The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns
Abstract
1. The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns
2. Discourse Functions of English Intonation
3. L1 vs. L2 English Intonation Use
4. Effects of Speech Elicitation Instruments on Intonation Patterns Variation
5. Present Study
6. Materials and Methods
6.1. Corpora
6.2. Tone Choice Coding
6.3. Data Analysis
7. Results
- HI proFESsor SMITH i was WONdering if YOU COULD WRITE a LETter of recommenDAtion for ME to apPLY for a scholar SHIP ⬀
- 2.
- DOES it afFect the LANguage or THE FORmat HERE ⇨
- 3.
- WE can reDUCE the RATES AND WHAT do you THINK ⇨
- 4.
- HI mrs BLACK, i was WONdering if i could SOMEhow get in an english one oh FIVE this seMESter ⇨
- 5.
- CAN i CHANGE my CLASS ENGlish one oh FIVE ⬀ i wanna GO with my FRIEND ⬀
8. Discussion
9. Implications
10. Limitations
11. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Aijmer, K. (1997). Conversational routines in English: Convention and creativity (1st ed.). Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beebe, L., & Clark Cummings, M. (2006). Natural speech act data versus written questionnaire data: How data collection method affects speech act performance. In S. Gass, & J. Neu (Eds.), Speech acts across cultures: Challenges to communication in a second language (pp. 65–88). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biber, D., & Staples, S. (2014). Exploring the prosody of stance: Variation in the realization of stance adverbials. In T. Raso, & H. Mello (Eds.), Spoken corpora and linguistic studies (pp. 271–294). John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Björkman, B. (2012). Questions in academic ELF interaction. Journal of English as a Lingua Franca, 1(1), 93–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2020). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 4 August 2020).
- Brazil, D. (1997). The communicative value of intonation in English. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, G., Currie, K., & Kenworthy, J. (2015). Questions of intonation (1st ed.). Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, W., Greaves, C., & Warren, M. (2005). The creation of a prosodically transcribed intercultural corpus: The Hong Kong Corpus of Spoken English (prosodic). ICAME Journal, 29, 47–68. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, W., Greaves, C., & Warren, M. (2008). A corpus–driven study of discourse intonation: The Hong Kong corpus of spoken English (prosodic) (Vol. 32). John Benjamins Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, W., & Warren, M. (2005). //CAN I help you: The use of rise and rise-fall tones in the Hong Kong Corpus of Spoken English. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 10(1), 85–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, W., & Warren, M. (2008). //→ONE country two SYStems//: The discourse intonation patterns of word associations. In A. Ädel, & R. Reppen (Eds.), Corpora and discourse: The challenges of different settings (pp. 135–153). John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chun, D. (2002). Discourse intonation in L2: From theory and research to practice. John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crawford, W., & McDonough, K. (2021). Introduction to the corpus of collaborative oral tasks. In W. Crawford (Ed.), Multiple perspectives on learner interaction: The corpus of collaborative oral tasks (pp. 7–16). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. (2004). Interlanguage refusals: Linguistic politeness and length of residence in the target community. Language Learning, 54(4), 587–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. (2005). Indirectness and politeness in Mexican requests. In D. Eddington (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the 7th Hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 66–78). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
- Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. (2007). Natural speech vs. elicited data: A comparison of natural and role play requests in Mexican Spanish. Spanish in Context, 4(2), 159–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geeslin, K. (2010). Beyond “naturalistic”: On the role of task characteristics and the importance of multiple elicitation methods. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 3(2), 501–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goh, C. (2000). A discourse approach to the description of intonation in Singapore English. In A. Brown, D. Deterding, & E. L. Low (Eds.), The English language in Singapore: Research on pronunciation (pp. 35–45). Singapore Association for Applied Linguistics. [Google Scholar]
- Golato, A. (2003). Studying compliment responses: A comparison of DCTs and recordings of naturally occurring talk. Applied Linguistics, 24(1), 90–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hewings, M. (1995). Tone choice in the English intonation of non-native speakers. IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 33(3), 251–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ikeda, N. (2017). Measuring L2 oral pragmatic abilities for use in social contexts: Development and validation of an assessment instrument for L2 pragmatics performance in university settings [Doctoral dissertation, University of Melbourne]. [Google Scholar]
- Kachru, B. B. (Ed.). (1992). The other tongue: English across cultures. University of Illinois press. [Google Scholar]
- Kang, O., Kermad, A., & Taguchi, N. (2021). The effect of study abroad and proficiency on speech acts. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 7(3), 343–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kostromitina, M. (2024). An exploratory study of intonational variation in L1 and L2 English speakers’ pragmatic production of high imposition requests and refusals. Applied Pragmatics, 6(1), 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kostromitina, M., & Miao, Y. (2024). Listener perception of appropriateness of L1 and L2 refusals in English. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 14(2), 291–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Levis, J. M., & Wichmann, A. (2015). English intonation–Form and meaning. In M. Reed, & J. M. Levis (Eds.), The handbook of English pronunciation (pp. 139–155). Wiley. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, L. (2001). The role of tone choice in improving ITA communication in the classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 35(2), 233–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, L. (2009). Intonation as a pragmatic resource in ELF interaction. Intercultural Pragmatics, 6(2), 235–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, L. (2018). Discourse intonation: A discourse-pragmatic approach to teaching the pronunciation of English. University of Michigan Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pickering, L., & Wiltshire, C. (2000). Pitch accent in Indian-English teaching discourse. World Englishes, 19(2), 173–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramirez-Verdugo, M. D., Jimenez Vilches, R., Rodriguez Merchan, B., & Aronsson, B. (2017). First and second language prosody: A study on speech production, perception and pragmatic features. In T. Navarro Tomas (Ed.), Tendencias actuales en fonética experimental: Cruce de disciplinas en el centenario del Manual de Pronunciación Española (pp. 268–270). UNED. [Google Scholar]
- Ramírez-Verdugo, D. (2008). A cross–linguistic study on the pragmatics of intonation in directives. In J. Romero–Trillo (Ed.), Pragmatics and corpus linguistics (pp. 205–233). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reiter, R. M. (2002). A contrastive study of conventional indirectness in Spanish: Evidence from Peninsular and Uruguayan Spanish. Pragmatics, 12(2), 135–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ripley, B., & Venables, W. (2016). nnet: Feed-forward neural networks and multinomial log-linear models. The Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN). [Google Scholar]
- Romero-Trillo, J. (2019). Prosodic pragmatics and feedback in intercultural communication. Journal of Pragmatics, 151, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scarcella, R. (1979). On speaking politely in a second language. In C. Yorio, K. Perkins, & J. Schachter (Eds.), On TESOL ‘79: The learner in focus (pp. 275–287). TESOL. [Google Scholar]
- Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica, 8, 289–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Staples, S. (2015). The discourse of nurse-patient interactions: Contrasting the communicative styles of US and international nurses. John Benjamins. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N., Hirschi, K., & Kang, O. (2021). Longitudinal L2 development in the prosodic marking of pragmatic meaning: Prosodic changes in L2 speech acts and individual factors. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44(3), 843–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N., & Roever, C. (2017). Second language pragmatics. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Warren, M. (2004). A corpus-driven analysis of the use of intonation to assert dominance and control. In Applied corpus linguistics (pp. 21–33). Brill. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yuan, Y. (2001). An inquiry into empirical pragmatics data-gathering methods: Written DCTs, oral DCTs, field notes, and natural conversations. Journal of Pragmatics, 33(2), 271–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Name | Description | Sample Used |
---|---|---|
Speech Act Experiment (Kostromitina, 2024) | Recordings of multi-turn video-based speech acts produced by L1 and L2 English undergraduate students | 28 recordings of 34 L1 and 14 L2 English speakers completing DCTs, 1185 tone choices |
Corpus of Collaborative Oral Tasks (CCOT) (Crawford & McDonough, 2021) | Intensive English Program students from various L1 backgrounds completing a collaborative task in pairs | 62 recordings of 104 L1 Chinese learners, of English, 412 tone choices |
Hong Kong Corpus of Spoken English (HKCSE) (Cheng et al., 2008) | Naturalistic recordings of L1 and HKE speakers in academic and business contexts | 11 recording of 32 L1 and HKE speakers, 1159 tone choices sampled randomly |
Dataset | Lang. Bkgr. | Fall | Rise | Level | Total | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
freq. | % | freq. | % | freq. | % | freq. | % | ||
Elicited (DCT) | L1 | 344 | 53.00% | 99 | 15.25% | 206 | 31.74% | 649 | 100% |
L2 | 269 | 50.19% | 248 | 46.27% | 19 | 3.54% | 536 | 100% | |
Structured (Collaborative Task) | L1 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
L2 | 249 | 60.44% | 57 | 13.83% | 106 | 25.73% | 412 | 100% | |
Naturalistic (Corpus) | L1 | 312 | 49.21% | 102 | 16.09% | 220 | 34.70% | 634 | 100% |
HKE | 317 | 60.38% | 70 | 13.33% | 138 | 26.29% | 525 | 100% |
Predictors | Coefficient | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
level (Intercept) | 0.73 | 0.12 | 5.99 | <0.001 | *** |
level L2/HKE | −3.30 | 0.27 | −12.30 | <0.001 | *** |
level naturalistic | 0.04 | 0.17 | 0.21 | 0.834 | |
level L2/HKE × structured | 1.59 | 0.15 | 11.00 | <0.001 | *** |
level L2/HKE × naturalistic | 3.21 | 0.33 | 9.80 | <0.001 | *** |
fall (Intercept) | 1.25 | 0.11 | 10.90 | <0.001 | *** |
fall L2/HKE | −1.16 | 0.14 | −8.08 | <0.001 | *** |
fall naturalistic | −0.13 | 0.16 | −0.79 | 0.429 | |
fall L2/HKE × structured | 0.70 | 0.09 | 8.14 | <0.001 | *** |
fall L2/HKE × naturalistic | 1.56 | 0.23 | 6.88 | <0.001 | *** |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hirschi, K.; Kostromitina, M. The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns. Languages 2025, 10, 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080193
Hirschi K, Kostromitina M. The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns. Languages. 2025; 10(8):193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080193
Chicago/Turabian StyleHirschi, Kevin, and Maria Kostromitina. 2025. "The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns" Languages 10, no. 8: 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080193
APA StyleHirschi, K., & Kostromitina, M. (2025). The Differential Impact of Data Collection Methods and Language Background on English Tone Choice Patterns. Languages, 10(8), 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10080193