Next Article in Journal
The Response of Layer Hen Productivity and Egg Quality to an Additional Limestone Source When Offered Diets Differing in Calcium Concentrations and the Inclusion of Phytase
Next Article in Special Issue
Social and Seasonal Factors Contribute to Shifts in Male African Elephant (Loxodonta africana) Foraging and Activity Patterns in Kruger National Park, South Africa
Previous Article in Journal
Determination of the Minimum Infusion Rate of Alfaxalone Combined with Electroacupuncture in Goats
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals

1
Fakultät für Mathematik und Naturwissenschaften, Zoologie und Didaktik der Biologie, University of Wuppertal, Gaußstraße 20, D-42119 Wuppertal, Germany
2
Endocrinology Laboratory, German Primate Centre, Kellnerweg 4, D-37077 Goettingen, Germany
3
Zoological Garden Halle (Saale), Fasanstr. 5a, D-06114 Halle (Saale), Germany
4
Opel-Zoo Kronberg, Am Opel-Zoo 3, D-61476 Kronberg im Taunus, Germany
5
Serengeti-Park Hodenhagen, Am Safaripark 1, D-29693 Hodenhagen, Germany
6
ZOO Dvůr Králové, Štefánikova 1029, C-544 01 Dvůr Králové nad Labem, Czech Republic
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(10), 2990; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102990
Submission received: 31 August 2021 / Revised: 13 October 2021 / Accepted: 14 October 2021 / Published: 18 October 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Elephant Communication)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

African elephants are highly social animals that perform a so-called Greeting Ceremony in the wild when meeting elephants they are familiar with but have not seen for a certain timespan. Until now, it has not been known whether zoo elephants also show this unique behaviour. Therefore, this study was designed around the reunifications of two mother–daughter pairs that had been separated for 2 and 12 years, and two unifications of unrelated elephants, as a comparison. First contact was conducted in a protected setting, i.e., there was a fence between the animals to prevent possible fighting. Signs of the Greeting Ceremony shown by the elephants, the distance they kept to the separating fence, and the time until the elephants’ trunks touched for the first time were observed. The results demonstrate that the related elephants showed all behavioural characteristic of the Greeting Ceremony, kept close to the fence, and touched trunks after only a few seconds, while elephants that were not familiar with each other did not show a full Greeting Ceremony, stayed further from the fence, and touched trunks for the first time only after several minutes upon meeting. This study testifies that zoo elephants show the same typical social behaviour known from wild elephants (namely the Greeting Ceremony) and, therefore, behave species-specific. It also confirms the strong family bonds of elephants and the cognitive abilities of elephants, specifically their long-term social memory.

Abstract

The introduction of elephants into new groups is necessary for breeding programmes. However, behavioural studies on the reactions of these animals at first encounters are missing. In the present study, female African elephants (Loxodonta africana) living in zoos were observed during unifications with unfamiliar elephants (introduction of two to one females and one to two females; n = 6) and reunifications with related elephants (two mother–daughter-pairs; n = 4) that were separated for 2 and 12 years, respectively. First encounters of the elephants were observed and recorded by scan sampling. The parameters measured were (a) signs of the characteristic Greeting Ceremony, (b) distance to the fence separating the elephants during first contact, and (c) time until trunks touched for the first time. The data were statistically analysed with SPSS. The results showed that related elephants performed a full Greeting Ceremony on reunifications. Unrelated elephants only expressed a minor greeting. During first encounters, related elephants predominantly showed affiliative behaviour (p = 0.001), whilst unrelated elephants expressed more agonistic behaviour (p = 0.001). The distance to the fence was significantly smaller for related elephants than for unrelated elephants (p = 0.038). first contact of trunks occurred on average after 3.00 s. in related elephants and 1026.25 s. in unrelated elephants. These findings indicate that related elephants recognise their kin after up to 12 years of separation, meet them with a full Greeting Ceremony during reunification, and seek contact to the related elephant, while unrelated elephants are hesitant during unifications with unfamiliar elephants and express more agonistic behaviour. The results testify that zoo elephants show the same species-specific social behaviour as their conspecifics in the wild. It also confirms the cognitive abilities of elephants and the significance of matrilines for breeding programmes.

1. Introduction

1.1. Elephant Communication

1.1.1. Greeting Ceremony

Known to be highly sensitive mammals with a complex social structure and extraordinarily developed ways of communication, elephants and their behaviour have been a frequent topic of research [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. However, it is mainly olfactory [8,9,10,11,12] and auditory [4,13,14,15,16,17,18,19] communication that has been investigated [7]. While sexual and breeding behaviour and communication are well-represented [20,21,22,23,24,25,26], the so-called Greeting Ceremony [7] with its enormous olfactory, visual, tactile, and acoustic aspects is investigated poorly for ex situ living African elephants, so far.
While elephants usually greet other elephants by flapping their ears, lifting the head, and sometimes touching the head of the other individual with their trunk (referred to as Little Greeting) [27], the Greeting Ceremony is much more complex and usually restricted to interactions between closely related elephants [7]. The ethogram in Table 1 shows the behavioural items that form the Greeting Ceremony [7,18,28,29,30,31].

1.1.2. Affiliative and Agonistic Communication

Communication expressed by behaviours during greetings can be further classified as affiliative, agonistic, and neutral [7,18,28,29,30,31]. The neutral behavioural eating/drinking is listed under (re)unification, as it is used as an indicator for stress in the animals. Since stress induces a rise in cortisol, it operates anorexiant [32,33,34,35,36,37]. Thus, only animals that are more relaxed during (re)unification are expected to show this behaviour. Table 2 lists all behaviours included in this study.

1.2. Elephant Transfers

1.2.1. Unifications

The management of the African elephant population in European zoos has to maintain a defined birth rate to ensure the viability of the population and its biodiversity [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45]. Thus, elephant transfers to bring animals in potential breeding situations are common. This applies mostly for males, but when space becomes limited, sometimes females need to be transferred as well [44,45]. Hence, elephants have to be acquainted with new housing conditions; new surroundings; and most importantly, new herd members. Those unifications of unrelated elephants are very difficult situations when handling elephants [38,44]. Maintaining such a situation with the right caution is essential for the successful joining of different elephant groups. Knowing how elephants behave on such occasions is highly beneficial to prevent possible aggressive behaviour or a failure in merging the two groups.

1.2.2. Reunifications

Nowadays, European zoos seek to keep elephants in herd structures similar to the way elephants live in the wild [38,40,45], with cows living with their female offspring in multigenerational herds [37,38]. In the past, however, occasional separation of mothers and daughters took place in European zoos [42]. Given the information from the wild, a reunification of related individuals might provide different results in comparison with unification of unrelated animals, with possibly different behaviour in the elephants involved. Scientific understanding of the underlying factors during (re)unification are important for the preservation of the species-specific social structure and the well-being of African elephants in modern zoos.

1.3. Aims of the Study

The so-called Greeting Ceremony is an indicator for elephants’ recognition of and a friendly attitude towards each other [7,31]. Whilst frequently described for wild-ranging elephants [28,29], to the best of our knowledge, there is no empirical data on the Greeting Ceremony for zoo-living elephants. This study aims to investigate the behaviour of related and unrelated African elephants at first encounters during (re)unification and the possible expressing of the characteristic Greeting Ceremony in a zoo environment. Confirming that zoo-socialised elephants express the same social behaviour and make use of the same ways of communication as in situ living individuals is of particular importance, as the zoos and studbooks aim to ensure a species-specific development of the zoo-bred African elephants [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45].
It can be expected that elephants that were separated for a certain timespan will make use of the Greeting Ceremony on reunification, while unfamiliar elephants will not show signs of a Greeting Ceremony when unified [7]. Hypothesising that related elephants will easily be adjoined and show intense emotional behaviour on reunification, it would give evidence of the long-term memory of this species. Recognition of a related animal after a longer period of separation, using the Greeting Ceremony, would attest to this particular ability in African elephants.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Animals

In the framework of the European Endangered Breeding Programme (EEP) for the African elephant, recommendations were made to transfer a daughter (Panya) to her mother (Bibi) and a mother (Pori) back to her daughter (Tana). It was also recommended to transport two unrelated cows (Lilak and Kariba) to another place with another single elephant (Zimba) and one unrelated elephant (Drumbo) to two unrelated cows (Saly and Umbu).
Even though most of the elephants were born in the wild, they were transferred to European zoos at a young age and socialised under zoo conditions.
For more detail on the elephants, see Table 3.
All unifications and reunifications took place under the same (testing) conditions. The sample size for related elephants was n = 4, and the sample size for unrelated elephants was n = 6. The sample size for reunifications of the related and unrelated elephant groups waws n = 2.
During the unification of Zimba with Lilak and Kariba, Zimba was in her stable when Lilak and Kariba were released separately into the enclosure next to hers; therefore, two data sets are presented (Zimba&Lilak and Zimba&Kariba). During the unification of Drumbo with Umbu and Saly, Drumbo was in the stable and Umbu and Saly entered the enclosure next to hers together, resulting in one data set. During the reunifications, the daughters (Tana and Panya) were in their enclosures and their mothers (Pori and Bibi) entered the adjacent enclosure.

2.2. Ethological Data Collection

All behaviours of the elephants on first protected meeting through a fence were documented utilising the ethogram (Table 1) according to scan sampling by the same human observer [46,47,48,49,50,51], focusing on signs of behaviour characteristic for the Greeting Ceremony [7,18,28,29,30,31,52,53]. Acoustic signals (trumpets, rumbles, and roars) were noted and specified when heard. Additionally, the ethogram differentiated between signs of agitation related to excitement (affiliative connotation) and signs of agitation related to fear (agonistic connotation) [7,27,45,46]. Procedures were observed while elephants were still separated through a fence, though in tactile, visual, auditory, and olfactory contact, as first meetings during the introduction of new herd members were performed with a barrier for safety reasons. Even though observation times ranged between 35 to 78 min, most behaviours occurred in the first 30 min. Therefore, only the first 30 min were used for analyses.
The distance that the elephants kept to the fence throughout the (re)unification was measured in meters to assess their willingness to touch the other individual [47]. The distance was based on direct contact (meaning tactile contact to the fence or animal) or distance of <1 m, 1–2 m, 3–4 m, and >4 m. The elephants’ distance to the fence was recorded every 10 s during the (re)unification.
For all elephants, the first moment of tactile contact during (re)unification was determined and is referred to as first contact of trunks throughout this paper. This indicator was used to describe the willingness of the elephants to reach for and touch the other elephant and for their curiosity [47].
The sets of data for behaviour and distance to the fence were classified numerically [54,55]. Statistical analysis for all data was performed using SPSS 27, and whether there were significances in the differences in the data sets between elephants on reunifications and unifications was calculated. Utilising the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, it was determined whether the data distribution was normal, followed by intercorrelation calculations (Spearman’s ρ) of the subscales [56,57]. As the data of both the behaviour analysis and the distance analysis showed no even distribution of significance (p ≤ 0.05) [58,59], the data sets were not normal in distribution and the Mann–Whitney U Test was used to determine the significant differences (p ≤ 0.05) [55,56,57,58] between (re)unifications.
For the analysis of the signs shown in the Greeting Ceremony, a Chi-Square Test was performed and the Fisher’s Exact Test was used to detect the significance, as the data sets partially had less than size items and the effect size was calculated utilising the Monte Carlo Simulation (x2) [60,61].
The distribution for the data set of affiliative and agonistic behaviours was normal, and a t-test and the Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances was calculated to determine the significance in the differences between related and unrelated elephants during (re)unifications [51,62,63,64].
As the data for the measurement of first trunk contact during (re)unifications were distributed evenly according to the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, an unpaired t-test and the Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances were calculated to show the significant differences between the two sample groups [51,62,63,64,65].
The distribution differed between both groups for the shown distinct behaviours during the (re)unifications, (Kolmogorov–Smirnov p < 0.05); therefore, the Mann–Whitney U Test was used to determine if there were significant differences in greeting behaviour [61,62,63,64].
The distribution between both groups for the data set distance to fence differed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov p < 0.05); thus, the Mann–Whitney U Test was calculated again, to determine if there were differences in the distance that the elephants kept from the fence between related and unrelated elephants [55,56,57,58].
The effect size was calculated with Pearson’s correlation coefficient: r = z n [57,59]. For all tests, the significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05 [65].

3. Results

3.1. Signs of Greeting Ceremony and General Behaviour during (Re)Unifications

Based on the behavioural components of the Greeting Ceremony, listed in Table 1, a first analysis was performed to determine if elephants expressed the typical signs of the Greeting Ceremony during (re)unifications. Table 4 summarises the results and shows that all elephants that were reunited showed every behavioural item of the Greeting Ceremony. The behavioural items of the Greeting Ceremony that were also shown by all of the elephants on unifications were raising head and that with minor exceptions of one to two elephants were touching trunk, lifting tail, and glandular secretion. Only one elephant on unification emitted acoustic signals and the behavioural items running towards elephant, clicking tusks, entwine trunks together, opening mouth, touching head, spinning around, and defecating/urinating were not shown by elephants on unifications.
The statistical analysis of the data shows a significant difference for the behavioural items running towards each other, clicking of tusks, entwining trunks together, opening mouth, touching head, spinning round, acoustic signals, and defecating and urinating. There was no significant difference for the items touching trunk, folding, lifting, spreading, flapping ears, raising head (is a constant), lifting tail, and glandular secretion.
The results for affiliative and agonistic behaviours based on the ethogram in Table 2 show that elephants on reunification showed ~79.52% of the affiliative and ~19.65% of the agonistic behaviours while ~0.82% was neutral behaviour, and that unrelated elephants showed ~12.5% of the affiliative, ~85.08% of the agonistic, and ~2.41% of the neutral behaviours during unification (Figure 1).
Levene’s Test shows no statistical significance for the category affiliative behaviour (0.568); therefore, equal variance is given. The t-test shows that the mean time of affiliative behaviour was more than 50% higher for related elephants (95%-CI [33.30641, 66.87859]) than for unrelated elephants. There was a statistically significant difference between the time that the two groups expressed affiliative behaviour: t(9) = 6.751, p = 0.001, d = 4.231. For the category agonistic behaviour, the variance is unequal. The t-test shows that the mean time of agonistic behaviour was more than 60% lower for related elephants (95%-CI [−82.62850, −37.75650]). There was a statistically significant difference of t(9) = −6.370, p = 0.001, d = −3.026 (Table 5).

3.2. Distance to Fence during (Re)Unification

The percentage of time that the elephants spent at a certain distance to the fence at first encounter with the (un)related elephant/s is presented in Figure 2. Elephants reuniting spend ~28.31% of time in direct contact, while elephants uniting for the first time spend ~10.23% of time in direct contact. For the category <1 m, the percentages were ~23.19% (related elephants) and ~7.93% (unrelated elephants); for 1–2 m, they were ~30.12% (related) and ~15.17% (unrelated); for 3–4 m, they were ~13.05% (related) and ~33.18% (unrelated); and for >4 m, they were ~5.32% (related) and ~33.49% (unrelated).
There was a statistically significant difference in the distance to the fence in the categories direct and 1–2 m but not in the categories <1 m, 3–4 m, and >4 m (Table 6).

3.3. First Contact of Trunks

The time until first contact of trunks is shown in Table 7. Related elephants demonstrated instant contact of trunks, whilst the time until trunk contact in unrelated elephants ranged from ~100 s to more than 900 s. The elephants Umbu and Drumbo did not touch trunks during unification. Therefore, a value is not shown for this pair.
Table 8 shows the statistical differences between the two test groups for first contact of trunks. The Levene’s Test yields no statistical significance (0.165); therefore, equal variances are given. The t-test shows that the mean time until first contact of trunks was −1023.25 s (95%-CI [−3456.35, 1409.85]) lower for the related elephants than for the unrelated elephants. The difference between time until first contact of trunks for related and unrelated elephants during (re)unifications was statistically significant, t(10) = −2.453, p = 0.034.

4. Discussion

4.1. Signs of Greeting Ceremony and General Behaviour during (Re)Unifications

Free-ranging elephants live in a complex fission–fusion society, and separations and unifications are common events [28,47]. Zoo elephants, in contrast, live in stable groups, and re-unifications of related animals are very rare. We used the opportunity to monitor the exceptional situations of the reunification of two mother–daughter pairs and compared them to the unifications of six unrelated females. The results presented here are the first to describe and analyse the occurrence of behaviours displayed in both situations at first encounters in zoo elephants. We found differences in the Greeting Ceremony expressed for elephants united and reunited. While all elephants on reunification expressed all behavioural items described for the Greeting Ceremony [7,18,28,29,30,31], elephants on unifications only showed some of those behavioural items and, therefore, not a full Greeting Ceremony [27]. This testifies that, even in a zoo environment, the whole ceremony is only displayed if elephants know each other. This study also attests that related elephants living ex situ express the same characteristic Greeting Ceremony, as African elephants living in situ. This provides signs for their species-specific evolvement and preservation of species-specific behaviour. As shown in Table 3, elephants of the study were either zoo-born or transferred to zoos at an early age of just two years. This implies that they were still too young to learn all of the behaviour of the Greeting Ceremony in the wild and that the shown behaviour must be genetically determined in the species. The study also confirms that African elephants living in zoos recognise family members after up to 12 years of separation [7]. This provides further evidence for the long-term memory reported also for free-ranging animals [66]. The study reveals that ex situ living elephants generally showed certain greeting behaviours, even when they were unrelated, and therefore certifies the highly social behaviour in African elephants living in zoos, which is also known for the species in situ [7,67,68,69,70,71]. The study also investigated the affiliative and agonistic behaviours shown by the elephants during (re)unifications. The results clearly prove that there is a statistically significant difference for the categories affiliative behaviour and agonistic behaviour, with related elephants expressing ~50.00% more affiliative and ~60% less agonistic behaviour during reunifications than unrelated elephants. Elephants encountered familiar animals friendly and forward going (~79.52% affiliative behaviour), while elephants on unifications were hesitant and showed predominantly agonistic behaviour (~85.08%) (see Figure 1). This confirms the significance of family bonds and the general understanding of the intense social relationships of elephants [7,45,67,68,69,70,71] and their hesitation when confronted with unfamiliar individuals, which is also known from the wild [7,47]. Elephants living in situ rely on family members when raising calves, protecting the herd, and searching for food and water [1,2,3,5,47]. The results of the study indicate that behaviour that is connected to a close family bond, such as the Greeting Ceremony, is generically anchored in elephants and preserved in zoo-socialised elephants. It was also observed that elephants on reunifications spend more time on the neutral behaviour eating/drinking than elephants on unifications. It can be assumed that elephants on reunifications were relaxed enough to spend time eating and drinking, as the situation did not cause them an exceedingly high amount of stress [32,33,34,35,36,37], whereas elephants being united with unfamiliar elephants did not calm down enough to eat and drink, a behaviour they display typically most of the time [32,33,34,35,36,37].

4.2. Distance to Fence during (Re)Unification

The analysis of the distance that the elephants kept from the fence (and therefore to the closest point of contact they could reach during (re)unification) shows that elephants being reunited lingered closer to the fence than elephants that were united. Related elephants spent most of the time during reunification at a distance under two meters from the fence, while unrelated elephants stood most of the time at a distance of three meters or more, maintaining a wider distance (see Figure 2). This shows that elephants on unifications were reluctant to approach during the unifications and did not want to get close to the unfamiliar elephant. Unknown individuals can always be a threat and elephants avoid living with individuals they are not related to [47]. Their reluctance to meet unknown elephants must therefore be considered species-specific. Equally, approaching familiar and related elephants on an encounter and especially during the Greeting Ceremony is species-specific for African elephants [7,18,27,28,29,30,31]. These data give further evidence for species-specific behaviour present in ex situ living African elephants and the preservation of strong family bonds. Even after several years of separation, they seek close contact with their relatives.

4.3. First Contact of Trunks

The results of the time until first contact of trunks during (re)unifications also show a major difference between related and unrelated elephants (see Table 7). The time until first contact of trunks for related elephants is only 3 s on average; for unrelated elephants, in contrast, it is 102,625 s, being on average 342 times higher. Of the four pairs that were observed during unification, one group did not touch trunks at all during the entire first encounter. However, the range for the time until first contact of trunks during unifications is wide in unrelated elephants. Some elephants seemed to be less hesitant to touch the unfamiliar elephants than others (Saly and Drumbo, 107 s; Zimba and Lilak, 336 s). An individual distinctive disposition can be assumed, which might originate from some elephants being more curious than others, having a different social status, being of different age (and therefore less or more experienced), or having made certain previous experiences. Generally, unrelated elephants are described to be reluctant to touch the unfamiliar elephant on first encounter, while related elephants immediately seek contact with the familiar individual [1,7,47]. This observation additionally attests to the strong bonds between mother–daughter groups, which this study also found in African elephants in zoos even after a long period of separation from each other. It also confirms that related elephants on reunifications immediately approach, reach out for, and seek tactile contact with the other animal. As the olfactory and auditory senses in elephants are highly developed [9,18], these results indicate that the individuals recognised the other animal before the moment of first direct contact and wanted to engage in tactile contact with the other individual as soon as possible. Unrelated elephants, on the other hand, are aware that they are not familiar with the other individual and therefore hesitate to engage in tactile contact.

5. Conclusions

Even though the number of animals in the present study is small, the data presented here give further evidence of the strong bonds between mother–daughter groups. They also testify that elephants recognise each other after long-term separation by showing a full Greeting Ceremony, even after living apart for up to 12 years and therefore feature a species-specific behaviour even under zoo conditions, comparable with that shown in the wild. This provides evidence of recognition of their kin for the exceptional memory of this species. Keeping mothers and daughters together to build up matrilines can be considered as an important goal in the care of elephants living in European zoos [43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,72,73].
The strong reactions expressed by mother and daughter elephants during reunifications and the empirical data of this study, demonstrating their urge to seek contact with the related animal, testify that zoo elephants, whether wild-caught or zoo-born, still belong to those species-specific mother–daughter groups. This verifies the hypothesis that elephant cows and their female offspring are better held together and that separations should be avoided in the future, where possible, to facilitate better living conditions for the animals.
Even though unifications of unrelated female elephants are a part of the European breeding programme for African elephants, elephant transfers are not frequent events and behavioural data were missing so far. Additionally, chances to observe reunifications of family members are extremely rare. Therefore, caution must be taken when interpreting ethological data, as sample size and statistical power are limited in this study [74,75]. Our preliminary findings support the need for further research.

Author Contributions

F.H.: conceptualisation, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, visualisation, and writing—original draft; A.-K.O.: conceptualisation, project administration, supervision, validation, and writing—review and editing; D.W.H.M.: resources, and writing—review and editing; U.W.: resources, and writing—review and editing; I.A.-S.: resources, and writing—review and editing; J.H.: resources, and writing—review and editing; G.P.: resources, supervision, validation, and writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

We acknowledge support from the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Wuppertal.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Animal welfare was not affected by the collection of data at any point of the study, as the elephants were not affected by the study’s behavioural observations and transports were conducted according to the recommendations within the network of the European population management programme. The observations did not involve any direct contact with the animals or any changes in the zoo-given conditions during transport and (re)uniting. All handling of the animals during the observed process was conducted by the zoos with the greatest care and with a high focus on the animals’ welfare.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all persons involved in the study at the Tierpark Berlin, Bergzoo Halle, Serengeti Park Hodenhagen, Opel-Zoo Kronberg, and Safaripark Dvůr Králové for the opportunity to conduct this study. We especially thank the keepers of all elephant facilities for their help and cooperation during the whole project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Abbreviations

CIConfidence interval
dEffect size after Cohen
dfDegree of freedom
MMean
mMeter
N/nSample size
pSignificance
rEffect size after Pearson
SDStandard Deviation
secSeconds
tt-Statistic
UMann-Whitney U-Statistic
X2Effect size after Monte Carlo
ZZ-Statistic
%Percent

References

  1. Archie, E.A.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. The ties that bind: Genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 2005, 273, 513–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Charif, R.A.; Ramey, R.R.; Langbauer, W.R.; Payne, K.B.; Martin, R.B.; Brown, L.M. Spatial relationships and matrilineal kinship in African savanna elephant (Loxodonta africana) clans. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005, 57, 327–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Estes, R.D. The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivors, Primates; The University of California Press: Oxford, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  4. Langbauer, W.R. Elephant communication. Zoo Biol. Publ. Affil. Am. Zoo Aquar. Assoc. 2000, 19, 425–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. McComb, K.; Moss, C.; Durant, S.; Baker, L.; Sayialel, S. Matriarchs as Repositories of Social Knowledge in African Elephants. Science 2001, 292, 491–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E. Keeping an “ear” to the ground: Seismic communication in elephants. Physiology 2007, 22, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Poole, J.; Granli, P. Signals, Gestures, and Behavior of African Elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 109–124. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bates, L.A.; Sayialel, K.N.; Njiraini, N.W.; Poole, J.H.; Moss, C.J.; Byrne, R.W. African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biol. Lett. 2008, 4, 34–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Rasmussen, B. The biggest smeller. J. Elephant Manag. Assoc. 1995, 6, 58–60. [Google Scholar]
  10. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Hall-Martin, A.J.; Hess, D.L. Chemical profiles of male African elephants, Loxodonta africana: Physiological and ecological implications. J. Mammal. 1996, 77, 422–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Rasmussen, B.; Schulte, B. A medley of chemical signals. J. Elephant Manag. Assoc. 1996, 7, 61–64. [Google Scholar]
  12. Wheeler, J.W.; Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Ayorinde, F.; Buss, I.O.; Smuts, G.L. Constituents of temporal gland secretion of the African elephant, Loxodonta africana. J. Chem. Ecol. 1982, 8, 821–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Langbauer, W.R.; Payne, K.B.; Charif, R.A.; Thomas, E.M. Responses of captive African elephants to playback of low-frequency calls. Can. J. Zool. 1989, 67, 2604–2607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Langbauer, W.R.; Payne, K.B.; Charif, R.A.; Rapaport, L.; Osborn, F. African elephants respond to distant playbacks of low-frequency conspecific calls. J. Exp. Biol. 1991, 157, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Payne, K. Elephant talk. Natl. Geogr. 1989, 176, 264–277. [Google Scholar]
  16. Payne, K. Elephants Calling; Crown Publishers, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1992; p. 36. [Google Scholar]
  17. Poole, J. Elephant trunk calls. Swara 1988, 11, 28–31. [Google Scholar]
  18. Poole, J.; Payne, K.; Langbauer, W.R.; Moss, C. The social contexts of some very low frequency calls of African elephants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiologv 1988, 22, 385–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Rasmussen, L.E.; Schulte, B.A. Chemical signals in the reproduction of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 1998, 53, 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Andrews, J.; Mecklenborg, A.; Bercovitch, F. Milk Intake and Development in a Newborn Captive African Elephant (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 275–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Douglas-Hamilton, I. On the ecology and behaviour of the African elephant. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lee, P. Allomothering among African elephants. Univ. Camb. Anim. Behav. 1987, 35, 278–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Lee, P.; Moss, C. Early maternal investment in male and female elephant calves. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 1986, 18, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ortolani, A.; Leong, K.; Graham, L.; Savage, A. Behavioral Indices of Estrus in a Group of Captive African Elephants (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. Publ. Affil. Am. Zoo Aquar. Assoc. 2005, 24, 311–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Pinter-Wollman, N.; Isbell, L.A.; Hart, L.A. The relationship between social behaviour and habitat familiarity in African elephants (Loxodonta Africana). Proc. R. Soc. B 2009, 276, 1009–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Poole, J. Mate guarding, reproductive success and female choice in African elephants. Anim. Behav. 1989, 37, 842–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Poole, J. Behavioral Context of Elephant Acoustic Communication. In The Amboseli Elephnats: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 125–161. [Google Scholar]
  28. Moss, C. Social circles. Wildl. News 1981, 16, 2–7. [Google Scholar]
  29. Moss, C. Elephant Memories; William Morrow and Company, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1988; p. 336. [Google Scholar]
  30. Poole, J. Rutting behaviour in African elephants: The phenomenon of musth. Behaviour 1987, 102, 283–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Poole, J. Family Reunions. In The Smile of the Dolphin: Remarkable Accounts of Animal Emotions; Bekoff, M., Ed.; Discovery Books: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 22–23. [Google Scholar]
  32. Bernier, N.J. The corticotropin-releasing factor system as a mediator of the appetite-suppressing effects of stress in fish. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2006, 146, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Choi, C.Y.; Song, J.A.; Lee, T.H.; Park, Y.-S. Effect of green wavelength light on stress and appetite responses of olive flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus) following feed deprivation and re-feeding. Aquac. Rep. 2021, 19, 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  34. Crockett, C.M.; Shimoji, M.; Bowden, D.M. Behavior, appetite, and urinary cortisol responses by adult female pigtailed macaques to cage size, cage level, room change, and ketamine sedation. Am. J. Primatol. 2000, 52, 63–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gebregeziabhear, E. The Effect of Stress on Productivity of Animals: A review. J. Biol. Agric. Healthc. 2015, 15, 14–22. [Google Scholar]
  36. Janzen, W.J.; Duncan, C.A.; Riley, L.G. Cortisol treatment reduces ghrelin signaling and food intake in tilapia, Oreochromis mossambicus. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 2012, 3, 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lindsay, W.K. Habitat Use, Diet Choice, and Nutritional Status in Female and Male Amboseli Elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 51–73. [Google Scholar]
  38. Bossy, S. Elefantenhaltung vor dem Umbruch. Pinguinal. Mag. Des Zoo-Ver. Wupp. E.V. 2019, 24, 22–23. [Google Scholar]
  39. Conley, S. Die Internationale Elephant Foundation und der Grüne Zoo Wuppertal. Pinguinal 2019, 24, 25. [Google Scholar]
  40. EAZA. EAZA Position Statement of the Evolution of Elephant Management Systems at Member Zoos. 2019. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/assets/Up-loads/EAZA-Documents-Other/EAZA-Position-Statement-on-the-Evolution-of-Elephant-Management-Systems-at-Member-Zoos-FINAL.pdf (accessed on 13 July 2021).
  41. Thouless, C.R.; Dublin, H.T.; Blanc, J.J.; Skinner, D.P.; Daniel, T.E.; Taylor, R.D.; Maisels, F.; Frederick, H.L.; Bouché, P. African Elephant Status Report 2016. An Update from the African Database; IUCN: Gland, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  42. EAZA. Areas of Activity. 2020. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/about-us/areas-of-activity/ (accessed on 13 July 2021).
  43. Garaï, M.; Kurt, F. Sozialisation und das Wohlbefinden der Elefanten. Z. Des Kölner Zoo 2006, 49, 85–102. [Google Scholar]
  44. Schulte, B. Social Structure and Helping Behaviour in Captive Elephants. Zoo Biol. 2000, 5, 447–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Kowalski, N.; Dale, R.; Mazur, C. A survey of the management and development of captive African elephant (Loxodonta africana) calves: Birth to three month of age. Zoo Biol. 2010, 29, 104–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Kappeler, P. Verhaltensbiologie; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  47. Krull, H.P. Beobachtungs- und Protokollmethoden für Verhaltensbeobachtungen; Zooschule Krefeld: Krefeld, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  48. Martin, P.; Bateson, P.P.G. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 3rd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  49. Naguib, M.; Krause, E.T. Methoden der Verhaltensbiologie; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  50. Randler, C. Verhaltensbiologie; UTB GmbH: Stuttgart, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kahl, M.P.; Armstrong, B.D. Visual and tactile displays in African elephants, Loxodonta africana: A progress report (1991–1997). Elephant 2000, 2, 19–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Williams, E.; Carter, A.; Hall, C.; Bremner-Harrison, S. Social Interactions in Zoo-Housed Elephants: Factors Affecting Social Relationships. Animals 2019, 9, 747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Moss, C. The demography of an African elephant (Loxodonta africana) population in Amboseli, Kenya. J. Zool. 2001, 255, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Agresti, A. An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis, 2nd ed.; Wiley series in probability and mathematical statistics; Wiley-Interscience: Hoboken, NJ, USA; Chichester, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  55. American Psychological Association. APA Manual 6th ed (Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 6th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  56. Dinneen, L.C.; Blakesley, B.C. Algorithm AS 62: A Generator for the Sampling Distribution of the Mann-Whitney U Statistic. Appl. Stat. 1973, 22, 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ruxton, G.D. The unequal variance t-test is an underused alternative to Student’s t-test and the Mann-Whitney U test. Behav. Ecol. 2006, 17, 688–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bortz, J.; Döring, N. Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation: Für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler; Springer Medizin Verlag: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  59. Siegel, S.; Castellan, N.J. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; McGraw-Hill: Boston, MS, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  60. Adery, C.A. A Simplified Monte Carlo Significance Test Procedure. J. R. Stat. Society. Ser. B Methodol. 1968, 30, 582–598. [Google Scholar]
  61. Mehta, C.R.; Patel, N.R. A Network Algorithm for Performing Fisher’s Exact Test in r × c Contingency Tables. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1983, 78, 427. [Google Scholar]
  62. Kubinger, K.D.; Rasch, D.; Moder, K. Zur Legende der Voraussetzungen des t -Tests für unabhängige Stichproben. Psychol. Rundsch. 2009, 60, 26–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lakens, D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Rasch, D.; Kubinger, K.D.; Moder, K. The two-sample t test: Pre-testing its assumptions does not pay off. Stat. Pap. 2011, 52, 219–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Fritz, C.O.; Morris, P.E.; Richler, J.J. Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Byrne, R.W.; Bates, L.A.; Moss, C.J. Elephant cognition in primate perspective. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev. 2009, 4, 65–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Douglas-Hamilton, O. Unter Elefanten: Abenteuerliche Forschungen in der Wildnis Zentralafrikas; Gustav Lübbe Verlag GmbH: Bergisch Gladbach, Germany, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lee, P.C.; Moss, C. Calf Development and Maternal Rearing Strategies. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 224–237. [Google Scholar]
  69. Moss, C.; Lee, P.C. Female Reproductive Strategies: Individual Life Histories. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 187–204. [Google Scholar]
  70. Mutinda, H.; Poole, J.H.; Moss, C. Decision Making and Leadership in Using the Ecosystem. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 246–259. [Google Scholar]
  71. Archie, E.A.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. Friends and Relations: Kinship and the Nature of Female Elephant Social Relationships. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 238–245. [Google Scholar]
  72. Kurt, F. Elefanten in Zirkus und Zoo. In Staatliches Museum für Völkerkunde in München (Ed.), Mensch und Elefant; Umschau-Verlag: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 1994; pp. 117–133. [Google Scholar]
  73. Kurt, F. Die Geschichte der Haltung von Elefanten in Menschenobhut. Z. Des Kölner Zoo 2006, 49, 59–81. [Google Scholar]
  74. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  75. Ryan, T.P. Sample Size Determination and Power; Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics; John Wiley & Sons Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Percentage of affiliative and agonistic behaviours of the total behaviour shown by related and unrelated elephants during (re)unifications.
Figure 1. Percentage of affiliative and agonistic behaviours of the total behaviour shown by related and unrelated elephants during (re)unifications.
Animals 11 02990 g001
Figure 2. Percentage of time that related and unrelated elephants stood at a certain distance to the fence during (re)unification.
Figure 2. Percentage of time that related and unrelated elephants stood at a certain distance to the fence during (re)unification.
Animals 11 02990 g002
Table 1. Behaviour expressed during a Greeting Ceremony.
Table 1. Behaviour expressed during a Greeting Ceremony.
ItemBehaviour
Running towards elephantElephants run towards the elephant they intend to greet.
Clicking tusks and entwining trunks togetherElephants click tusks and entwine their trunks by winding them around each other.
Touching trunkElephants touch the trunk of the other elephant with their trunk.
Folding, lifting, spreading, and flapping earsElephant’s ears are in motion by folding them back, lifting them, enfolding them, and flapping them rapidly.
Raising headElephants raise their heads as high as possible above their shoulders.
Opening mouthElephants open their mouth widely.
Touching headElephants touch the head of the other elephant at eyes, mouth, and temporal glands with their trunk.
Spinning roundElephants rapidly turn around repeatedly, also changing direction.
Lifting tailElephants lift their tail to stick it out.
Acoustic signalsElephants emit loud vocalisation as oral rumbles, roars, and trumpets.
Defecating and urinatingElephants drop faeces and micturate.
Glandular secretionElephants exude fluid from the temporal glands.
Table 2. Affiliative, agonistic, and neutral behaviours of greetings.
Table 2. Affiliative, agonistic, and neutral behaviours of greetings.
Affiliative
Running towards fence/animalElephants run towards the elephant they intend to greet or the fence separating them from the elephant.
Pushing against the fenceElephants press their head or body against the fence to touch the other elephant/
Touching trunksElephants touch the trunk of the other elephant with their trunk.
Affiliative agitation Elephants raise their head, shake the tail, click their tusks, and flap with their ears.
Acoustic signals Elephants emit rumbles (low-frequent calls).
Defecating/urinatingElephants drop faeces and micturate.
Agonistic
Agonistic agitation Elephants shake the head, stick out the tail, role in their trunk, and fold their ears close to their head.
Acoustic signal Elephants emit roars (high-frequent calls).
Pacing backwardsElephants quickly diverge from fence/other elephants.
Showing servilityElephants bow their head, lower their shoulders, furl the trunk, and jam their tail between their hind legs.
Showing dominanceElephants stand tall, with raised heads and spread ears; they lift their trunk over their heads; they place the trunk on the other elephants’ head; and they run towards other elephants with sudden speed.
Neutral
Eating/ drinking Elephants eat and/or drink.
Table 3. List of elephants.
Table 3. List of elephants.
ElephantSexOriginDate of BirthDate of Transfer from Wild to the ZooTransferred from toRelated to (Only Elephants Included in the Study Are Listed)(Re)united with
PanyaFZoo-born22 August 2007-Bergzoo Halle to Serengeti Park HodenhagenDaughter of BibiBibi
BibiFWild-born19851987-Mother of PanyaPanya
PoriFWild-born19811983Tierpark Berlin to Bergzoo HalleMother of TanaTana
TanaFZoo-born4 May 2001--Daughter of PoriPori
LilakFWild-born19711973Tierpark Berlin to Opel-Zoo Kronberg-Zimba
KaribaFZoo-born17 March 2006-Tierpark Berlin to Opel-Zoo Kronberg-Zimba
ZimbaFWild-born19821984--Kariba and Lilak
DrumboFWild-born19901992Zoo Vienna Schönbrunn to Safaripark Dvur-Saly, Umbu
SalyFWild-born19821984--Drumbo
UmbuFWild-born19811983--Drumbo
Table 4. Differences in expressed behaviour during (re)unifications.
Table 4. Differences in expressed behaviour during (re)unifications.
UnrelatedRelatedExact Sig. (2-Sided)
(Fisher’s Exact Test)
Effect Size (Monte Carlo Simulation)
BehaviourSalyUmbuDrumboZimbaKaribaLilakBibiPanyaTanaPoripX2
Running towards elephant------++++0.00311.00
Clicking tusks, entwine trunks together------++++0.00311.00
Touching trunk-+++++++++1.00.629
Folding, lifting, spreading, flapping ears+-++++++++1.00.629
Raising head++++++++++--
Opening mouth------++++0.00311.00
Touching head------++++0.00311.00
Spinning round------++++0.00311.00
Lifting tail+-++++++++1.00.629
Acoustic signals----+-++++0.0157.543
Defecating and urinating------++++0.00311.00
Glandular secretion--++++++++0.4911.397
Table 5. Significances for affiliative and agonistic behaviours for related and unrelated elephants on behaviour during (re)unifications.
Table 5. Significances for affiliative and agonistic behaviours for related and unrelated elephants on behaviour during (re)unifications.
Levene’s Test for Equality of Variancest-Test for Equality of MeansEffect Size
Sig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed) pMean DifferenceStd. Error Difference95% Confidence Interval of the Differenced
LowerUpper
affiliativeEqual variances assumed0.5686.75190.00050.092507.4203933.3064166.878594.231
Equal variances not assumed 7.0667.2710.00050.092507.0887633.4558966.72911
agonisticEqual variances assumed0.010−4.82790.001−60.1925012.46980−88.40116−31.98384−3.026
Equal variances not assumed −6.3706.8620.000−60.192509.44953−82.62850−37.75650
Table 6. Significances for distance to the fence between related and unrelated elephants during (re)unification.
Table 6. Significances for distance to the fence between related and unrelated elephants during (re)unification.
a
Direct<1 m1–2 m3–4 m>4 m
Mann–Whitney U3.0006.0003.00010.0005.000
Z−2.079−1.512−2.079−0.756−1.701
Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed) p0.0380.1310.0380.4500.089
Pearson’s correlation coefficient r−0.627−0.456−0.627−0.228−0.513
a. Group variable: related, 1; unrelated, 2.
Table 7. Seconds until first contact of trunks during (re)unifications for the different pairs that were (re)united.
Table 7. Seconds until first contact of trunks during (re)unifications for the different pairs that were (re)united.
SettingElephant PairTime until Contact (s)Average
UnificationSaly and Drumbo107450
Umbu and Drumbonot displayed
Zimba and Lilak936
Zimba and Kariba362
ReunificationBibi and Panya23
Pori and Tana4
Table 8. Significances for related and unrelated elephants on first contact of trunks during unification.
Table 8. Significances for related and unrelated elephants on first contact of trunks during unification.
Levene’s Test for Equality of Variancest-Test for Equality of Means
Sig.tdfSig. (2-tailed) pMean DifferenceStd. Error Difference95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
LowerUpper
First Contact of TrunksEqual variances assumed0.002−2.453100.034−723.250294.809−1380.126−66.374
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hörner, F.; Oerke, A.-K.; Müller, D.W.H.; Westerhüs, U.; Azogu-Sepe, I.; Hruby, J.; Preisfeld, G. Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals. Animals 2021, 11, 2990. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102990

AMA Style

Hörner F, Oerke A-K, Müller DWH, Westerhüs U, Azogu-Sepe I, Hruby J, Preisfeld G. Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals. Animals. 2021; 11(10):2990. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102990

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hörner, Franziska, Ann-Kathrin Oerke, Dennis W. H. Müller, Uta Westerhüs, Idu Azogu-Sepe, Jiri Hruby, and Gela Preisfeld. 2021. "Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals" Animals 11, no. 10: 2990. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102990

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop