Cross-Cultural Comparison of Relationships between Empathy and Implicit Theories of Emotions (in Chinese and Russians)
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Empathy
1.2. Implicit Theories of Emotions
1.3. Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Psychological Assessments
- Implicit theories of emotion scale (ITE in [45]) includes 4 items, with 2 items indexing constant and incremental theories each. The participants were asked to evaluate the extent to which they agree with a set of statements using a 5-point Likert scale. The outcome metric from this assessment was based on the one-factor solution and indexed the extent to which the person thinks that they can control their own emotions (e.g., by endorsing a statement such as “Everyone can learn to control their emotions”). A preliminary study by our group confirmed a one-factor solution. Using Cronbach’s alpha (internal consistency) coefficient, we estimated the reliability at scale ITE α = 0.623 for Chinese living in China; α = 0.710 for Chinese living in Russia; and α = 0.692 for Russians living in Russia.
- Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy, QCAE [18]; for the Chinese adaptation, see [40,55]. In our adaptation [56], QCAE includes 31 items requiring the participant to indicate the extent to which they agree with the statement using a 4-point Likert scale. Unlike Davis’ IRI [16], QCAE also includes two second-order factors—cognitive empathy and affective empathy. Unlike in other studies [40,55], in our Chinese sample, a 4-factor model of the questionnaire was established. The cognitive empathy component consisted of 2 subscales: Perspective taking (10 items) assesses the extent to which participants can see things from another’s point-of-view (e.g., “I can easily figure out what another person might want to talk about”); online simulation (9 items) measures the extent to which a person tries to or wants to put oneself in another person’s position by imagining what that person is feeling (e.g., “Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I was in their place”). The Affective empathy component consisted of 2 subscales: Emotional contagion (8 items) assesses the automatic mirroring of the feelings of others (e.g., “I am happy when I am with a cheerful group and sad when the others are glum”); peripheral responsiveness (4 items) assesses ones’ emotional responsiveness to the moods of others in a detached social context (e.g., “I usually stay emotionally detached when watching a film”).
2.3. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Group Differences
3.2. Gender Differences
3.3. Correlational Analysis
3.4. Latent Class Analysis
- Russians (Figure 5)
- Chinese in Russia (Figure 6)
- Chinese in China (Figure 7)
3.5. Class-Specific Correlational Analysis
3.6. Relationships in Student Subsamples
4. Discussion
5. Limitations
6. Conclusions
- We established significant cross-cultural differences between the Chinese and the Russian groups: individuals from China showed higher cognitive and affective empathy levels and tended to adopt more constant implicit theories of emotion, compared to Russians. Chinese living in Russia showed lower empathy compared with Chinese living in China. Russians demonstrated the highest levels of adoption of incremental theories of emotion, including emotional control.
- The role of current and immediate social surroundings is evident in a preferential coupling between incremental implicit theories of emotion and empathy in Chinese living in China but not Chinese living in Russia. These findings suggest a dissonance in emotional regulation in those individuals who live abroad, manifested in this case as negative relationships between empathy and measured orientation towards the emotions of others as well as belief in malleability and controllability of one’s emotions.
- Latent class analysis is a powerful person-centered technique that identified two distinct latent classes of participants that mapped onto several types of psychological regulation that differentiated between different cultural groups.
- The gender differences are less pronounced in the Russian sample in comparison to the Chinese sample, which can be interpreted as evidence towards viewing Russia as being more aligned with the individualistic West and China as more collective.
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
ITE | Cognitive Empathy | Affective Empathy | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
ITE | Group 1 | 1 | ||
Group 2 | ||||
Group 3 | ||||
Cognitive Empathy | Group 1 | 0.078 | 1 | |
Group 2 | 0.061 | |||
Group 3 | 0.212 ** | |||
Affective Empathy | Group 1 | 0.011 | 0.330 ** | 1 |
Group 2 | 0.055 | 0.219 ** | ||
Group 3 | 0.007 | 0.195 ** |
References
- Triandis, H.C. Individualism and Collectivism; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; ISBN 9780813318509. [Google Scholar]
- Grossmann, I.; Santos, H. Individualistic cultures. In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiaxue, C.A.O. The analysis of tendency of transition from collectivism to individualism in China. Cross-Cult. Commun. 2009, 5, 42–50. [Google Scholar]
- Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Q.; Conway, M.A. The stories we keep: Autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-Aged adults. J. Personal. 2004, 72, 911–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofstede Insights. 1 May 2020. Available online: https://www.hofstede-insights.com (accessed on 1 May 2020).
- Kitayama, S.; Markus, H.R.; Kurokawa, M. Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cogn. Emot. 2000, 14, 93–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyamoto, Y.; Ma, X. Dampening or savoring positive emotions: A dialectical cultural script guides emotion regulation. Emotion 2011, 11, 1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gökçen, E.; Furnham, A.; Mavroveli, S.; Petrides, K.V. A cross-cultural investigation of trait emotional intelligence in Hong Kong and the UK. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 65, 30–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ji, L.J.; Zhang, Z.; Usborne, E.; Guan, Y. Optimism across cultures: In response to the severe acute respiratory syndrome outbreak. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 7, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Powell, P.A. Individual differences in emotion regulation moderate the associations between empathy and affective distress. Motiv. Emot. 2018, 42, 602–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Telle, N.T.; Pfister, H.R. Not only the miserable receive help: Empathy promotes prosocial behaviour toward the happy. Curr. Psychol. 2012, 31, 393–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfattheicher, S.; Nockur, L.; Böhm, R.; Sassenrath, C.; Petersen, M.B. The emotional path to action: Empathy promotes physical distancing and wearing of face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 1363–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zirenko, M.S.; Kornilova, T.V.; Zhou, Q.; Izmailova, A. Personality regulation of decisions on physical distancing: Cross-cultural comparison (Russia, Azerbaijan, China). Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 170, 110418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Longji, S. The Deep Structure of Chinese Culture; Huaxia Publishing House: Beijing, China, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Davis, M.H. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 44, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henet, P.; Givron, Н.; Desseilles, M. Empathy in medical students: A rapid review of the literature in 10 key questions. Acta Psychiatr. Belg. 2020, 120, 38–48. [Google Scholar]
- Reniers, R.L.; Corcoran, R.; Drake, R.; Shryane, N.M.; Völlm, B.A. The QCAE: A questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J. Personal. Assess. 2011, 93, 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelm, Z.; Womer, J.; Walter, J.K.; Feudtner, C. Interventions to cultivate physician empathy: A systematic review. BMC Med. Educ. 2014, 14, 219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hoffman, M.L. Empathy and Moral Development; Cambridge University Press Online: Cambridge, UK, 2002; ISBN 9780511805851. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, C.; Wang, Y.; Yu, G.; Wang, Y. A review of the related theories of empathy and exploration of dynamic models. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 17, 964–972. [Google Scholar]
- Kornilova, T.V. Эмпатия в структуре интеллектуальнo-личнoстнoгo пoтенциала: единствo интеллекта и аффекта. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal 2021, 42. in press. (In Russian, Abstr. In English). [Google Scholar]
- Nourkova, V.V. Cultural development of Empathy-identification and Empathy-modeling. Natl. Psychol. J. 2020, 4, 3–17, (In Russian, Abstr. In English). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández-Abascal, E.G.; Martín-Díaz, M.D. Relations between dimensions of emotional intelligence, specific aspects of empathy, and non-verbal sensitivity. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, M.; Du, T. Associations of emotional intelligence and gratitude with empathy in medical students. BMC Med. Educ. 2020, 20, 116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schipper, M.; Petermann, F. Relating empathy and emotion regulation: Do deficits in empathy trigger emotion dysregulation? Soc. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lasota, A.; Tomaszek, K.; Bosacki, S. How to become more grateful? The mediating role of resilience between empathy and gratitude. Curr. Psychol. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarrionandia, A.; Mikolajczak, M. A meta-analysis of the possible behavioural and biological variables linking trait emotional intelligence to health. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 220–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Decety, J. The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Dev. Neurosci. 2010, 32, 257–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bodrogi, B.; Bereczkei, T.; Deak, A. Be aware, make it clear, and take the Lead: Emotion regulation difficulties and emotional intelligence as moderators of cognitive reappraisal. Curr. Psychol. 2020, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Habashi, M.M.; William, G.; Graziano, W.G.; Hoover, A.E. Searching for the Prosocial Personality: A Big Five Approach to Linking Personality and Prosocial Behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 42, 1177–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Batson, C.D.; Lishner, D.A.; Stocks, E.L. The empathy—Altruism hypothesis. In Oxford Library of Psychology; Schroeder, D.A., Graziano, W.G., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stocks, E.L.; Lishner, D.A.; Decker, S.K. Altruism or psychological escape: Why does empathy promote prosocial behavior? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 39, 649–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrio, V.D.; Aluja, A.; García, L.F. Relationship between empathy and the Big Five personality traits in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2004, 32, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baron-Cohen, S. The Science of Evil: On Empathy and the Origins of Cruelty; Chinese edition; Basic books; Guangxi Normal University Press: Guilin, China, 2012; ISBN 9787559810397. [Google Scholar]
- Barlińska, J.; Szuster, A.; Winiewski, M. Cyberbullying among adolescent bystanders: Role of affective versus cognitive empathy in increasing prosocial cyberbystander behavior. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Karyagina, T.D.; Kukhtova, N.V.М. Davis empathy test: Content validity and adaptation in an intercultural context. Couns. Psychol. Psychother. 2016, 24, 33–61, (In Russian, Abstr. In English). [Google Scholar]
- Decety, J.; Michalska, K.J. Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. Dev. Sci. 2010, 13, 886–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Kornilov, S.A. Latent profiles of personality traits related to decision making about social distancing (in Russia and Azerbaijan). Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal 2021, 42, 27–38, (In Russian, Abstr. In English). [Google Scholar]
- Wang, X.S.; Su, Y.J. Revision of QCAE empathy scale for Chinese adolescents. Psychol. Tech. Appl. 2019, 7, 536–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belousova, A.I.; Geyvandova, M.R. Сognitive and affective empathy: Approbation of the questionnaire on a russian sample. Bull. Mosc. Region. State Univ. Ser. Psychol. 2021, 31, 6–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dweck, C.S. Can personality be changed? The role of beliefs in personality and change. Personal. Chang. 2008, 17, 391–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, R.; Forsythe, G.; Hedlund, J.; Horvath, J.A.; Wagner, R.K.; Williams, W.M.; Snook, S.A.; Grigorenko, E.L. Practical Intelligence in Everyday Life; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; ISBN 0521650569. [Google Scholar]
- Cui, Y.; Wang, P.; Tan, C. The influence of implicit personality theory on the processing strategies of others’ impressions. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2016, 48, 1538–1550. [Google Scholar]
- Tamir, M.; John, O.P.; Srivastava, S.; Gross, J.J. Implicit theories of emotion: Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De France, K.; Hollenstein, T. Implicit theories of emotion and mental health during adolescence: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Cogn. Emot. 2021, 35, 367–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodman, F.R.; Kashdan, T.B.; İmamoğlu, A. Valuing emotional control in social anxiety disorder: A multimethod study of emotion beliefs and emotion regulation. Emotion 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kneeland, E.T.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S.; Dovidio, J.F.; Gruber, J. Убеждения o пoдатливoсти эмoций влияют на сoстoяние регуляцииэмoций. Мoтивация и эмoции 2016, 40, 740–749. [Google Scholar]
- Schroder, H.S.; Dawood, S.; Yalch, M.M.; Donnellan, M.B.; Moser, J.S. The role of implicit theories in mental health symptoms, emotion regulation, and hypothetical treatment choices in college students. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2015, 39, 120–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Brien, E.; Konrath, S.H.; Grühn, D.; Hagen, A.L. Empathic concern and perspective taking: Linear and quadratic effects of age across the adult life span. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2013, 68, 168–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singer, T.; Seymour, B.; O’Doherty, J.P.; Stephan, K.E.; Dolan, R.J.; Frith, C.D. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 2006, 439, 466–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hojat, M.; Gonnella, J.S.; Mangione, S.; Nasca, T.J.; Veloski, J.J.; Erdmann, J.B.; Callahan, C.A.; Magee, M. Empathy in medical students as related to academic performance, clinical competence and gender. Med. Educ. 2002, 36, 522–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kret, M.E.; De Gelder, B. A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 1211–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chen, W.; Lu, J.; Liu, L.; Lin, W. Gender Differences of Empathy. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 22, 1423–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liang, Y.S.; Yang, H.X.; Ma, Y.T.; Lui, S.S.; Cheung, E.F.; Wang, Y.; Chan, R.C. Validation and extension of the Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy in the Chinese setting. PsyCh J. 2019, 8, 439–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Qiuqi, Z. Empathy and implicit theories of emotions and personality in a Chinese sample. Mosc. Univ. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 1, 114–143, (In Russian, Abstr. In English). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asparouhov, T.; Muthén, B. Using Mplus TECH11 and TECH14 to test the number of latent classes. Mplus Web Notes 2012, 14, 22. [Google Scholar]
- Li, D.; Xu, H.; Kang, M.; Ma, S. Empathy in Chinese eight-year medical program students: Differences by school year, educational stage, and future career preference. BMC Med. Educ. 2018, 18, 241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nisbett, R.E.; Peng, K.; Choi, I.; Norenzayan, A. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 291–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Eisenberg, N.; Lennon, R. Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychol. Bull. 1983, 94, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Russians in Russia | Chinese in Russia | Chinese in China | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | X2 | p |
1. Implicit Theory of Emotions | 3.60 | 0.84 | 3.10 | 0.41 | 3.43 | 0.72 | 117.20 | 0 |
2. Perspective Taking | 28.76 | 4.20 | 27.05 | 4.16 | 28.63 | 4.77 | 47.69 | 0 |
3. Online Simulation | 22.94 | 3.27 | 23.84 | 3.43 | 27.36 | 3.70 | 298.24 | 0 |
4. Peripheral Responsiveness | 9.06 | 1.91 | 10.06 | 1.55 | 10.26 | 2.21 | 77.70 | 0 |
5. Cognitive Empathy | 51.69 | 6.23 | 50.89 | 6.81 | 55.99 | 7.59 | 129.32 | 0 |
6. Affective Empathy | 30.66 | 4.65 | 30.40 | 3.71 | 32.86 | 4.88 | 76.48 | 0 |
7. Age | 25.16 | 7.02 | 21.32 | 5.68 | 24.78 | 6.97 | 132.65 | 0 |
Model | AIC | BIC | aBIC | Entropy | aLRT P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Russian group, class(1) | 2705.82 | 2725.78 | 2709.91 | / | / |
Russian group, class(2) | 2574.12 | 2618.03 | 2583.12 | 0.59 | 0 |
Russian group, class(3) | 2571.60 | 2639.46 | 2585.52 | 0.66 | 0.34 |
Russian group, class(4) | 2575.07 | 2666.88 | 2593.90 | 0.83 | 0.13 |
Chinese in Russia, class(1) | 1993.78 | 2009.36 | 1996.67 | / | / |
Chinese in Russia, class(2) | 1868.29 | 1903.34 | 1874.79 | 0.60 | 0 |
Chinese in Russia, class(3) | 1871.13 | 1925.65 | 1881.23 | 0.60 | 0.14 |
Chinese in Russia, class(4) | 1878.28 | 1952.27 | 1891.99 | 0.75 | 0.37 |
Chinese in China, class(1) | 2326.75 | 2342.92 | 2330.23 | / | / |
Chinese in China, class(2) | 2242.10 | 2278.49 | 2249.93 | 0.60 | 0.0000 |
Chinese in China, class(3) | 2246.75 | 2303.35 | 2258.92 | 0.65 | 0.19 |
Chinese in China, class(4) | 2254.18 | 2330.99 | 2270.70 | 0.59 | 0.45 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kornilova, T.; Zhou, Q. Cross-Cultural Comparison of Relationships between Empathy and Implicit Theories of Emotions (in Chinese and Russians). Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 137. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11100137
Kornilova T, Zhou Q. Cross-Cultural Comparison of Relationships between Empathy and Implicit Theories of Emotions (in Chinese and Russians). Behavioral Sciences. 2021; 11(10):137. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11100137
Chicago/Turabian StyleKornilova, Tatiana, and Qiuqi Zhou. 2021. "Cross-Cultural Comparison of Relationships between Empathy and Implicit Theories of Emotions (in Chinese and Russians)" Behavioral Sciences 11, no. 10: 137. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11100137
APA StyleKornilova, T., & Zhou, Q. (2021). Cross-Cultural Comparison of Relationships between Empathy and Implicit Theories of Emotions (in Chinese and Russians). Behavioral Sciences, 11(10), 137. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11100137