Next Article in Journal
PLC-Controlled Intelligent Conveyor System with AI-Enhanced Vision for Efficient Waste Sorting
Previous Article in Journal
Geochemistry of Pyrite from the Jiaojia Gold Deposit, Jiaodong Peninsula, North China Craton: Implications for Source of Ore-Forming Fluids and Gold Precipitation
Previous Article in Special Issue
Predictive and Cross-Validation Analysis of Aerobic and Anaerobic Performance Based on Maximum Strength
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Not All Forms of Exercise Lead to Positive Affect: A Comparative Monitoring Between an Imposed and Self-Adjusted Prescription in Recreational RunnersA Cross-Sectional Randomized Controlled Study

by
Izabela Linhares Campos Monturil
1,
Marcelo Magalhães Sales
2,
Pedro Augusto Inacio
1,
Vicente Aprigliano
3,*,
Patricia Sardinha Leonardo
1,4,
Iransé Oliveira-Silva
1,
Raphael Martins Cunha
1,
Gaspar R. Chiappa
1,5,
James Oluwagbamigbe Fajemiroye
1,
Rodolfo P. Vieira
1,
Rodrigo Alvaro Brandão Lopes-Martins
1,5 and
Alberto Souza Sá Filho
1
1
Graduate Program, Department of Human Movement and Rehabilitation (PPGMHR), Evangelical University of Goiás (UniEVANGÉLICA), Anápolis 75083-515, Brazil
2
Graduate Program in Environmental and Society, Academic Institute of Health and Biological Sciences, State University of Goiás, Southwest Campus, Quirinópolis 75862-196, Brazil
3
Escuela de Ingeniería de Construcción y Transporte, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso, Avda Brasil 2147, Valparaíso 2362804, Chile
4
Centro Universitário Redentor (UniRedentor Afya), Itaperuna 28300-000, Brazil
5
Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Providencia, Santiago 7500912, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2025, 15(3), 1549; https://doi.org/10.3390/app15031549
Submission received: 21 October 2024 / Revised: 9 January 2025 / Accepted: 29 January 2025 / Published: 3 February 2025

Abstract

:
Objective: To evaluate the affective responses to running with imposed velocity or self-selected time trials in runners, as well as the effects on volume load (VL) and training impulse (TRIMP). Anxiety was also determined. We established the level of association between the dependent variables. Methods: Three visits were carried out. The 1st visit consisted of a maximum running effort test (VO2Max). In the 2nd and 3rd visits, participants were divided between the time limit (TLim) or time trial 1000 m running at self-selected intensity (T1000). Participants responded to the felt arousal, feeling, and anxiety SUDS scale before, during, and after TLim and T1000. Results: TLim vs. T1000 (p < 0.001) and VPeak × V1000 (p = 0.013) showed differences, but did not influence VLTLim vs. VL1000 (3181.34 ± 872.22 vs. 3570.60 ± 323.3; p = 0.062). TRIMP showed no differences (p = 0.068). Arousal did not differ between the pre-exercise (p = 0.772) and post-exercise (p = 0.083) conditions but was different during (p = 0.035). There were differences between groups in the pre-exercise (p = 0.012), during (p < 0.001), and post-exercise (p < 0.001) conditions for feeling and anxiety scores. The correlation between TRIMP and affective scores showed an association with TLim (r = 0.46; p = 0.043). Conclusion: The self-selected exercise generated positive affective responses, but the same did not occur for the imposed TLim. VL and TRIMP presented equality. There was association between TRIMP and the TLim feeling scale. TLim significantly increased anxiety scores.

1. Introduction

The literature presents evidence that intensities characterized as vigorous or close to VO2Max stand out for promoting rapid physiological adaptations in a shorter time of exposure to exercise [1,2,3,4]. Workouts popularly known as high-intensity interval training (HIIT) [1,2,3,4] have become an interesting alternative that counteracts one of the barriers related to adherence to regular training programs, in this case, the time spent in each session. Thus, these workouts reverberate an improvement in the time–efficiency relationship [5].
However, while this exercise mode appears to be efficient in the physiological aspect [6,7,8], when in exaggerated doses or when incompatible with the training status [9], it appears to provide negative affective responses [10,11], which could encourage evasion. On the other hand, the self-selection of workloads, according to the practitioner’s preference, seems to be an interesting alternative [9,12,13,14]. Since self-regulation of the magnitude of the external load is based on interoceptive and cognitive interactions, supported by previous experiences, this condition would enable an increase in self-efficacy [15] and the achievement of a positive affective valence, without the need for fixed patterns of intensities or elaborate prescriptions.
The meta-analysis produced by Oliveira, Deslandes, and Santos [16] presents the idea that self-selected exercise vs. imposed prescription have different influences on affective responses, and that the “equal intensity” condition would demonstrate better affective responses in the self-selected exercise condition, based on the premise of greater autonomy [16]. The authors also point out that intensities above the ventilatory threshold seem to induce worse responses regarding affect. Based on this understanding, the “Dual Model” Theory seems to be the foundation for understanding such responses [16]. Additionally, it is worth noting that negative affective responses seem to reverberate in anxiety symptoms [17].
Numerous studies have observed the behavior of affective responses to self-selected and imposed work intensities [8,13,18,19] in athletes [20,21], sedentary [15,22,23,24], and overweight/obese individuals [25]. However, there are no known studies that have investigated affective and anxiety responses in middle-aged people who practice recreational physical exercise, nor in the face of stimuli with a maximum time trial characteristic, i.e., an open task. In this case, adherence is not the heart of the matter but rather the understanding of the nature of the stimulus, as well as the possible deleterious psychophysiological effects, such as on anxiety levels. Furthermore, the volume load (VL) and training impulse (TRIMP) responses to a self-selected protocol are apparently unknown. Thus, we do not know, in the “let it run freely” condition, whether VL and the TRIMP performed for the self-selected protocol could be significantly attenuated compared to the imposed programming, since these would not be practitioners engaged in high performance.
Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate the affective responses of middle-aged recreational runners to running stimuli with imposed and self-selected velocity (time trial characteristics), as well as the effects on aerobic performance variables (VL and TRIMP). Secondarily, as a means of observing possible stressful conditions of exercise, anxiety responses were also determined. Finally, as a tertiary outcome, the level of association between VO2Max and anxiety scores, as well as between TRIMP and the post-exercise feeling scale, was investigated. Our primary hypothesis (H1) is based on the construct that self-selected speed stimuli will enable ideal adjustments to effort regulation, and consequently, better affective responses, in addition to equal results on aerobic performance parameters. As a secondary hypothesis (H2), a significant reduction in anxiety levels will be observed for the self-selected intensity condition, to the detriment of the imposed prescription. Finally, as a final hypothesis (H3), higher VO2Max levels will represent greater variations in anxiety scores, and higher TRIMP levels will be related to the feeling scale scores.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Experimental Approach

