Learning to Search. The Importance of Eye Movements in the Decrease of Response Times During a Visual Choice Reaction Time Task
Abstract
:Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusions
Acknowledgments
References
- Antonova, I., C. van Swam, D. Hubl, T. Dierks, I. Griskova-Bulanova, and T. Koenig. 2016. Reaction Time in a Visual 4-Choice Reaction Time Task: ERP Effects of Motor Preparation and Hemispheric Involvement. Brain Topography 29, 4: 491–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baayen, R. H., and P. Milin. 2015. Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research 3, 2: 12–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bahill, A. T., M. R. Clark, and L. Stark. 1975. The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements. Mathematical Biosciences 24, 3: 191–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balakrishnan, G., G. Uppinakudru, G. Girwar Singh, S. Bangera, A. Dutt Raghavendra, and D. Thangavel. 2014. A Comparative Study on Visual Choice Reaction Time for Different Colors in Females. Neurology Research International. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ballesteros, S., A. Prieto, J. Mayas, P. Toril, C. Pita, L. Ponce de León, and J. Waterworth. 2014. Brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergman, I., K. Johansson, O. Almkvist, and C. Lundberg. 2016. Health-adjusted neuropsychological test norms based on 463 older Swedish car drivers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 57, 2: 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernard, J.-B., C. Aguilar, and E. Castet. 2016. A New Font, Specifically Designed for Peripheral Vision, Improves Peripheral Letter and Word Recognition, but Not Eye-Mediated Reading Performance. PLoS ONE 11, 4: e0152506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, O., C. Weigelt, and J. J. Bloomberg. 2010. Cognitive Demand of Human Sensorimotor Performance During an Extended Space Mission: A Dual-Task Study. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine 81, 9: 819–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bunce, D., B. I. Haynes, S. R. Lord, Y. J. Gschwind, N. A. Kochan, S. Reppermund, and K. Delbaere. 2016. Intraindividual Stepping Reaction Time Variability Predicts Falls in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, glw164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ching, H.-H., M. Koo, T.-H. Tsai, and C.-Y. Chen. 2015. Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation Course on Learning and Cognitive Performance among University Students in Taiwan. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine: eCAM. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chung, S. T. L. 2002. The Effect of Letter Spacing on Reading Speed in Central and Peripheral Vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 43, 4: 1270–1276. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavior science. Lawrance Eribaum Association. [Google Scholar]
- Damos, D. L., and C. D. Wickens. 1977. Dual-task performance and the hick-hyman law of choice reaction time. Journal of Motor Behavior 9, 3: 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davidson, D. J., and A. E. Martin. 2013. Modeling accuracy as a function of response time with the generalized linear mixed effects model. Acta Psychologica 144, 1: 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Desapriya, E., R. Harjee, J. Brubacher, H. Chan, D. S. Hewapathirane, S. Subzwari, and I. Pike. 2014. Vision screening of older drivers for preventing road traffic injuries and fatalities. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (2): CD006252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Destrebecqz, A., P. Perruchet, A. Cleeremans, S. Laureys, P. Maquet, and P. Peigneux. 2010. The influence of temporal factors on automatic priming and conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63, 2: 291–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deubel, H. 2008. The time course of presaccadic attention shifts. Psychological Research 72, 6: 630–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duchowski, A. T. 2007. Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice, 2nd ed. London: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Dye, M. W. G., C. S. Green, and D. Bavelier. 2009. Increasing Speed of Processing With Action Video Games. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18, 6: 321–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Federighi, P., G. Cevenini, M. T. Dotti, F. Rosini, E. Pretegiani, A. Federico, and A. Rufa. 2011. Differences in saccade dynamics between spinocerebellar ataxia 2 and late-onset cerebellar ataxias. Brain 134, 3: 879–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, J. E., and B. Subramaniam. 1995. The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception & Psychophysics 57, 6: 787–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsieh, Y.-H., C. J. Lin, and H.-C. Chen. 2007. Effect of vibration on visual display terminal work performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills 105, 3 Pt 2: 1055–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Inami, A., R. Matsuda, T. Grobosch, H. Komamura, K. Takeda, Y. Yamada, and M. Tashiro. 2016. A simulated car-driving study on the effects of acute administration of levocetirizine, fexofenadine, and diphenhydramine in healthy Japanese volunteers. Human Psychopharmacology 31, 3: 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- JASP Team. 2016. JASP (Version 0.7.5.6). Available online: https://jasp-stats.org.
- Jones, S. A. H., B. C. Butler, F. Kintzel, A. Johnson, R. M. Klein, and G. A. Eskes. 2016. Measuring the Performance of Attention Networks with the Dalhousie Computerized Attention Battery (DalCAB): Methodology and Reliability in Healthy Adults. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knopman, D. 1991. Long-term retention of implicitly acquired learning in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 13, 6: 880–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kosinski, B., and J. Cummings. 1999. The scientific method: An introduction using reaction time. Tested Studies for Laboratory Teaching (ABLE Proceedings) 25: 219–234. [Google Scholar]
- Kuperman, V., D. Drieghe, E. Keuleers, and M. Brysbaert. 2013. How strongly do word reading times and lexical decision times correlate? Combining data from eye movement corpora and megastudies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006) 66, 3: 563–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwon, Y. H., and J. W. Kwon. 2013. Is transcranial direct current stimulation a potential method for improving response inhibition? Neural Regeneration Research 8, 11: 1048–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Land, W. M. 2016. Action Effects and Task Knowledge: The Influence of Anticipatory Priming on the Identification of Task-Related Stimuli in Experts. PLoS ONE 11, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lieberman, H. R., J. P. Karl, P. J. Niro, K. W. Williams, E. K. Farina, S. J. Cable, and J. P. McClung. 2014. Positive Effects of Basic Training on Cognitive Performance and Mood of Adult Females. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 56, 6: 1113–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, R., and M. Kwon. 2016. Integrating oculomotor and perceptual training to induce a pseudofovea: A model system for studying central vision loss. Journal of Vision 16, 6: 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luce, R. D. 1991. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Meermeier, A., S. Gremmler, and M. Lappe. 2016. The influence of image content on oculomotor plasticity. Journal of Vision 16, 8: 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Munuera, J., P. Morel, J.-R. Duhamel, and S. Deneve. 2009. Optimal Sensorimotor Control in Eye Movement Sequences. The Journal of Neuroscience 29, 10: 3026–3035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murtha, S., R. Cismaru, R. Waechter, and H. Chertkow. 2002. Increased variability accompanies frontal lobe damage in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8, 3: 360–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parkhurst, D., K. Law, and E. Niebur. 2002. Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vision Research 42, 1: 107–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirce, J. W. 2009. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 2: 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perea, M., A. Marcet, B. Uixera, and M. Vergara-Martínez. 2016. Eye movements when reading sentences with handwritten words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ribeiro, M. J., J. S. Paiva, and M. Castelo-Branco. 2016. Spontaneous Fluctuations in Sensory Processing Predict Within-Subject Reaction Time Variability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosnow, R. L., and R. Rosenthal. 1989. Statistical procedures and the justification of knowledge in psychological science. American Psychologist 44, 10: 1276–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salvia, E., C. Petit, S. Champely, R. Chomette, F. Di Rienzo, and C. Collet. 2016. Effects of Age and Task Load on Drivers’ Response Accuracy and Reaction Time When Responding to Traffic Lights. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shiraishi, E., K. Suzuki, A. Harada, N. Suzuki, and H. Kimura. 2016. The Phosphodiesterase 10A Selective Inhibitor TAK-063 Improves Cognitive Functions Associated with Schizophrenia in Rodent Models. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 356, 3: 587–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Q., J. (Cecilia) Xia, T. Falkmer, and H. Lee. 2016. Investigating the Spatial Pattern of Older Drivers’ Eye Fixation Behaviour and Associations with Their Visual Capacity. Journal of Eye Movement Research 9, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Townsend, J. T., and F. G. Ashby. 1983. Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes. CUP Archive. [Google Scholar]
- Truszczynski, O., R. Lewkowicz, M. Wojtkowiak, and M. P. Biernacki. 2014. Reaction time in pilots during intervals of high sustained g. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine 85, 11: 1114–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tuch, A. N., J. A. Bargas-Avila, K. Opwis, and F. H. Wilhelm. 2009. Visual complexity of websites: Effects on users’ experience, physiology, performance, and memory. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 67, 9: 703–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tye, C., K. A. Johnson, S. P. Kelly, P. Asherson, J. Kuntsi, K. L. Ashwood, and G. McLoughlin. 2016. Response time variability under slow and fastincentive conditions in children with ASD, ADHD and ASD+ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watanabe, T., K. Ishida, S. Tanabe, and I. Nojima. 2016. Preparatory state and postural adjustment strategies for choice reaction step initiation. Neuroscience 332: 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weigard, A., C. Huang-Pollock, and S. Brown. 2016. Evaluating the consequences of impaired monitoring of learned behavior in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using a Bayesian hierarchical model of choice response time. Neuropsychology 30, 4: 502–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wifall, T., E. Hazeltine, and J. T. Mordkoff. 2015. The roles of stimulus and response uncertainty in forcedchoice performance: An amendment to Hick/Hyman Law. Psychological Research 80, 4: 555–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woods, D. L., J. M. Wyma, E. W. Yund, T. J. Herron, and B. Reed. 2015. Factors influencing the latency of simple reaction time. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodward, N. D., B. Duffy, and H. Karbasforoushan. 2014. Response selection impairment in schizophrenia transcends sensory and motor modalities. Schizophrenia Research 152: 446–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yabe, Y., and M. A. Goodale. 2015. Time Flies When We Intend to Act: Temporal Distortion in a Go/NoGo Task. The Journal of Neuroscience 35, 12: 5023–5029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2016 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kroll, A.; Mak, M.; Samochowiec, J. Learning to Search. The Importance of Eye Movements in the Decrease of Response Times During a Visual Choice Reaction Time Task. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2016, 9, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.9.5.5
Kroll A, Mak M, Samochowiec J. Learning to Search. The Importance of Eye Movements in the Decrease of Response Times During a Visual Choice Reaction Time Task. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2016; 9(5):1-11. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.9.5.5
Chicago/Turabian StyleKroll, Aleksandra, Monika Mak, and Jerzy Samochowiec. 2016. "Learning to Search. The Importance of Eye Movements in the Decrease of Response Times During a Visual Choice Reaction Time Task" Journal of Eye Movement Research 9, no. 5: 1-11. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.9.5.5
APA StyleKroll, A., Mak, M., & Samochowiec, J. (2016). Learning to Search. The Importance of Eye Movements in the Decrease of Response Times During a Visual Choice Reaction Time Task. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 9(5), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.9.5.5