Shifting | Card Sorting | Oral | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.1206 [−0.0872, 0.3283] | 0.1058 |
Interference Control | Stroop | Coherence | 0.1614 [−0.0464, 0.3691] | 0.1058 | |||||||
Shifting | Trail Making Test | Coherence | 0.1923 [−0.0154, 0.4001] | 0.1058 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Corsi | Coherence | 0.2877 [0.0799, 0.4954] | 0.1058 | |||||||
Park, 2014 (dissertation) [80] | USA | Typically Developing (n = 10) | 10.00 | 9–11 | Shifting | Trail Making Test | Oral | Macro-structural | GAO units | 0.4611 [−0.2797, 1.2019] | 0.3780 |
Trail Making Test | Macro-structural | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.1318 [−0.609, 0.8726] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.0993 [−0.6415, 0.8401] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Tower of London | Macro-structural | GAO units–episodic structure | 0.038 [−0.7028, 0.7788] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Shifting | Card Sorting | Macro-structural | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.2079 [−0.5329, 0.9487] | 0.3780 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span Backword | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.2586 [−0.4822, 0.9994] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Digit Span Backword | Macro-structural | GAO units | 0.5682 [−0.1726, 1.3089] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Shifting | Card Sorting | GAO units | 0.8053 [0.0645, 1.5461] | 0.3780 | |||||||
Deaf or hard to hearing (n = 11) | 10.00 | 9–11 | Planning | Tower of London | Oral | Macro-structural | GAO units | 0.5874 [−0.1056, 1.2803] | 0.3536 | ||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span Backword | GAO units | 0.3451 [−0.3479, 1.038] | 0.3536 | |||||||
Shifting | Card Sorting | GAO units | 0.2384 [−0.4545, 0.9314] | 0.3536 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span Backword | Macro-structural | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.1145 [−0.5785, 0.8074] | 0.3536 | ||||||
Planning | Tower of London | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.1155 [−0.5774, 0.8085] | 0.3536 | |||||||
Shifting | Trail Making Test | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.1348 [−0.5581, 0.8278] | 0.3536 | |||||||
Trail Making Test | Macro-structural | GAO units | 0.231 [−0.4619, 0.924] | 0.3536 | |||||||
Shifting | Card Sorting | Macro-structural | Complete GAO units (Integrity) | 0.4047 [−0.2882, 1.0977] | 0.3536 | ||||||
Peristeri et al., 2020 [79] | Greece | Autism Spectrum Disorder (n = 20) | 9.80 | 7–12 | Updating of Working Memory | 2-back | Oral | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1246 [−0.3507, 0.6] | 0.2425 |
2-back | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1522 [−0.3232, 0.6275] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.2501 [−0.2253, 0.7254] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.046 [−0.4293, 0.5214] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.146 [−0.3293, 0.6214] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.4153 [−0.06, 0.8907] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.0993 [−0.376, 0.5747] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3272 [0.1482, 0.8026] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.031 [−0.4444, 0.5064] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.047 [−0.4283, 0.5224] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.0591 [−0.4163, 0.5344] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.353 [−0.1224, 0.8283] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1206 [−0.3548, 0.5959] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0621 [−0.4133, 0.5374] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated | 0.0902 [−0.3851, 0.5656] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of relatives | 0.019 [−0.4564, 0.4944] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.4822 [0.0068, 0.9576] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.0661 [−0.4093, 0.5415] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.4562 [−0.0191, 0.9316] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.3598 [−0.1156, 0.8351] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.2942 [−0.1812, 0.7696] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.3372 [−0.1381, 0.8126] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.037 [−0.4383, 0.5124] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.049 [−0.4263, 0.5244] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.4648 [−0.0105, 0.9402] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2715 [−0.2039, 0.7468] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.1013 [−0.374, 0.5767] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.045 [−0.4303, 0.5204] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.482 [0.0068, 0.9576] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.0661 [−0.4093, 0.5415] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Peristeri et al., 2020 [79] | Greece | Typically Developing (n = 20) | 9.80 | 7–12 | Updating of Working Memory | 2-back | Oral | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1257 [−0.3497, 0.601] | 0.2425 |
2-back | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0862 [−0.3891, 0.5616] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.2048 [−0.2705, 0.6802] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.146 [−0.3293, 0.6214] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.1064 [−0.369, 0.5818] | 0.2425 | |||||||
2-back | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.231 [−0.2443, 0.7064] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.0621 [−0.4133, 0.5374] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.2779 [−0.1974, 0.7533] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.045 [−0.4303, 0.5204] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.9417 [0.4663, 1.4171] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.2342 [−0.2412, 0.7096] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.1389 [−0.3365, 0.6142] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.041 [−0.4343, 0.5164] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.5139 [0.0386, 0.9893] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.0923 [−0.3831, 0.5676] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.7137 [0.2384, 1.1891] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.3496 [−0.1258, 0.8249] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.0701 [−0.4052, 0.5455] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 1.211 [0.7357, 1.6864] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.5308 [0.0554, 1.0062] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.2877 [−0.1877, 0.763] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.3507 [−0.1247, 0.8261] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.7582 [0.2828, 1.2335] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.2533 [−0.2221, 0.7286] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Interference Control | Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Lexical Variety | 0.1206 [0.3548, 0.5959] | 0.2425 | ||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0741 [−0.4012, 0.5495] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of subordinated clauses | 0.1186 [−0.3568, 0.5939] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Micro-structural | Number of relative clauses | 0.3586 [−0.1167, 0.834] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Story Structure | 0.6155 [0.1402, 1.0909] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) | Macro-structural | Referential Accuracy | 0.002 [−0.4734, 0.4774] | 0.2425 | |||||||
Puranik, 2006 (dissertation) [57] | USA | Typically Developing (n = 90) | 10.22 | 8–12 | Working Memory capacity | Competing Language Processing Task | Written | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.4001 [0.1899, 0.6102] | 0.1072 |
Working Memory capacity | Digit Ordering | Total Lexical Production | 0.3316 [0.1215, 0.5418] | 0.1072 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Competing Language Processing Task | Macro-structural | Information | 0.4118 [0.2017, 0.6219] | 0.1072 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Ordering | Information | 0.3884 [0.1783, 0.5986] | 0.1072 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Competing Language Processing Task | Micro-structural | Number of Utterance | 0.2986 [0.0884, 0.5087] | 0.1072 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Ordering | Number of Utterance | 0.2661 [0.056, 0.4762] | 0.1072 | |||||||
Salas and Silvente, 2020 | Spain | Typically Developing (n = 1337) | 10.17 | 7–14 | Interference Control | Stroop | Written | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.0802 [0.0265, 0.1338] | 0.0265 |
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2237 [0.17, 0.2773] | 0.0265 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.0802 [0.0265, 0.1338] | 0.0265 | ||||||
Interference Control | Stroop | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.2342 [0.1805, 0.2879] | 0.0265 | ||||||
Swanson and Berninger, 1996a [63] | USA | Typically Developing (n = 300) | 11.09 | 9–12 | Working Memory capacity | Listening Recall, Listening Generate Recall | Written | Micro-structural | Number of Utterance | 0.2769 [0.1631, 0.3906] | 0.0583 |
Listening Recall, Listening Generate Recall | Macro-structural | Content and organization | 0.2554 [0.1417, 0.3691] | 0.0583 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Matrix | Micro-structural | Number of Utterance | 0.0601 [−0.0537, 0.1738] | 0.0583 | ||||||
Matrix | Macro-structural | Content and organization | 0.1206 [0.0069, 0.2343] | 0.0583 | |||||||
Swanson and Berninger, 1996b [58] | USA | Typically Developing (n = 50) | 10.50 | 9–12 | Working Memory capacity | Sentence Span Test | Written | Macro-structural | Content | 0.3095 [0.0236, 0.5945] | 0.1459 |
Sentence Span Test | Micro-structural | Mean Length of Utterance | 0.2769 [−0.009, 0.5628] | 0.1459 | |||||||
Sentence Span Test | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.3654 [0.0796, 0.6513] | 0.1459 | |||||||
Vanderberg and Swanson, 2006 [94] | USA | Typically Developing (n = 160) | 15.21 | 14–15 | Working Memory capacity | Rhyming words | Written | Macro-structural | Structure | 0.182 [0.0256, 0.3384] | 0.0800 |
Rhyming words | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1511 [−0.0053, 0.3076] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Rhyming words | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0902 [−0.0662, 0.2467] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Sentence Span | Macro-structural | Structure | 0.1104 [−0.046, 0.2669] | 0.0800 | ||||||
Sentence Span | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.0701 [−0.0863, 0.2265] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Sentence Span | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.1409 [−0.0155, 0.2973] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Visual Matrix | Macro-structural | Structure | 0.0902 [−0.0662, 0.2467] | 0.0800 | ||||||
Visual Matrix | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | 0.1409 [−0.0155, 0.2973] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Visual Matrix | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.0601 [−0.0964, 0.2165] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Working Memory capacity | Mapping | Macro-structural | Structure | 0.01 [−0.1464, 0.1664] | 0.0800 | ||||||
Mapping | Micro-structural | Total Lexical Production | −0.0601 [−0.2165, 0.0964] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Mapping | Micro-structural | Morphosyntactic Complexity | 0.02 [−0.1364, 0.1764] | 0.0800 | |||||||
Fernandez et al., 2010 [66] | Spain | Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (n = 26) | 8.50 | 6–11 | Behavioural Inhibition | Matching Familiar Figure Test | Oral | Macro-structural | Coherence | 0.4236 [0.015, 0.8323] | 0.2086 |
Working Memory capacity | Digit Span | Oral | Coherence | 0.1104 [−0.2982, 05191] | 0.2086 | ||||||
Interference Control | Stroop | Oral | Coherence | 0.2661 [−0.1426, 0.6748] | 0.2086 | ||||||
Working Memory capacity | Rey Figure | Oral | Coherence | 0.4973 [0.0886, 0.906] | 0.2086 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Scionti, N.; Zampini, L.; Marzocchi, G.M. The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Children 2023, 10, 1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391
Scionti N, Zampini L, Marzocchi GM. The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Children. 2023; 10(8):1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391
Chicago/Turabian StyleScionti, Nicoletta, Laura Zampini, and Gian Marco Marzocchi. 2023. "The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Children 10, no. 8: 1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391
APA StyleScionti, N., Zampini, L., & Marzocchi, G. M. (2023). The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Children, 10(8), 1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391