This study followed the assumptions described in the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement Guideline for randomized controlled cross-sectional study designs [26]. The research was carried out throughout the period of 2022, and had a total duration of four months, following Resolution 466/2012 of the National Health Council and being approved by the institutional research ethics committee no. (#1.220.339). All participants obtained the necessary information about the study and had their questions answered. Those who met the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate in the experiment were presented with an understanding of the possible risks inherent to the exercise, and later signed the free and informed consent form (ICF).
A total of three visits were conducted with each volunteer, with a minimum and maximum interval of 48 and 72 h, respectively, carried out in a gym. The first visit consisted of a maximum progressive effort test to estimate VO2Max, performed on a treadmill (Technogym® Run Now 700, Cesena, Italy). In the second and third visits, participants were divided between the time limit (TLim) or time trial 1000 m running at self-selected intensity (T1000). All volunteers performed the procedures in a closed environment, with 12 treadmills available, free from influences from the rest of the gym and with a controlled temperature between 20 and 21°. The participants performed the tests on alternate days and at the same time of day (8:00 A.M. to 10:00 A.M.). Subjects were instructed to avoid any physical exercise, alcoholic or caffeinated beverages, black tea, and the ingestion of any stimulant substance that could affect the variables tested, for 24 h before the test sessions. For the two experimental visits, each volunteer responded to three scales (felt arousal scale, feeling scale, and SUDS anxiety scale) before, during, and after an open- and closed-task test (TLim and T1000). In addition, the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) short version was applied to confirm the level of physical activity [27]. The results section presents Flowchart 1, referring to the processes of call, exclusion, and inclusion for the study.

2.2. Sample

The selected participants had to be physically active, properly stratified by the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) short version [27], and familiar with regular recreational running practice. They were recruited to participate in the study through a public call via digital media, in a high-standard gym located in the west zone of Rio de Janeiro. The calls were made during the period from June 2022 to October 2022. Participants who regularly ran at least 3 times a week and freely accepted the testing conditions, fulfilling all testing requirements, were included. Volunteers with diagnosed chronic non-communicable diseases, those who used ergogenic substances, those who had osteomyoarticular injuries, or those with a history of smoking were excluded. The characteristics of the participants are described in Table 1. For the sample calculation, an ANOVA for repeated measures was considered, establishing and input of effect size = 0.40, alfa α = 0.05; Power (1-β err prob) = 0.80, number of groups = 2; number of measurements= 3; corr among rep measures = 0; this designated a sample size of 19 participants (G*Power, Version 3.1.9.4).

2.3. Procedures

2.3.1. Body Morphology

The anthropometric assessment consisted of measuring body mass using a digital scale, and height was measured using a standard wall-mounted stadiometer (Sanny, Brazil).

2.3.2. Progressive Maximal Exercise Test

The subjects started by walking on the treadmill at 5.0 km·h−1 at 0% slope for two minutes. From this initial stage, increments of 1.0 km·h−1 (approx. 1 MET) were administered every two minutes, aiming to achieve maximum performance and effort until voluntary exhaustion. Maximum oxygen consumption was estimated according to the equation proposed by the American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) and is shown in Table 2. Heart rate responses (Polar® monitor model RS800, Kempele, Finland) as well as rate of perceived exertion—RPE (Borg 0–10) were monitored and recorded every minute until exhaustion. The presence of signs or symptoms mentioned by the participants, or maximum voluntary exhaustion itself, was used as a criterion for completing the test.

2.3.3. Time to Exhaustion Protocol (TLim)

A 5 min warm-up was performed at a self-selected intensity. Participants underwent a TLim test on a treadmill without an incline being imposed. After 5 min, participants moved off the treadmill, where for a period of one minute the treadmill was adjusted to peak velocity (VPeak). Participants were continuously encouraged to sustain the intensity for as long as possible. The test aimed to determine the maximum achievable duration at the intensity associated with VO2Max (100%) or VPeak. The values collected in minutes were converted to seconds for later analysis. RPE was monitored at 60 s intervals and then 15 min after the end of the session (session RPE).

2.3.4. 1000 m Time Trial Protocol (T1000)

After a 5 min warm-up at a self-selected intensity, participants took a 1 min rest. During the rest, the intensity was adjusted to 8 km·h−1, and then the participants stood on the treadmill. After getting on, participants were free to increase or decrease their working velocity in order to complete the 1000 m as quickly as possible. RPE was monitored at 60 s intervals and then 15 min after the end of the session (session RPE).

2.3.5. Subjective Measurement Instruments

Body arousal and feeling scales were applied at three times: pre-exercise, during (between 2 and 2 min 30 s), and 5 min after exercise. The SUDS anxiety scale was applied before and 5 min after the exercise sessions. The perceived exertion (RPE Borg 0–10) was applied after 15 min of the exercise session.
Felt arousal scale: the level of body arousal was analyzed in a self-perceived way during the experimental conditions: pre- and post-physical exercise performed. It comprised a Likert scale, which varied linearly from 1 = slightly activated, to 6 = very activated, with intermediate values.
Feeling scale: the dimensions of affective responses were determined by the level of positive, neutral, or bad sensation provided by the aerobic exercise, being distributed on a bipolar ordinal scale, ranging from zero (0) as a neutral position; +1 = reasonably good to +5 = very good; −1 = reasonably bad, up to −5 = very bad.
Rating of perceived exertion (RPE): the adapted linear RPE scale (0 to 10) as produced and described by Borg was used, where “0” refers to the perception of ‘extremely light’ effort, reaching “total fatigue” at 10. For the self-selected sessions, participants were induced and motivated to obtain the maximum global effort index.
SUDS anxiety scale: each participant described how they felt at that moment according to their perceived mental state, scoring on a linear scale between 0—absolutely no anxiety, and 10—extremely anxious.

2.3.6. Calculation of Volume Load and Training Impulse (TRIMP)

The volume load (VL) was calculated based on the product of the total work achieved for TLim or T1000 performed in both tests by the average velocity. To calculate the TRIMP, as suggested by Banister and adapted by Foster, the VL measurement was used, multiplied by the RPE of the session [28,29].

2.3.7. Randomization Process

Participants, after eligibility and written informed consent, were randomly assigned to experimental assessments of TLim and T1000 time trials. The randomization sequence was computer generated (www.randomizer.org, accessed on August 2022) and allocated by a third member of the research group, kept blind until the time of the experiment.

2.3.8. Data Analysis and Processing

To avoid possible biases in the analysis, the data were collected by a researcher associated with the project and the research group (P.A and E.P.) and analyzed by a third researcher (group leader A.S). The researcher responsible for data analysis remained blind throughout the data collection process. The names of all participants remained confidential, being excluded from the technical file and replaced by numbers.

2.3.9. Statistical Analysis

A descriptive analysis of the data was performed and presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD), 95% confidence interval (CI95%), median, and maximum and minimum values. Data normality was tested using the Shapiro–Wilk test. If normality was present (p > 0.05), data were expressed as mean ± standard deviation. If normality was violated (p < 0.05), data were expressed as median and maximum and minimum values. Given that the aerobic performance variables (TLim, T1000, VPeak, V1000) presented normality, the Student’s t-test for paired samples was used. In addition, a Wilcoxon test was used to compare the scores of the arousal, feeling, and anxiety scales. As pairwise comparisons, that is, to investigate the effect of time, the Friedman test was performed. A correlation test was applied between the VO2Max variable with differences in anxiety level scores, as well as TRIMP and feeling scores. Delta % (Δ%) was also calculated for the anxiety score variables. All analyses were carried out using SPSS software (Version 27.0 for Windows®; Chicago, IL, USA), with a statistical significance of p ≤ 0.05 adopted.

3. Results

Figure 1 presents the entry, inclusion, and exclusion criteria for the sample participants. After Shapiro–Wilk distribution analysis, normality was observed for the variables VO2Max (p = 0.170), VPeak (p = 0.167), TLim (p = 0.242), T1000 (p = 0.072), V1000 (p = 0.200), and VL (p = 0.359). However, the same was not observed for perception of exertion (RPE: p < 0.001), which was therefore treated in a non-parametric manner. The general results regarding the conditioning of the participants in this study are presented in Table 3.

3.1. Primary Outcomes

Significant differences were observed between TLim and T1000 (p < 0.001) and VPeak and V1000 (p = 0.013). However, there were no significant differences regarding VLTLim and VL1000 (3181.34 ± 872.22 vs. 3570.60 ± 323.3; p = 0.062), suggesting equality in the work performed. Likewise, RPETLim and RPE1000 (median = 10.0; CI95% = 9.8–10.0 vs. median = 10.0; CI95% = 9.3–9.9, respectively, for TLim and T1000) were analyzed through the non-parametric Mann–Whitney test, in which no significant difference was observed (Mann–Whitney U = 141.50; p = 0.072). Similarly, for the TRIMP between the TLim and T1000 sessions, no significant difference was evidenced (Mann–Whitney U = 118.000; p = 0.068). Figure 2 shows the comparison of the TRIMP between experimental protocols.
Regarding the comparison between the types of treatment (TLim vs. T1000) for the felt arousal scale pre-exercise (Z = −0.284; p = 0.776), during (Z = −2.111; p = 0.035), and post-exercise (Z = −1.732; p = 0.083), differences were observed only during the experimental procedures. The Friedman test was used for comparison of the time factors, with a significant difference being observed for both TLim and T1000 (p = 0.001; p = 0.001, respectively), both increasing significantly after intervention. Figure 3 shows the differences between groups and moments for the activation scale.
For the feeling scale, the Wilcoxon test showed significant differences between groups for the pre-exercise (Z = −2.501; p = 0.012), during (Z = −3.739; p < 0.001), and post-exercise (Z = −3.532; p < 0.001) conditions. The Friedman test was used to compare the time factor in the feeling scale, showing differences for both interventions (p = 0.015; p < 0.001, respectively, for TLim and T1000). For the TLim intervention, there was a significant reduction, while for T1000, there was a significant increase in affective valence. Figure 4 shows the differences between groups and moments for the feeling scale.

3.2. Secondary Outcome

After determining the differences between the pre and post conditions for anxiety scores, the Wilcoxon analysis showed no differences between groups between the pre conditions (Z = −1.515; p = 0.130); however, it demonstrated differences between groups for the post conditions (Z = −3.485; p < 0.001). For the time factor, differences were observed for TLim (Z = −2750; p = 0.006) and T1000 (Z = −3.501; p < 0.001). Table 4 presents the pre- and post-intervention anxiety response values. Figure 5 presents the individual anxiety responses for both experimental protocols.

3.3. Tertiary Outcome

Spearman’s correlation showed no association between VO2Max values and the percentage variation in anxiety scores (r = 0.125; p = 0.610; r = 0.122; p = 0.631), suggesting that anxiety variations were independent of a low or high level of VO2Max. However, Pearson’s correlation (normality observed only for TRIMP derived from TLim) between the TRIMP performance measure and the final scores of the affective scale showed a significant and positive association for the TLim experiment (r = 0.46; p = 0.043), suggesting that the greater the training impact achieved, the better the affective scores. The correlation between TRIMP and the sensation scale for the self-selected experiment was not significant (p = 0.190). Figure 6 shows the correlation performed. The affective domains are presented graphically based on the circumplex model represented in Figure 7. The circumplex model was structured in two dimensions using the feeling and felt arousal scales, represented in a Cartesian parameter.

3.4. Unintended Harm

Risks to health and physical integrity are inherent to physical exercise. Therefore, we monitored the effects of exercise sessions and the potential risks and harmful effects resulting from the exercise administered. For this purpose, in our study, no negative severe events resulting from the imposition of high-intensity load were observed. The sample showed potential adaptation regarding work in critical efforts. However, it is worth noting that the imposed exercise (TLim) induced significant discomfort and nausea during its practice, mainly in participants with lower capacity and tolerance. Since this format of running performance is not routine, the appearance of discomfort at a high intensity is possible. Adequate guidance was provided to the participants and contact was maintained until the appropriate period.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to evaluate the affective responses of middle-aged recreational runners to running stimuli with an imposed or self-selected velocity in a time trial format. Secondarily, as a means of observing possible stressful conditions of exercise, anxiety responses were also determined during the session with imposed or self-selected loads. Finally, relationships between VO2Max and anxiety, as well as TRIMP and sensation responses, were also established.
Firstly, our hypothesis H1 proved to be true, since the 1000 m time trial stimulus produced positive effects on affect. In principle, to our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the nature of time trial stimulus. For the most part, when the self-selection of intensity system is proposed, the literature establishes continuous patterns of intensity maintenance [30] or RPE fixed at a certain score [8]. Our findings establish that self-selected stimuli, even at their maximum demand, are an interesting form of aerobic exercise prescription, contributing to the optimization of training time (time-efficient strategy) and a faster development of cardiorespiratory skills.
In contrast, when we exposed participants to rectangular stimuli at constant load in the severe domain of exercise (100% of the velocity associated with VO2Max), the physiological stress became exacerbated and constant for several minutes, quickly leading to exhaustion. The TLim observed in our study differed in its magnitude when compared to trained subjects (220.7 ± 43.8 vs. 404.0 ± 101 s), respectively, for our study and in the study of Billat et al. [31], suggesting that participants, despite being regular in their running schedule, are far from a status classified as trained (i.e., the largest portion of the population that regularly practices exercise). Thus, our results seem to support the idea that the nature of this type of stimulus (under severe domain), even in the presence of participants engaged and experienced in the recreational activity of running, is not sustainable as a means of prescribing training.
The negative outcomes of affect observed in TLim (Pre: 1.16 ± 0.9 vs. Post: −1.32 ± 3.7), compared to self-selected intensity, can be explained based on the dual model theory, where cognitive and interoceptive factors define behavior. According to Acevedo and Ekkekakis [14,25,32], in this exercise domain, the disruption of homeostasis is severe, therefore, it suffers a strong influence from peripheral metabolic signaling. However, both experimental conditions passed through the severe exercise domain (TLim: 100% vs. T1000: 90%) and obtained different responses. In this particular case, we believe that cognitive factors predominated to the detriment of interoceptive mechanisms, reflecting inexperience with the nature of TLim. It appears that under these conditions, there is a significant decrease in prefrontal activity combined with increased activation of the amygdala, resulting in a type of emotional coping, which would explain the decline seen in affective valence [33].
When comparing the aerobic performance variables, we can observe that both experimental models presented significant differences (TLim vs. T1000p < 0.001; VPeak × V1000p = 0.013). However, it is important to highlight that both TLim and T1000 passed through the same exercise domain, which confers, according to Hill, Poole, and Smith [34], similar physiological instability effects between the tasks, and similar adaptive potentials [35], only differing in the time taken to reach VO2Max and exhaustion [33,34]. It is worth highlighting that such divergence was presented with a compensation mechanism between the variables (time and velocity), which is reflected in the equality in the results of VL and TRIMP, which did not differ between the experimental tasks.
TRIMP has been used to characterize exercise load during competitive time trial tasks and has been useful for controlling training load in long-term planning [28,29]. However, to date, it has not been used to observe possible associations with affective responses or the impact between training models. First, our study demonstrates that the self-selected protocol did not underestimate the impact of exercise, validating our initial hypothesis. The prescription in the maximum time trial model; therefore, can be designed and incorporated into training programs for adults already engaged in regular training programs. In addition, there was a significant and positive association between TRIMP and post-exercise sensation scores for the imposed TLim exercise model, suggesting that the greater the external load (100%) and the time achieved, the better the affective responses. This can be explained by the variability between participants and the heterogeneity of performances, demonstrating that those who were most potentially adapted modulated the feelings of fatigue and exhaustion, even when faced with a high imposed load, in a differentiated way in comparison to those with a lower training status.
Verame et al. [36] presented positive and similar psychoaffective responses to two protocol models and to high intensity (ratio 1:2: RPE 17 vs. 1:0.5: RPE 16) in trained participants, which suggests that trained participants showed a better modulation of positive responses, regardless of the stimulus configuration [36]. However, Rose and Parfitt [15] point out significant variability in affective responses to imposed (above the ventilatory threshold) and self-selected workloads, such that sedentary participants transitioned positively to intensities above the ventilatory threshold, with no differences to the group of active women. Logically, the domain in which the exercise was performed was different from that suggested in our study (heavy vs. severe), which would explain the differences in our outcomes.
Finally, the circumplex model shows us that despite the high physiological demands of the self-selected time trial exercise model, such activation and feeling responses largely moved throughout the upper right quadrant of the Cartesian plane during the exercise, suggesting that the self-adjustment of intensities throughout the exercise reflected high levels of excitement and reward upon its completion. Differently, the circumplex model for TLim moved under the upper left quadrant of tension, which would explain the results related to anxiety. We know that an exacerbation of the anxiety response is appropriate in the face of threatening conditions and reverberates as physiological changes; however, to a certain extent, it also negatively affects performance results [37]. Although we did not observe a significant association between VO2Max levels and anxiety scores in our study, refuting our hypothesis, we believe that such outcomes interfered with the participants individually in the TLim protocol, especially with regard to participants with a lower training status, who generally have a lower tolerance to high levels of effort and the ability to deal with threatening conditions.
In general terms, physical exercise is related to a reduction in anxiety levels in healthy people and in patients diagnosed with generalized anxiety disorders [38], panic [39], and depressives. This appears to occur at moderate intensities and even during maximum effort tests [40]. Despite this understanding, no study has specifically measured the potential effects of a TLim protocol on anxiety levels, which makes comparisons difficult. In our study, we observed a 23% increase in anxiety scores after TLim, which potentially had a negative impact on performance. Furthermore, it is clear to us, as shown in Figure 4, that participants exhibited significant variability in response to this stimulus model, which further suggests a dependence on intrinsic physiological factors not measured in our study.
In practical terms, if coaches, physiologists, and runners wish to promote an increase in affective responses, even in maximum exercise sessions, we suggest the application of open-ended tasks (T1000), since, in addition to appearing to be associated with improvements in performance metrics, they appear to be associated with positive affective responses. This is unique knowledge and can be applied to runners and eventually to programs whose objective is to improve the emotional well-being of those involved. It is worth noting that the findings of the present study should be limited to the population that comprised our sample, which comprised adult and middle-aged recreational runners. Notably, apparently, it is this population that is commonly engaged in middle- and long-distance running [41].
The cross-sectional research design of the present study can be characterized as a limitation. The main characteristic of cross-sectional studies is that they obtain measurements at a single point in time. Therefore, there was no follow-up period for the individuals, which does not allow us to know what would happen to the variables investigated after a training period, and we could not establish a suitable cause and effect relationship [42]. Therefore, the results of the present study cannot be interpreted in a causal sense. Furthermore, we suggest that future investigations perform the same measures chronically, in an attempt to track these variables over a follow-up period.

5. Conclusions

In our primary outcome, the self-selected time trial exercise protocol was able to generate significant and positive affective responses, even when faced with an imposition of maximal effort. The same did not occur for the imposed TLim. The different results of the aerobic performance variables did not impact the VL and TRIMP, which were equal. However, the positive association between TRIMP and the scale of sensation in relation to TLim suggests that participants with greater performance potential tend to manifest positive affective responses. Finally, the TLim protocol generated a significantly stressful environment, resulting in higher post-exercise anxiety scores, and was also not associated with VO2Max levels.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.M.S., I.L.C.M. and A.S.S.F.; methodology, J.O.F., I.O.-S. and P.S.L.; document validation and statistical analysis, A.S.S.F., R.M.C., V.A. and G.R.C.; data collection and preliminary writing, I.L.C.M., R.M.C. and P.A.I.; supervision, A.S.S.F., M.M.S., P.S.L. and R.A.B.L.-M.; different review steps, R.P.V., G.R.C. and V.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Evangelical University of Goiás (approval number: (#1.220.339)—approved in 10 April 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Atakan, M.M.; Li, Y.; Kosar, S.N.; Turnagol, H.H.; Yan, X. Evidence-Based Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training on Exercise Capacity and Health: A Review with Historical Perspective. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Gibala, M.J.; Little, J.P.; Macdonald, M.J.; Hawley, J.A. Physiological adaptations to low-volume, high-intensity interval training in health and disease. J. Physiol. 2012, 590, 1077–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ito, S. High-intensity interval training for health benefits and care of cardiac diseases—The key to an efficient exercise protocol. World J. Cardiol. 2019, 11, 171–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Weston, K.S.; Wisloff, U.; Coombes, J.S. High-intensity interval training in patients with lifestyle-induced cardiometabolic disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2014, 48, 1227–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Salmon, J.; Owen, N.; Crawford, D.; Bauman, A.; Sallis, J.F. Physical activity and sedentary behavior: A population-based study of barriers, enjoyment, and preference. Health Psychol. 2003, 22, 178–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Buchheit, M.; Laursen, P.B. High-intensity interval training, solutions to the programming puzzle: Part I: Cardiopulmonary emphasis. Sports Med. 2013, 43, 313–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Buchheit, M.; Laursen, P.B. High-intensity interval training, solutions to the programming puzzle. Part II: Anaerobic energy, neuromuscular load and practical applications. Sports Med. 2013, 43, 927–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kellogg, E.; Cantacessi, C.; McNamer, O.; Holmes, H.; von Bargen, R.; Ramirez, R.; Gallagher, D.; Vargas, S.; Santia, B.; Rodriguez, K.; et al. Comparison of Psychological and Physiological Responses to Imposed vs. Self-selected High-Intensity Interval Training. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 2019, 33, 2945–2952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Oliveira, B.R.R.; Santos, T.M.; Kilpatrick, M.; Pires, F.O.; Deslandes, A.C. Affective and enjoyment responses in high intensity interval training and continuous training: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0197124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Foster, C.; Farland, C.V.; Guidotti, F.; Harbin, M.; Roberts, B.; Schuette, J.; Tuuri, A.; Doberstein, S.T.; Porcari, J.P. The Effects of High Intensity Interval Training vs Steady State Training on Aerobic and Anaerobic Capacity. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2015, 14, 747–755. [Google Scholar]
  11. Oliveira, B.R.; Slama, F.A.; Deslandes, A.C.; Furtado, E.S.; Santos, T.M. Continuous and high-intensity interval training: Which promotes higher pleasure? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Batista, D.R.; Meneghel, V.; Ornelas, F.; Moreno, M.A.; Lopes, C.R.; Braz, T.V. Respostas fisiológicas e afetivas agudas em mulheres na pós menopausa durante exercício aeróbio prescrito e auto-selecionado. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte 2019, 19, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ekkekakis, P. Let them roam free? Physiological and psychological evidence for the potential of self-selected exercise intensity in public health. Sports Med. 2009, 39, 857–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ekkekakis, P.; Lind, E.; Vazou, S. Affective responses to increasing levels of exercise intensity in normal-weight, overweight, and obese middle-aged women. Obesity 2010, 18, 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Rose, E.A.; Parfitt, G. Exercise experience influences affective and motivational outcomes of prescribed and self-selected intensity exercise. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2012, 22, 265–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Oliveira, B.R.; Deslandes, A.C.; Santos, T.M. Differences in exercise intensity seems to influence the affective responses in self-selected and imposed exercise: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 61105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rutter, L.A.; Ten Thij, M.; Lorenzo-Luaces, L.; Valdez, D.; Bollen, J. Negative affect variability differs between anxiety and depression on social media. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0272107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Oliveira, G.T.A.; Costa, E.C.; Santos, T.M.; Bezerra, R.A.; Lemos TM, A.M.; Mortatti, A.L.; Elsangedy, H.M. Effect of High-Intensity Interval, Moderate-Intensity Continuous, and Self-Selected Intensity Training on Health and Affective Responses. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport. 2024, 95, 31–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Haile, L.; Goss, F.L.; Andreacci, J.L.; Nagle, E.F.; Robertson, R.J. Affective and metabolic responses to self-selected intensity cycle exercise in young men. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 205, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Collins, D.; Hale, B.; Loomis, J. Differences in Emotional Responsivity and Anger in Athletes and Nonathletes: Startle Reflex Modulation and Attributional Response. J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 2017, 17, 171–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dias, M.R.; Simao, R.; Machado, G.H.; Furtado, H.; Sousa, N.F.; Fernandes, H.M.; Saavedra, F.J. Relationship of different perceived exertion scales in walking or running with self-selected and imposed intensity. J. Hum. Kinet. 2014, 43, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Wardwell, K.; Focht, C.; Courtney Devries, A.; O’connell, A.; Buckworth, J. Affective responses to self-selected and imposed walking in inactive women with high stress: A pilot study. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 2013, 53, 701–712. [Google Scholar]
  23. Parfitt, G.; Rose, E.A.; Burgess, W.M. The psychological and physiological responses of sedentary individuals to prescribed and preferred intensity exercise. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2006, 11 Pt 1, 39–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Reed, J.; Berg, K.E.; Latin, R.W.; La Voie, J.P. Affective responses of physically active and sedentary individuals during and after moderate aerobic exercise. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 1998, 38, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  25. Ekkekakis, P.; Lind, E. Exercise does not feel the same when you are overweight: The impact of self-selected and imposed intensity on affect and exertion. Int. J. Obes. 2006, 30, 652–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Von Elm, E.; Altman, D.G.; Egger, M.; Pocock, S.J.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Vandenbroucke, J.P.; STROBE Initiative. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2008, 61, 344–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Matsudo, S.; Araújo, T.; Matsudo, V.; Andrade, D.; Andrade, E.; Oliveira, L.C.; Braggion, G. International Physical Activity Questionnaire (lPAQ): Study of Validaty and Reliability in Brazil. Rev. Bras. Atividade Física Saúde. 2001, 6, 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  28. Foster, C.; Florhaug, J.A.; Franklin, J.; Gottschall, L.; Hrovatin, L.A.; Parker, S.; Doleshal, P.; Dodge, C. A new approach to monitoring exercise training. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 2001, 15, 109–115. [Google Scholar]
  29. Banister, E.W.; Carter, J.B.; Zarkadas, P.C. Training theory and taper: Validation in triathlon athletes. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1999, 79, 182–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Oliveira Deslandes, A.C.; Nakamura, F.Y.; Viana, B.F.; Santos, T.M. Self-selected or imposed exercise? A different approach for affective comparisons. J. Sports Sci. 2015, 33, 777–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Billat, V.; Renoux, J.C.; Pinoteau, J.; Petit, B.; Koralsztein, J.P. Reproducibility of running time to exhaustion at VO2max in subelite runners. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1994, 26, 254–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Acevedo, E.; Ekkekakis, P. Psychobiology of Physical Activity; Human Kinetics Publishers: Champaign, IL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  33. MacInnis, M.J.; Gibala, M.J. Physiological adaptations to interval training and the role of exercise intensity. J. Physiol. 2017, 595, 2915–2930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hill, D.W.; Poole, D.C.; Smith, J.C. The relationship between power and the time to achieve VO2max. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2002, 34, 709–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Billat, V.; Renoux, J.C.; Pinoteau, J.; Petit, B.; Koralsztein, J.P. Times to exhaustion at 90, 100 and 105% of velocity at VO2 max (maximal aerobic speed) and critical speed in elite long-distance runners. Arch. Physiol. Biochem. 1995, 103, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Verame, A.; Santana, W.; da Silva, C.A.; Barbosa, E.; Figueira Júnior, A. Physiological and Psychoaffective Responses of Adults Trained in Acute HIIT Protocols. Rev. Bras. Med. Esporte 2024, 30, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ganasarajah, S.; Poromaa, I.S.; Thu, W.P.; Kramer, M.S.; Logan, S.; Cauley, J.A.; Yong, E.-L. Objective measures of physical performance associated with depression and/or anxiety in midlife Singaporean women. Menopause 2019, 26, 1045–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Mochcovitch, M.D.; Deslandes, A.C.; Freire, R.C.; Garcia, R.F.; Nardi, A.E. The effects of regular physical activity on anxiety symptoms in healthy older adults: A systematic review. Braz. J. Psychiatry 2016, 38, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Machado, S.; Telles, G.; Magalhaes, F.; Teixeira, D.; Amatriain-Fernández, S.; Budde, H.; Imperatori, C.; Murillo-Rodriguez, E.; Monteiro, D.; Correia, D.T.; et al. Can regular physical exercise be a treatment for panic disorder? A systematic review. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 2022, 22, 53–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. O’Connor, P.J.; Petruzzello, S.J.; Kubitz, K.A.; Robinson, T.L. Anxiety responses to maximal exercise testing. Br. J. Sports Med. 1995, 29, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Torres, F.C.; Gomes, A.C.; Silva, S.G. Characteristics of training and association with injuries in recreational road runners. Rev. Bras. Med. Esporte 2020, 5, 410–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Savitz, D.A.; Wellenius, G.A. Can Cross-Sectional Studies Contribute to Causal Inference? It Depends. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2023, 192, 514–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Entry, inclusion, and exclusion criteria for sample participants.
Figure 1. Entry, inclusion, and exclusion criteria for sample participants.
Applsci 15 01549 g001
Figure 2. Comparison of training impulse (TRIMP) between experimental protocols. VL = volume load; RPE = rate of perceived exertion.
Figure 2. Comparison of training impulse (TRIMP) between experimental protocols. VL = volume load; RPE = rate of perceived exertion.
Applsci 15 01549 g002
Figure 3. Differences between groups and moments for felt arousal scale. TLim = time limit until exhaustion; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. = Significant difference between sessions (Tlim vs. T1000). * = Significant intra-session difference, i.e., between moments (pre; during; post) of the same session.
Figure 3. Differences between groups and moments for felt arousal scale. TLim = time limit until exhaustion; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. = Significant difference between sessions (Tlim vs. T1000). * = Significant intra-session difference, i.e., between moments (pre; during; post) of the same session.
Applsci 15 01549 g003
Figure 4. Differences between groups and moments for the feeling scale in the face of both interventions. TLim = time limit until exhaustion; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. = Significant difference between sessions (Tlim vs. T1000). * = Significant intra-session difference, i.e., between moments (pre; during; post) of the same session.
Figure 4. Differences between groups and moments for the feeling scale in the face of both interventions. TLim = time limit until exhaustion; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. = Significant difference between sessions (Tlim vs. T1000). * = Significant intra-session difference, i.e., between moments (pre; during; post) of the same session.
Applsci 15 01549 g004
Figure 5. Individual variability of anxiety responses to experimental protocols.
Figure 5. Individual variability of anxiety responses to experimental protocols.
Applsci 15 01549 g005
Figure 6. Correlation between training impulse (TRIMP) and affective responses for the imposed time limit until exhaustion (TLim) experiment. VL = volume load; RPE = rate of perceived exertion.
Figure 6. Correlation between training impulse (TRIMP) and affective responses for the imposed time limit until exhaustion (TLim) experiment. VL = volume load; RPE = rate of perceived exertion.
Applsci 15 01549 g006
Figure 7. Circumplex model of affect.
Figure 7. Circumplex model of affect.
Applsci 15 01549 g007
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Sample CharacteristicsExperimental Group (Mean ± SD)
Age (years)43.3 ± 4.2
Body mass (kg)68.3 ± 9.5
Stature (cm)170.5 ± 8.3
Body mass index (kg/m2)23.6 ± 1.3
Training experience (years)5.4 ± 4.3
Table 2. VO2Max estimation equation.
Table 2. VO2Max estimation equation.
VO2Max = (0.2 × velocity × slope) + 3.5
Where
VO2Max—maximum oxygen consumption in mL·kg1·min1;
Velocity—m·min−1;
Slope—centesimal unit.
Table 3. Aerobic performance responses to the imposed TLim and self-selected T1000 experimental protocol.
Table 3. Aerobic performance responses to the imposed TLim and self-selected T1000 experimental protocol.
ImposedSelf-Selected
VO2MaxTLimVPeakT1000V1000
(mL·kg−1·min−1)(s)(km·h−1)(%)(s)(km·h−1)(%)
Mean51.1220.714.3100.0284.812.990.0
SD5.743.81.70.035.11.76.4
TLim = time limit until exhaustion; VPeak = peak velocity; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. V1000 = average velocity for 1000 m; SD = standard deviation.
Table 4. Pre- and post-exercise anxiety scores in the face of TLim intervention and T1000 time trial.
Table 4. Pre- and post-exercise anxiety scores in the face of TLim intervention and T1000 time trial.
TLim T1000
Pre (Score)Post (Score)Diff (Absolute)∆ (%)Pre (Score)Post (Score)Diff (Absolute)∆ (%)
Mean3.94.80.9234.32.71.6−37
SD0.91.3 1.61.4
TLim = time limit until exhaustion; T1000 = time taken to cover 1000 m. ∆% = percentage variation from pre- and post-exercise moments for the anxiety scale. Diff = differences between pre- and post-exercise conditions.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Monturil, I.L.C.; Sales, M.M.; Inacio, P.A.; Aprigliano, V.; Leonardo, P.S.; Oliveira-Silva, I.; Cunha, R.M.; Chiappa, G.R.; Fajemiroye, J.O.; Vieira, R.P.; et al. Not All Forms of Exercise Lead to Positive Affect: A Comparative Monitoring Between an Imposed and Self-Adjusted Prescription in Recreational RunnersA Cross-Sectional Randomized Controlled Study. Appl. Sci. 2025, 15, 1549. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15031549

AMA Style

Monturil ILC, Sales MM, Inacio PA, Aprigliano V, Leonardo PS, Oliveira-Silva I, Cunha RM, Chiappa GR, Fajemiroye JO, Vieira RP, et al. Not All Forms of Exercise Lead to Positive Affect: A Comparative Monitoring Between an Imposed and Self-Adjusted Prescription in Recreational RunnersA Cross-Sectional Randomized Controlled Study. Applied Sciences. 2025; 15(3):1549. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15031549

Chicago/Turabian Style

Monturil, Izabela Linhares Campos, Marcelo Magalhães Sales, Pedro Augusto Inacio, Vicente Aprigliano, Patricia Sardinha Leonardo, Iransé Oliveira-Silva, Raphael Martins Cunha, Gaspar R. Chiappa, James Oluwagbamigbe Fajemiroye, Rodolfo P. Vieira, and et al. 2025. "Not All Forms of Exercise Lead to Positive Affect: A Comparative Monitoring Between an Imposed and Self-Adjusted Prescription in Recreational RunnersA Cross-Sectional Randomized Controlled Study" Applied Sciences 15, no. 3: 1549. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15031549

APA Style

Monturil, I. L. C., Sales, M. M., Inacio, P. A., Aprigliano, V., Leonardo, P. S., Oliveira-Silva, I., Cunha, R. M., Chiappa, G. R., Fajemiroye, J. O., Vieira, R. P., Lopes-Martins, R. A. B., & Sá Filho, A. S. (2025). Not All Forms of Exercise Lead to Positive Affect: A Comparative Monitoring Between an Imposed and Self-Adjusted Prescription in Recreational RunnersA Cross-Sectional Randomized Controlled Study. Applied Sciences, 15(3), 1549. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15031549

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop