Next Article in Journal
A Statistical Mixture Method to Reveal Bottom-Up and Top-Down Factors Guiding the Eye-Movements
Previous Article in Journal
Time Course and Hazard Function: A Distributional Analysis of Fixation Duration in Reading
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Behavioural and Neurophysiological Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkeys

by
Donald C. Brien
1,
Brian D. Corneil
2,
Jillian H. Fecteau
1,
Andrew H. Bell
1 and
Douglas P. Munoz
1
1
Queen’s University, Kingston, ON K7L 3N6, Canada
2
University of Western Ontario, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2009, 3(2), 1-12; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.2.4
Published: 22 December 2009

Abstract

:
Systematic modulations of microsaccades have been observed in humans during covert orienting. We show here that monkeys are a suitable model for studying the neurophysiology governing these modulations of microsaccades. Using various cue-target saccade tasks, we observed the effects of visual and auditory cues on microsaccades in monkeys. As in human studies, following visual cues there was an early bias in cue-congruent microsaccades followed by a later bias in cue-incongruent microsaccades. Following auditory cues there was a cue-incongruent bias in left cues only. In a separate experiment, we observed that brainstem omnipause neurons, which gate all saccades, also paused during microsaccade generation. Thus, we provide evidence that at least part of the same neurocircuitry governs both large saccades and microsaccades.

Introduction

Small, involuntary and unperceivable eye movements occur during visual fixation and are classified into three types: tremor, drift and microsaccades (Barlow, 1952; Ratliff and Riggs, 1950; Steinman, Haddad, Skavenski, and Wyman, 1973; Martinez-Conde et al., 2004). Although there is some debate as to their function (see Rolfs, 2009 and Martinez-Conde et al., 2009 for reviews), it appears that these fixational eye movements serve as a means of introducing stochastic displacement to the retinal image in order to aid in counteracting retinal adaptation (Ditchburn et al., 1959; Martinez-Conde et al., 2006b; Troncoso et al., 2008). If all eye movements were suppressed while viewing a visual scene, the image on the retina would eventually fade away (Riggs et al., 1953) in about 80ms (Coppola and Purves, 1996) and these small eye movements prevent this from happening.
Among these small eye movements, microsaccades produce the largest displacements and are therefore the easiest to study. These eye movements have the same velocity amplitude relationship as larger saccades (Zuber et al., 1965). The study of microsaccade metrics in humans has revealed that biases in the inherent randomness of microsaccades can be produced under certain conditions and these biases may reveal underlying neurophysiological processes (Hafed and Clark, 2002; Engbert and Kliegl, 2003; Galfano et al., 2004; Laubrock et al., 2005; Rolfs et al., 2004; Rolfs et al., 2005; Martinez-Conde and Macknik, 2007).
The pattern of microsaccades is biased in covert orienting tasks. Where our eyes are oriented (overt attention) and where our attention is actually focused (covert attention) can be separated with cueing tasks (Posner, 1980). A visual cue can be presented during fixation in a predictive (in the same location) or non-predictive (in a diametrically opposite location) location of an upcoming target. By reducing the time between the cue and the target (the CTOA – cue-target onset asynchrony), shorter reaction times to cue congruent targets were observed. This has been referred to as reflexive capture of attention (Jonides, 1981; Posner and Cohen, 1984; Remington et al., 1992). This early capture of attention is followed by a later epoch that results in longer reaction times to cue congruent targets and this has been referred to as inhibition of return (IOR) (Posner and Cohen, 1984; Posner et al., 1985; Maylor and Hockey, 1985).
In human studies, it has been observed that microsaccade properties are modulated during these cueing tasks, but the modulation varies depending on the type of cue and experimental conditions used. First, microsaccade rate is affected by the sudden onset of both auditory and visual cues (Engbert and Kliegl, 2003; Rolfs et al., 2005). During steady fixation of a fixed visual display, microsaccades have a baseline rate of around one per second, but following the appearance of a cue in the periphery there is an early decrease in frequency of microsaccades around 150 ms after cue onset, followed by a later increase in frequency of microsaccades that peaks at twice that of baseline around 350 ms after cue onset. The early decrease in frequency was expected as this is also seen for larger saccades (Reingold and Stampe, 2002), but the later increase is novel to microsaccades (Engbert and Kliegl, 2003). Second, many studies have shown that microsaccade direction is affected by cue location (Hafed and Clark, 2002; Engbert and Kliegl, 2003; Galfano et al., 2004; Laubrock et al., 2004; Rolfs et al., 2004; Rolfs et al., 2005). Following a peripheral cue, microsaccade directions are first biased towards the cue during the period of capture of attention and then biased away from the cue during the period of inhibition of return (Hafed and Clark, 2002; Galfano et al., 2004; Laubrock et al., 2004; Rolfs et al., 2004; Rolfs et al., 2005) The magnitude of the influence of microsaccades on behavior is still debated (Horowitz et al., 2007a, Horowitz et al., 2007b and Laubrock et al., 2007) and appears to be much smaller and more complicated (Kliegl et al., 2009),than the influence of the cue. However, because the stochastic nature of microsaccades can be biased by spatial attention, the connection of attention to saccade programming is reinforced and they present an opportunity to study the underlying oculomotor processing that occurs during covert orienting.
As we attempt to understand the neurophysiology that underlies microsaccades and their relationship to multisensory input and integration, attention and oculomotor control, cell recording in animal models is necessary. Monkeys have been used extensively as a model for studying the neurophysiology of the oculomotor and attention systems (Fecteau and Munoz, 2006). While there have been studies on microsaccades in monkeys (Van Gisbergen et al., 1981; Martinez-Conde, 2000; Martinez-Conde et al., 2002; Horwitz, et al., 2003; Martinez-Conde et al., 2006a; Hafed et al., 2009), as well as other animals (Martinez-Conde and Macknik, 2008), to our knowledge there exists only a brief description of microsaccade characteristics during covert orienting using nonpredictive cues (Corneil et al., 2008). Therefore, it is important to verify that equivalent microsaccade behavior is observed in monkeys. Saccade behavior in monkeys during cue-target tasks is consistent with that in humans (Fecteau et al., 2005). We therefore hypothesize that microsaccade metrics will be modulated analogously to what has been observed previously in human experiments. This analogy must be established before commencing detailed neurophysiological studies.
In order to verify the modulation of microsaccade metrics in monkeys, we used cue-target experiments (Figure 1A), similar to that of Posner (1980). In the first set of experiments we investigated the modulation of microsaccades during a purely visual task using predictive and non-predictive visual cues. In a follow up experiment we investigated the modulation of microsaccades during a multisensory cueing task using auditory cues and visual targets. We then compared these results to those found previously in humans. Finally, we present preliminary data showing how the discharge of brainstem omnipause neurons is modulated during the generation of microsaccades. Omnipause neurons are brainstem neurons that act as a gating mechanism for the initiation of saccades (See Scudder et al., 2002 for review). We hypothesize that, if microsaccade modulation is a result of saccade programming during covert attention, omnipause neurons should pause for microsaccades just as they do for larger saccades.

Methods

Two male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) weighing approximately 7 and 10 kg performed in this study. All protocols were approved by the Queen’s University. Animal Care Committee and were in full compliance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care policy for the use of laboratory animals.

Cue-target Experiments

We investigated the effects of visual and auditory cues on microsaccade rates and directions during an oculomotor version of the cue-target task (Figure 1A). Separate analyses from these experiments were previously published to study the neural mechanisms of cueing, independent of microsaccades (Bell et al., 2004; Fecteau et al., 2004). Here, we reanalyzed these same data, but now in the context of microsaccade detection and analysis.
Figure 1A depicts the cue-target task used in these experiments. All tasks began with the animal fixating on a fixation point (FP) in the center of a visual screen. After a variable amount of fixation time a cue was flashed in the periphery. After another variable amount of fixation time, the target appeared at the same location as the cue or at a diametrically opposite location. The time between cue and target onset is defined as the cue target onset asynchrony (CTOA). The animal was required to make a saccadic eye movement to the target and was rewarded for doing so. The cue and target appeared left or right and in any combination for all of these tasks with random probability.
Three separate, blocked experiments were performed auditory cue was presented on the left or the right. The animal the cue and target were visual and the cue did not predict the target (Fecteau et al., 2004). In experiment 2, the cue and target were visual, but the cue predicted the location of the target 75% of the time (Fecteau et al., 2004). In experiment 3, the cue was auditory, the target was visual, and the cue was not predictive of the target (Bell et al., 2004). The auditory cue was placed in a position that was commensurate with the position of the visual target. In experiments 1 and 2, the first fixation period was 5001000 ms long, the cue was flashed for 30ms and CTOAs of 50, 100, 200, 500 and 1200 ms were used. In experiment 3, the first fixation period was 600-800 ms long, the cue was presented for 50ms and CTOAs of 60, 160 and 610 ms were used. Note that because all of these experiments were originally designed for neurophysiological recording, the actual positions of targets and cues were aligned to the receptive fields of the neurons being studied in the superior colliculus. Therefore, we excluded all blocks of trials where the cue and target locations deviated more than 20o from the horizontal meridian. This also meant that cue and target eccentricities were variable, ranging from 2-20o (Figure 1B).
Horizontal and vertical eye position data were recorded with the search coil system and were digitized at 1 KHz for experiments 1 and 2 and 500 Hz for experiment 3. This technique has a spatial resolution of 0.1o. In our analysis we concentrated on the long CTOA trials (500 ms and 1200 ms for experiments 1 and 2 and 610 ms for experiment 3) for each experiment. We also only used data from trials where the animal performed the task correctly. We detected the microsaccades during the fixation period (i.e., from when the animal first started fixating the central FP until the target appeared) using the algorithm outlined below.

Recording Omnipause Neurons and Microsaccades

In a separate set of experiments (Everling et al., 1998) we recorded previously from brainstem omnipause neurons (OPNs) in a gap saccade task. Here we describe the discharge of OPNs modulated during the generation of microsaccades from this same data set.
In the gap saccade task, the animal began each trial by fixating on a central FP. In this task there was no cue. After a fixation period of 500-1000 ms the FP disappeared and, following a 200 ms gap when nothing was present on the screen, a target then appeared 10o to the left or right. The animal was required to make a saccade to the target. The monkeys performed several blocks of this gap saccade task. Horizontal and vertical eye position data was digitized at 500 Hz. Neuron activity was sampled at 1 kHz after passing through a window discriminator that filtered for action potentials that met both amplitude and temporal constraints. We detected microsaccades during the fixation period using the algorithm outlined below. Data from one monkey is presented.

Microsaccade Detection

Microsaccades were detected using an algorithm similar to that of Martinez-Conde and colleagues (Martinez-Conde et al., 2000). We first differentiated the eye position data to produce instantaneous horizontal and vertical velocities. These velocity data were smoothed using a 21 ms rectangular filter. From the velocity traces we calculated the instantaneous speed and direction at each point. We then thresholded these two values to determine when the eyes were stationary and when they were making a saccade. The instantaneous direction of the eyes could not change more than 15o to be considered a saccade. The instantaneous speed of the eyes had to be at least 8 o/sec. This speed threshold was chosen such that it gave the best main sequence as described by Zuber and colleagues (1965) (see the results section for an example). A saccade was then defined as a sequence where the eye was moving for a period of at least 5 ms. From the eye movement data we then calculated the onset and termination of all saccades, the peak velocity, the saccade vector and the saccade amplitude. Finally, the amplitude was used as a threshold such that we were only interested in microsaccades that were between 0.2-2o.

Microsaccade Analysis

We analyzed microsaccade direction using a technique developed by Rolfs and colleagues (Rolfs et al., 2005). In order to identify when microsaccade rates differed from randomness significantly over time, we plotted them against randomized data. After removing vertical microsaccades (those greater than 60o in direction away from horizontal), we shuffled the directions of all microsaccades from the original data, but maintained the microsaccade onsets (i.e., the direction of one microsaccade was randomly assigned to another microsaccade and this was done for each microsaccade). This shuffling of directions maintained any overall biases in direction and any changes in the rate of microsaccades. The process was done 100 times and the rates of congruent and incongruent microsaccades were calculated for each iteration. A congruent microsaccade was one in which the direction was towards the cue location, whereas an incongruent microsaccade was one in which the direction was away from the cue location. A 100 ms rectangular filter was applied to the individual rates to compensate for splitting the data. The rates were then averaged to get a distribution representing the null hypothesis that there was no significant difference in congruent and incongruent microsaccades. We then calculated the actual rates for congruent and incongruent microsaccades and smoothed those with a 100 ms rectangular filter. Finally, we plotted the rate deviation of the actually congruent and incongruent microsaccades from the corresponding null distributions (e.g., Figure 3B, Figure 4B, Figure 5B and Figure 6B). We also plotted two standard deviations away from the randomized data (the shaded area of Figure 3B, Figure 4B, Figure 5B and Figure 6B). The rate deviation was significant when it differed from the corresponding randomized data by more than two standard deviations. We only considered deviations that lasted more than 20 ms as significant.

Results

We first calculated the main sequence relationship to verify our microsaccade detection algorithm (Figure 2). This figure depicts the expected linear relationship between saccade amplitude and peak velocity of 6112 microsaccades from both monkeys during experiment 1. There was a correlation coefficient of 0.9589 (p < 0.5) between the two variables and a linear regression to these points had a slope of 0.6868 deg/sec/minarc with an intercept of 4.4182 deg/sec. These results verify the algorithm, based on the fact that microsaccades follow the same amplitude-velocity relationship as larger saccades (Zuber et al., 1965).

Experiment 1: non-predictive visual cues

Behavior. In this experiment, cue location did not predict target location. See Fecteau et al. (2004) for the original analysis of behavior. At the short CTOAs (50 ms), saccadic reaction times were significantly shorter when the cue appeared at the same location as the target, compared to when the cue and target appeared on opposite sides. This has been called attention capture. However, at longer CTOAs (100-1200 ms), saccadic reaction times were significantly longer when the cue appeared at the same location as the target. This has been referred to as inhibition or return.
Microsaccade statistics. Figure 3A summarizes the average microsaccade rate from 2 monkeys during the fixation period of the visual non-predictive cueing task. The monkeys performed 5190 trials and 4275 microsaccades were detected. The rate for each animal was also smoothed using a 10 ms rectangular filter. There was an over-representation of microsaccades early in the fixation epoch before cue appearance that was likely due to small corrective saccades after the animal reached the fixation window around the central FP. Just before the cue appeared there was a stable baseline rate of about 0.5 saccades/s. Following cue onset, microsaccade rate began to decrease after about 50 ms and reached a minimum of about 0.2 saccades/s during the interval 120-250 ms after cue onset. The rate then began to increase again and reached a peak of about 0.7 saccades/s 400 ms after cue onset. We chose 3 epochs to test these rates quantitatively. t1 refers to a 50ms epoch before the onset of the cue. t2 refers to aa epoch 100-150 ms after onset of the cue and is used to test for a decrease in microsaccade rate soon after cue onset. t3 refers to an epoch 400-450 ms after onset of the cue and is used to test for an increase in microsaccade rate after cue onset. Using a t-test we found a significant decrease in microsaccade rate during t2, when compared to t1 (p < 0.5), and a significant increase in microsaccade rate during t3, when compared to t1 (p < 0.5).
Two significant deviations from the randomized data were observed for non-predictive visual cues. The first significant difference occurred 112-150 ms after cue appearance (see * in Figure 3B). During this period there was a significant increase in cue-congruent microsac-Figure 4B). This was followed by a later and more procades and a brief, but significant increase in cue-congruent micr cades and a brief, but significant decrease in incongruent microsaccades (i.e., microsaccades tended to point in the direction of the cue). The second significant differences occured 280-475 ms after the cue Figure 3B). During this time there was a significant decrease in cue-congruent microsaccades and a signifcant increase in cue-incongruent mimicrosaccades tended to point in the opposite direction of the cue).

Experiment 2: predictive visual cues

Behavior. In this experiment, cue location was predictive of target location on 75% of trials. See Fecteau et al., 2004 for the original analysis of behavior. For targets appearing at the same location as the cue, it was found that predictive cues enhanced the effects of capture of attention. Specifically, saccadic reaction times were faster for predictive cues than for non-predictive cues and this enhancement effect was present for all but the longest CTOA (1200 ms). For targets appearing at the opposite location to the cue, this enhancement effect was not present. However, for the 50 ms CTOA, saccadic reaction times were faster for non-predictive cues than for predictive cues.
Microsaccade statistics. Figure 4A summarizes the average microsaccade rate for blocks of trials utilizing predictive cues. 6302 trials were performed between the two animals and 3306 microsaccades were de Similar to the non-predictive cues, we observed a bas line of about 0.5 saccades/s before cue presentation. Following cue presentation, microsaccade rate started to decline at about 80 ms and reached a minimum of 0.05 saccades/s at 120-200 ms. The rate then began to increase steadily until it reached a peak of cades/s at approximately 450 ms. Again, significant decrease in microsaccade rate during t2, wh compared to t1 (p < 0.5), and a significant increase in microsaccade rate during t3, when compared to t1 (p < 0.5).
To study microsaccade directions we performed the null distribution analysis. Similar deviations from randomness to the non-predictive cueing task could be observed during the predictive cueing task (Figure 4B). Specifically, there was an early significant increase in congruent saccades and consequently a significant decrease in incongruent saccades at 50-150 ms (see * in Figure 4B). This was followed by a later and more pro-cades and a brief, but significant decrease in congruent saccades and significant increase in incongruent saccades at approximately 280-500 ms (see ++ in Figure 4B).

Experiment 3: non-predictive auditory cues

Behaviour. We also employed auditory cues to determine their effect on microsaccade production. See Bell et al., 2004 for the original analysis of behavior. In contrast to what was observed for predictive and non-predictive visual cues, the auditory cues we used preceding a visual target did not have any significant effect on reaction time for either short or long CTOAs.
Microsaccade statistics. Microsaccade rates for auditory cues (Figure 5A) were analyzed in the same manner as for visual cues. 778 trials were performed between the two animals and 704 microsaccades were detected. The baseline microsaccade rate was about 0.8 saccades/s before cue presentation. The rate began to decline 20-30 ms after cue presentation and reached a minimum of 0.3 about saccades/s at 150-200 ms after cue onset. The rate then increased and reached a maximum of about 1.1 saccades/s at 450 ms. Again, we found a significant decrease in microsaccade rate during t2, when compared to t1 (p < 0.5), and a significant increase in microsaccade rate during t3, when compared to t1 (p < 0.5).
Microsaccade directions were again analyzed using the null distribution method. No significant deviations from randomness could be observed for either congruent or incongruent microsaccades (Figure 5B), but there were late trends of decreased congruent microsaccades and increased incongruent microsaccades at 250-450 ms after cue onset, which was consistent with the trends observed in the visual cueing experiments. To further investigate this trend, we split the congruent and incongruent data sets into their respective left and right cue conditions (Figure 6). In this analysis, we observed that left auditory cues were responsible for this late trend towards incongruent microsaccades. This trend was significant for left cues at approximately 350-425 ms, but did not exist for right cues (see ++ in Figure 6).

Experiment 4: recording from omnipause neurons

We had the opportunity to assess the activity of brainstem OPNs during microsaccade generation by reanalyzing data that was published previously (Everling et al., 1998). OPNs are tonically active for fixation and pause for all macrosaccades, thereby gating the occurrence of saccades (see Scudder et al., 2002 for review). Our goal here was to determine whether OPNs also paused for microsaccades. We recorded from 18 OPNs in one monkey and collapsed the data. The animal performed 1306 trials and we identified 250 microsaccades. The lower number of microsaccades was likely due to the limited fixation period analyzed and the absence of a cue. Figure 7 summarizes the activity of OPNs aligned on microsaccade onset for those microsaccades where there was at least 100 ms of fixation before and after microsaccade occurrence (177 trials). We identified two time periods to analyze the firing rate of OPNs. The first epoch, t1, refers to 35-50 ms before microsaccade onset and was used as the baseline firing rate. The second epoch, t2, refers to 0-15 ms after microsaccade onset. The baseline firing rate was 162 spikes/s (t1) whereas the rate directly after microsaccade onset was 62 spikes/s (t2). A t-test revealed that this difference was significant (p < 0.05). Thus, OPNs pause for microsaccades just as for larger macro saccades (i.e. > 2o amplitude).

Discussion

Using peripheral visual and auditory cues in a cuetarget paradigm, we have shown that microsaccade properties are modulated during reflexive and voluntary covert shifts of visual attention in monkeys. Microsaccade rates decreased quickly after cue presentation, reaching a minimum approximately 100 ms later. This early decrease of microsaccade frequency was followed by a enhancement, reaching a maximum approximately 400 ms after cue presentation. This early drop in rate is similar to saccade inhibition produced by transient changes in the visual display (Reingold and Stampe, 2002). Microsaccade directions were also biased by cue presentation. For visual cues there was an early cue congruent bias in microsaccades, followed by a later and more pronounced cue incongruent bias. For auditory cues there was only a cue incongruent bias and only for cues to the left.
Several studies have investigated the effects of covert orienting on microsaccades in humans using cue-target paradigms. Our results from monkeys are largely in agreement with the results of previous human studies. The modulation of microsaccades rates following cue presentation was first observed in humans by Engbert and Kliegl (2003) using predictive, endogenous visual cues. Immediately following cue presentation, they observed an inhibition of microsaccades to 20% of baseline, reaching a minimum approximately 150 ms later. Following this, they observed an enhancement of microsaccades to double the baseline rate, reaching a maximum approximately 350 ms after cue presentation. This characteristic pattern was also seen in monkeys (Figure 3 and Figure 4), although the inhibition appeared to occur earlier and reach a minimum earlier than in the human studies. They also found a baseline rate of 1 saccade/s during fixation, whereas we found a baseline rate closer to 2 saccades/s. The differences in time course and baseline rates could be due to many factors, such as experimental differences or extensive overtraining in the monkeys, but the overall pattern was the same. They also found that this modulation occurred during neutral cues that did not convey any information about the target. They concluded that any visual transient was sufficient to produce this result. This was supported by subsequently studies that found this characteristic modulation regardless of the type or meaning of the cue (Galfano, 2004 – non-predictive exogenous visual cues; Laubrock, 2004 – nonpredictive endogenous and exogenous visual cues; Rolfs et al., 2004 – predictive exogenous, visual and auditory cues; Rolfs et al., 2007 predictive visual cues). Our results are consistent with these findings because we observed this modulation in both predictive and non predictive cues, as well as with auditory cues.
Several studies have examined the effects of visual cues on microsaccade direction. Hafed and Clark (2002) first observed the characteristic pattern of a cuecongruent microsaccade bias soon after the presentation of a predictive cue, followed by a cue-incongruent microsaccade bias. They correlated these biases in microsaccade direction with behavioral differences and suggested that microsaccades were influenced by covert attentional shifts. Their experimental set up used a cue that was always on and involved multiple targets on the horizontal and vertical meridian. Despite the differences between their task and our horizontal cue-target tasks, our results are consistent with theirs. In separate studies, Laubrock and colleages (2005) and Rolfs and colleages (2005) used cue-target paradigms with predictive, transient visual cues and also found this characteristic pattern. Both studies identified an early cue-congruent bias at approximately 50-150 ms after cue presentation, followed later by a cue-incongruent bias at approximately 250-550 ms. We found similar epochs in our results for predictive visual cues (Figure 4). Galfano and colleagues (2004) used peripheral, non-predictive visual cues, but while they did find the cue-incongruent bias at 300ms after cue presentation, they did not observe the early cue-congruent effect that we found in our non-predictive visual cueing task (Figure 3). This could be the result of species differences or the amount of training that monkeys underwent prior to data collection. This may also be the result of different task conditions. First, we use flashed points for our cues, while Galfano and colleagues (2004) used boxes that were always on and became thicker and more intense when cued. Hence, it is possible that our cues elicited a stronger capture of attention response. Second, we use saccades to targets to signify a response and measure behavior, while they used manual button press responses. Because the monkeys were required to move their eyes towards the target, the effect of the visual cue may have been enhanced.
Support for visual cues enhancing covert attention responses in microsaccades comes from Rolfs and colleagues (2005) who also observed the effects of auditory cues on microsaccade direction. They found that in a purely auditory task (i.e., auditory cues and targets) there were no early cue-congruent or late cue-incongruent biases. There was only a cue-congruent bias that happened later, and only for left cues, around 200 ms after target presentation. They argued that this was a result of decreased occulomotor control resulting from a lack of visual information in the purely auditory tasks. Accordingly, when human subjects performed a task with auditory cues and visual targets, they observed a cueincongruent bias 300-400 ms after cue presentation for cues to the left. These results agree with our findings for auditory cues and visual targets in monkeys, where we found cue-incongruent biases for cues to the left at a similar epoch (Figure 6). Rolfs and colleagues (2005) suggested that this left-hemisphere bias could be explained by a left-hemisphere advantage to brief auditory cues (Brown and Nicholls, 1997, Nicholls et al., 1999) or by a right-hemisphere bias of attention (Reuter-Lorenz et al., 1990).
We provide further support that microsaccades reflect the orientation of covert spatial attention. As in studies with humans, we observed microsaccade directions that were consistent with an early capture of attention and a later inhibition of return. Fecteau and colleagues (2004) observed the largest capture of attention effect in monkeys at the 50ms CTOA in both predictive and nonpredictive visual cueing. During this time period we saw a significant bias in cue-congruent microsaccades. For CTOAs of 100-1200 ms they saw an inhibition of return response in both experiments that peaked at a CTOA of 200ms and then gradually declined. During this time period we saw a significant bias in cue-incongruent microsaccades. Both microsaccade rate and direction modulations in monkeys during cue-target tasks are consistent with that found in human studies. From these results we conclude that monkeys are a suitable model for studying the neurological pathways involved in the generation and manipulation of microsaccades. Future experiments can use cell recording to elucidate these pathways and how the activation of the visual system during covert orienting influences the behaviour of microsaccades.
We examined the modulation of brainstem OPNactivity during microsaccade generation (Figure 7). Hafed and Clark (2002) suggested that because microsaccades follow the same velocity-amplitude relationship as larger saccades (Zuber et al., 1965) and reflect the orientation of covert visual attention, it is likely that the same neurocircuitry governing larger saccades is responsible for the generation and modulation of microsaccades. One component of this circuitry is OPNs, which are brainstem neurons that are tonically active and only pause during saccade generation (Cohen and Henn 1972, Keller 1974, Luschei and Fuchs 1972). Specifically, OPNs are tonic inhibitors of burst neurons located in the paramedian pontine reticular formation and the rostral interstitial nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus (see Fuchs et al., 1985 for a review). These burst neurons produce high-frequency discharges during saccades and thus have the opposite discharge pattern of OPNs. Hence, OPNs act to stabilize the visual system and act as a gating mechanism for the generation of saccades (See Scudder et al., 2002 for a review). It is reasonable then that they would also gate microsaccades and indeed, this is what we observed. OPNs paused for microsaccades (Figure 7) as they do for larger saccades. Everling and colleagues (1998) observed that OPNs displayed a small burst in activity approximately 60 ms after target presentation during regular-latency saccades. This burst occurs approximately the same time that we observed the overall microsaccade rate to decrease after cue presentation. It is possible that increased OPN activity during this time triggered the decrease in microsaccade rate. Finally, OPNs were also implicated by Ashe et al. (1991) as a cause for microsaccadic flutter, which is a rare disorder characterized by saccadic oscillations. From these findings it seems likely that OPNs are involved in governing microsaccades.
Van Gisbergen and colleagues (1981) found that burst neurons and motoneurons burst or paused in time with microsaccades, supporting the hypothesis that burst neurons are controlling the generation and inhibition of microsaccades. More recently, Hafed and colleagues (2009) found that, during a fixation task, neurons in the rostral pole of the superior colliculus (SC) burst before and during microsaccades and were selective for the amplitude and direction of these saccades. They also found that inactivation of these neurons resulted in a decrease in microsaccade rate.
Behavioural studies have also provided insight into possible neural mechanisms governing microsaccades. Rolfs and colleagues (Rolfs et al., 2006) showed, in visual and memory guided tasks, that saccadic reaction time was increased when microsaccades occured within 150 ms of a required saccade. They further showed that microsaccade rate decreases steadily towards zero before saccade onset. They hypothesized that this provides support for microsaccades resulting from activity in the rostral pole of the SC, since larger saccades have to overcome this fixation activity before they are executed. Based on this idea, Rolfs and colleagues (Rolfs et al., 2008b) went on to postulate a common-field model for microsaccade and saccade generation. This model also predicted that saccadic reaction time would be correlated with the amplitude of microsaccades occurring directly before the saccade and they indeed did find this in their data (Rolfs et al., 2008a). However, Valsecchi and Turatto (Valsecchi and Turatto, 2007) showed that microsaccade rate responded similarly when stimuli were visible and invisible to the SC, suggesting that other brain areas are involved in microsaccade generation. Finally, OteroMillan and colleagues (Otero-Millan et. al 2008) showed that saccades and microsaccades have equivalent spatiotemporal characteristics during visual exploration, search and prolonged fixation. The results of these behavioural and neurophysiological studies, together with our results on omnipause neurons, suggest a common mechanism for the generation of microsaccade and larger saccades. Thus, biases in these neural mechanisms, such as those generated by shifts in visual attention, should modulate both microsaccades and larger saccades.
In summary, we have verified that microsaccade metrics in monkeys are modulated by reflexive covert attention analogously to what has been found in human studies. Consequently, we can use monkeys as a model for studying the neurophysiology of microsaccades. We have provided further evidence that microsaccade orientation reflects the orientation of covert visual attention. Finally, we have provided evidence that the same neurocircuitry is involved in the generation of microsaccades and larger saccades. Future experiments in monkeys will involve cell recording and will require longer fixation periods to generate an accurate picture of the time course of microsaccade production.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ann Lablans for outstanding technical assistance and members of the Munoz lab for commenting on an earlier draft of the manuscript. This work was supported by research grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Douglas P. Munoz was supported by the Canada Research Chair Program.

References

  1. Ashe, J., T. C. Hain, D. S. Zee, and N. J. Schatz. 1991. Microsaccadic flutter. Brain 114: 461–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Barlow, H. B. 1952. Eye movements during fixation. Journal of Physiology 116: 290–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bell, A. H., J. H. Fecteau, and D. P. Munoz. 2004. Using auditory and visual stimuli to investigate the beha-vioral and neuronal consequences of reflexive covert orienting. Journal of Neurophysiology 91: 2172–2184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Brown, S., and M.E. Nicholls. 1997. Hemispheric asym-metries for the temporal resolution of brief auditory stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics 59: 442–447. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cohen, B., and V. Henn. 1972. Unit activity in the pontine reticular formation associated with eye movements. Brain Research 46: 403–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Coppola, D., and D. Purves. 1996. The extraordinarily rapid disappearance of entopic images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, 15: 8001–8004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Corneil, B. D., D. P. Munoz, B. B. Chapman, T. Admans, and S. L. Cushing. 2008. Neuromuscular conse-quences of reflexive covert orienting. Nature Neuroscience 11, 1: 13–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ditchburn, R. W., D. H. Fender, and S. Mayne. 1959. Vision with controlled movements of the retinal image. Journal of Physiology 145: 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Engbert, R., and Kliegl. 2003. Microaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research 43: 1035–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Everling, S., M. Pare, M. C. Dorris, and D. P. Munoz. 1998. Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology 79: 511–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Fecteau, J. H., A. H. Bell, and D. P. Munoz. 2004. Neur-al correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology 92: 1728–1737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Fecteau, J. H., and D. P. Munoz. 2005. Correlates of cap-ture of attention and inhibition of return across states of visual processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 11: 1714–1727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fecteau, J. H., and D. P. Munoz. 2006. Salience, relevance, and spiking neurons: a priority map governs target selection. Trends in Cognitive Science 10: 382–390. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fuchs, A. F., C. R. S. Kaneko, and C. A. Scudder. 1985. Brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. An-nual Review of Neuroscience, 307–337. [Google Scholar]
  15. Galfano, G., E. Betta, and M. Turatto. 2004. Inhibition of return in microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research 159: 400–404. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hafed, Z.M., and J.J. Clark. 2002. Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Re-search 42: 2533–2545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hafed, Z.M., L. Goffart, and R.J. Krauzlis. 2009. A Neural Mechanism for Microsaccade Generation in the Primate Superior Colliculus. Science 323: 940–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Horwitz, G. D., and T. D. Albright. 2003. Short-latency fixational saccades induced by luminance increments. Journal of Neurophysiology 90: 1333–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Horowitz, T. S., E. M. Fine, D. E. Fencsik, S. Yurgenson, and J. M. Wolfe. 2007a. Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention. Psycological Science 18: 356–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Horowitz, T. S., E. M. Fine, D. E. Fencsik, S. Yurgenson, and J. M. Wolfe. 2007b. Microsaccades and attention. Does a weak correlation make an index? Reply to Laubrock, Engbert, Rolfs, and Kliegl. Psycological Science 18: 367–368. [Google Scholar]
  21. Jonides, J. 1981. Edited by J. Long and A. Badde-ley. Voluntary vs. automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In Attention and Performance IX. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: pp. 187–203. [Google Scholar]
  22. Keller, E. 1974. Participation of the medial pontine reticular formation in eye movement generation in the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 37: 316–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Kliegl, R., M. Rolfs, J. Laubrock, and R. Engbert. 2009. Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks. Psychological Research 73: 136–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Laubrock, J., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2004. Microsac-cade dynamics during covert attention. Vision Research 45: 721–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Laubrock, R., R. Engbert, M. Rolfs, and R. Kliegl. 2007. Microsaccades are an index of covert attention. Psycological Science 18: 364–366. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lushei, E.S., and A.F. Fuchs. 1972. Activity of brain stem neurons during eye movements of alert monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 35: 445–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, and D. H. Hubel. 2000. Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys. Nature Neuroscience 3, 3: 251–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknick, and D. H. Hubel. 2002. The function of bursts of spikes during visual fixation in the awake primate lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99, 21: 13920–13925. [Google Scholar]
  29. Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, and D. H. Hubel. 2004. The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Martinez-Conde, S. 2006a. Fixational eye movements in normal and pathological vision. Progress in Brain Research 154: 151–176. [Google Scholar]
  31. Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, X. G. Troncoso, and T. A. Dyar. 2006b. Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron 49: 297–305. [Google Scholar]
  32. Martinez-Conde, S., and S. L. Macknik. 2007. Windows on the Mind. Scientific American 297, 2: 56–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Martinez-Conde, S., and S. L. Macknik. 2008. Fixational eye movements across vertebrates: Comparative dynamics, physiology, and perception. Journal of Vi-sion 8, 14: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, X. G. Troncoso, and D. H. Hubel. 2009. Microsaccades: A neurophysio-logical analysis. Trends in Neurosciences 32, 9: 463–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Maylor, E. A., and R. Hockey. 1985. Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11: 777–787. [Google Scholar]
  36. Nicholls, M. E., M. Schier, C.K. Stough, and A. Box. 1999. Psychophysical and electrophysiological support for a left hemisphere temporal processing advantage. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology 12: 11–16. [Google Scholar]
  37. Otero-Millan, J, X. G. Troncoso, S. L. Macknik, I. Serra-no-Pedraza, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2008. Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: Foundations for a common saccadic generator. Journal of Vision 8, (14):21: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Posner, M. I. 1980. Orientation of attention. The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32A: 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Posner, M. I., and Y. Cohen. 1984. Components of visual orienting. In At-tention and performance X. Edited by H. Bouma and G. Bouwhuis D. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: pp. 531–556. [Google Scholar]
  40. Posner, M. I., R. D. Rafal, L. S. Choate, and J. Vaughan. 1985. Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2: 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ratliff, F., and L. A. Riggs. 1950. Involuntary motions of the eye during monocular fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology 40: 687–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reingold, E. M., and D. M. Stampe. 2002. Saccadic inhibition in voluntary and reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14: 371–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Remington, R. W., J. C. Johnston, and S. Yantis. 1992. Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Percept Psychophys 51: 279–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., A.P. Jha, and J.N. Rosenquist. 1990. Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition 12: 240–266. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. Riggs, L. A., F. Ratliff, J. C. Cornsweet, and T. N. Cornsweet. 1953. The disappearance of steadily fixated test objects. Journal of the Optical Society of America 43: 495–501. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rolfs, M., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2004. Microsaccade orientation supports attentional enhancement opposite a peripheral cue. Commentary on Tse, Shein-berg, and Logothetis (2003). Psychological Science 15: 705–707. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rolfs, M., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2005. Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition. Experimental Brain Research 166: 427–439. [Google Scholar]
  48. Rolfs, M., J. Laubrock, and R. Kliegl. 2006. Shortening and prolongation of saccade latencies following microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research 169: 369–376. [Google Scholar]
  49. Rolfs, M., J Laubrock, and R. Kliegl. 2008a. Microsaccade-induced prolongation of saccade latencies depends on microsaccade amplitude. Journal of Eye Movement Research 1(3), Art. 5: 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  50. Rolfs, M., R. Kliegl, and R. Engbert. 2008b. Toward a model of microsaccade generation: The case of microsaccadic inhibition. Journal of Vision 8, (11), Art. 5: 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rolfs, M. 2009. Microsaccades: Small steps on a long way. Vision Research 49: 2415–2441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Scudder, C. A., C. S. Kaneko, and A. F. Fuchs. 2002. The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements. A modern synthesis. Experimental Brain Research 142: 439–462. [Google Scholar]
  53. Steinman, R. M., G. M. Haddad, A. A. Skavenski, and D. Wyman. 1973. Miniature eye movements. Science 181: 810–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Troncoso, X. G., S. L. Macknik, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2008. Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in. Journal of Vision 8, 14: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Van Gisbergen, J. A. M., D. A. Robinson, and S. Gielen. 1981. A Quantitative Analysis of Generation of Saccadic Eye Movements by Burst Neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 45, 3: 417–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Valsecchi, M., and M. Turatto. 2007. Microsaccadic response to visual events that are invisible to the superior colliculus. Behavioral Neuroscience 121: 786–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Zuber, B. L., L. Stark, and G. Cook. 1965. Microsaccades and the velocity-amplitude relationship for saccadic eye movements. Science 150: 1459–1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. (A) Schematic representation of the cue-target tasks used. Each trial began with the animal fixating on a central fixation point (FP). After a variable time a brief visual or auditory cue was presented on the then maintained fixation again for another variable time: the cue-target onset asynchrony (CTOA). The fixation point was then extinguished and a visual target appeared to the left or right at the same eccentricity as the c preformed in separate experiments such that the animals performed blocks with visual cues that were either non predictive or predictive or separate blocks in which auditory cues were non-predictive. (B) Histograms of cue eccentricities used in each experiment.
Figure 1. (A) Schematic representation of the cue-target tasks used. Each trial began with the animal fixating on a central fixation point (FP). After a variable time a brief visual or auditory cue was presented on the then maintained fixation again for another variable time: the cue-target onset asynchrony (CTOA). The fixation point was then extinguished and a visual target appeared to the left or right at the same eccentricity as the c preformed in separate experiments such that the animals performed blocks with visual cues that were either non predictive or predictive or separate blocks in which auditory cues were non-predictive. (B) Histograms of cue eccentricities used in each experiment.
Jemr 03 00009 g001
Figure 2. The main sequence plot for monkey A co all visual cueing experiments. The expected linear relationship between amplitude and peak velocity was observed.
Figure 2. The main sequence plot for monkey A co all visual cueing experiments. The expected linear relationship between amplitude and peak velocity was observed.
Jemr 03 00009 g002
Figure 3. The modulation of microsaccade rates during fixation in a visual non-predictive cue-target task. Average microsaccade rate is plotted relative to cue onset (A). t1, t2 and t3, refer to time epochs chosen to analyze changes in rate after stimulus presentation. The rate deviation of congruent and incongruent microsaccades from randomized data is also plotted (B). Congruency refers to cue and microsaccade direction. The shaded area represents two standard deviations from the mean of 100 randomized data sets. Hence, when the rate deviation is greater than or larger than the shaded area, the deviation is significant.
Figure 3. The modulation of microsaccade rates during fixation in a visual non-predictive cue-target task. Average microsaccade rate is plotted relative to cue onset (A). t1, t2 and t3, refer to time epochs chosen to analyze changes in rate after stimulus presentation. The rate deviation of congruent and incongruent microsaccades from randomized data is also plotted (B). Congruency refers to cue and microsaccade direction. The shaded area represents two standard deviations from the mean of 100 randomized data sets. Hence, when the rate deviation is greater than or larger than the shaded area, the deviation is significant.
Jemr 03 00009 g003
Figure 4. The modulation of microsaccade rates du fixation in a visual predictive cue-target task. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Figure 3.
Figure 4. The modulation of microsaccade rates du fixation in a visual predictive cue-target task. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Figure 3.
Jemr 03 00009 g004
Figure 5. The modulation of microsaccade rates during fixation in an auditory non-predictive cue-target task. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Figure 3.
Figure 5. The modulation of microsaccade rates during fixation in an auditory non-predictive cue-target task. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Figure 3.
Jemr 03 00009 g005
Figure 6. The modulation of horizontal microsaccade rates during fixation in an auditory non-predictive cue-target task. The rate deviation of left and right microsaccades from randomized data during left and right cues is plotted. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Fig. 3.
Figure 6. The modulation of horizontal microsaccade rates during fixation in an auditory non-predictive cue-target task. The rate deviation of left and right microsaccades from randomized data during left and right cues is plotted. This figure was created using the same analysis as in Fig. 3.
Jemr 03 00009 g006
Figure 7. The modulation of omnipause neuron activity during microsaccade generation. The individual action potentials for 177 trials are plotted at the top. Followed by the spike density function representing the relative average number of spikes (convolved with a Gaussian function of width 10ms) and the eye position traces of microsaccades. t1 refers to a period 35-50ms before microsaccade onset and was used to calculate a baseline rate of neuronal activity. t2 refers to a period 0-15ms after microsaccade onset and was used to calculate the rate of neuronal activity during microsaccades.
Figure 7. The modulation of omnipause neuron activity during microsaccade generation. The individual action potentials for 177 trials are plotted at the top. Followed by the spike density function representing the relative average number of spikes (convolved with a Gaussian function of width 10ms) and the eye position traces of microsaccades. t1 refers to a period 35-50ms before microsaccade onset and was used to calculate a baseline rate of neuronal activity. t2 refers to a period 0-15ms after microsaccade onset and was used to calculate the rate of neuronal activity during microsaccades.
Jemr 03 00009 g007
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brien, D.C.; Corneil, B.D.; Fecteau, J.H.; Bell, A.H.; Munoz, D.P. The Behavioural and Neurophysiological Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkeys. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2009, 3, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.2.4

AMA Style

Brien DC, Corneil BD, Fecteau JH, Bell AH, Munoz DP. The Behavioural and Neurophysiological Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkeys. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2009; 3(2):1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.2.4

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brien, Donald C., Brian D. Corneil, Jillian H. Fecteau, Andrew H. Bell, and Douglas P. Munoz. 2009. "The Behavioural and Neurophysiological Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkeys" Journal of Eye Movement Research 3, no. 2: 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.2.4

APA Style

Brien, D. C., Corneil, B. D., Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2009). The Behavioural and Neurophysiological Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkeys. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(2), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.2.4

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop