Integrating Cognitive Factors and Eye Movement Data in Reading Predictive Models for Children with Dyslexia and ADHD-I
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
- (1)
- Control group: this group comprised 19 individuals, of which 78.9% were female.
- (2)
- Developmental Dyslexia: there were 19 children in this group, with 57.9% being female.
- (3)
- ADHD-I children: this group included 21 children, with 47.6% being female.
2.2. Criteria for Inclusion and Procedures
2.3. Psychometric and Linguistic Measures
2.3.1. Intellectual Ability
2.3.2. Phonological Awareness
2.3.3. Reading Comprehension
2.3.4. Reading Fluency and Accuracy Assessment
2.4. Eye-Tracking Measures
2.5. Materials
2.6. Procedure
2.7. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Neurocognitive Measures
3.2. Linguistic Measures
3.3. Multinomial Logistic Regression
3.4. Discussion and Conclusions
Ethics and Conflict of Interest
Acknowledgments
References
- Al Otaiba, S. 2002. Characteristics of Children Who Are Unresponsive to Early. Remedial and Special Education 23, 5: 300–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alloway, T. P., and K. Cockcroft. 2014. Working Memory in ADHD: A Comparison of British and South African Children. Journal of Attention Disorders 18, 4: 286–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- American Psychiatric Association. 2002. DSM-IV-TR Manual de Diagnóstico e Estatística das Perturbações Mentais. Climepsi (ed.); 4a Edição. American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed. Author. [Google Scholar]
- Bacelar do Nascimento, M., J. Casteleiro, M. Marques, F. Barreto, R. Amaro, and R. Veloso. n.d. Léxico Multifuncional Computorizado do Português Contemporâneo. Retrieved January 1, 2014. Available online: http://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/en/recurso/multifunction=al-computational-lexicon-contemporary-.
- Baddeley, A. D. 1990. Human memory: Theory and practice. Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. D. 2002. Is Working Memory Still Working? 1Copyright © 2001 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission from the original publication: Baddeley A. (2001). “Is Working Memory Still Working?” American Psychologist, 56, 849–864. This re-print. European Psychologist 7, 2: 85–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. D. 2003. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 10: 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A. D. 2012. Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 1: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. D., and G. Hitch. 1992. Working memory. Edited by G. A. Bower. In Science. Oxford University Press: Vol. 255, Issue 5044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. D., and B. Wilson. 1988. Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain and Cognition 7, 2: 212–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barkley, R. A. 1997. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin 121, 1: 65–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boets, B., B. Smedt, L. Cleuren, E. Vandewalle, J. Wouters, and P. Ghesquière. 2010. Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28, 1: 5–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowers, P. G., and G. Ishaik. 2003. Edited by S. Graham, H. Swanson and K. R. Lee Harris. RAN’s contribution to understanding reading disabilities. In Handbook of learning disabilities. Guilford Press: pp. 140–157. [Google Scholar]
- Cadime, I., I. S. Ribeiro, and F. L. Viana. 2012. TCL—Teste de Compreensão da Leitura. CEGOC-TEA Edições. [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas, M., J. Volín, and C. Hulme. 2005. Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 92, 2: 107–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carvalho, A. 2010. O REI—Teste de Avaliação da Fluência e Precisão da Leitura. EDIPSICO—Edições e investigação em psicologia, Lda. [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho, A., and M. Pereira. 2009. O Rei—Um Teste para Avaliação da Fluência e Precisão da Leitura no 1° e 2° ciclos do Ensino Básico. Psychologica 51: 283–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Jong, C. G. W. C. G. W., R. Licht, J. A. Sergeant, and J. Oosterlaan. 2012. RD, ADHD, and their comorbidity from a dual route perspective. Child Neuropsychology 18, 5: 467–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DuPaul, G. J., M. J. Gormley, and S. D. Laracy. 2013. Comorbidity of LD and ADHD. Journal of Learning Disabilities 46, 1: 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E. G., B. F. Pennington, R. Boada, J. S. Ogline, R. A. Tunick, N. A. Chhabildas, and R. K. Olson. 2001. A comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 1: 157–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Facoetti, A., A. N. Trussardi, M. Ruffino, M. L. Lorusso, C. Cattaneo, R. Galli, M. Molteni, and M. Zorzi. 2010. Multisensory Spatial Attention Deficits Are Predictive of Phonological Decoding Skills in Developmental Dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, 5: 1011–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Facoetti, A., M. Zorzi, L. Cestnick, M. L. Lorusso, M. Molteni, P. Paganoni, C. Umiltà, and G. G. Mascetti. 2006. The relationship between visuospatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23, 6: 841–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fletcher, J. M. 2009. Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 15, 04: 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frijters, J. C., M. W. Lovett, K. A. Steinbach, M. Wolf, R. A. Sevcik, and R. D. Morris. 2011. Neurocognitive Predictors of Reading Outcomes for Children With Reading Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 44, 2: 150–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fuchs, D., and C. L. Young. 2006. On the Irrelevance of Intelligence in Predicting Responsiveness to Reading Instruction. Exceptional Children 73, 1: 8–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S. E., and A. D. Baddeley. 1990. The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology 81, 4: 439–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S. E., T. P. Alloway, C. Willis, and A.-M. Adams. 2006. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 93, 3: 265–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgiou, G. K., R. Parrila, J. R. Kirby, and K. Stephenson. 2008. Rapid Naming Components and Their Relationship With Phonological Awareness, Orthographic Knowledge, Speed of Processing, and Different Reading Outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading 12, 4: 325–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Germanò, E., A. Gagliano, and P. Curatolo. 2010. Comorbidity of ADHD and Dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology 35, 5: 475–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goff, D. A., C. Pratt, and B. Ong. 2005. The Relations Between Children’s Reading Comprehension, Working Memory, Language Skills and Components of Reading Decoding in a Normal Sample. Reading and Writing 18, 7–9: 583–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gooch, D., M. Snowling, and C. Hulme. 2011. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52, 2: 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gustafson, S., and S. Samuelsson. 1999. Intelligence and dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 40, 2: 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoskyn, M., and H. L. Swanson. 2000. Cognitive processing of low achievers and children with reading disabilities: A selective meta-analytic review of the published literature. School Psychology Review 29, 1: 102–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulme, C., K. Goetz, D. Gooch, J. Adams, and M. J. Snowling. 2007. Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 96, 2: 150–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyönä, J., and R. K. Olson. 1995. Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21, 6: 1430–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnston, R. S., and M. Morrison. 2007. Toward a Resolution of Inconsistencies in the Phonological Deficit Theory of Reading Disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities 40, 1: 66–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kasper, L. J., R. M. Alderson, and K. L. Hudec. 2012. Moderators of working memory deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review 32, 7: 605–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kibby, M. Y. 2009. There are multiple contributors to the VSTM Deficit in Children with Developmental Reading Disabilities. Child Neuropsychology 15, 5: 485–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landerl, K., F. Ramus, K. Moll, H. Lyytinen, P. H. T. Leppänen, K. Lohvansuu, M. O’Donovan, J. Williams, J. Bartling, J. Bruder, S. Kunze, N. Neuhoff, D. Tóth, F. Honbolygó, V. Csépe, C. Bogliotti, S. Iannuzzi, Y. Chaix, J.-F. Démonet, and G. Schulte-Körne. 2013. Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54, 6: 686–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liberman, I. Y., and D. Shankweiler. 1985. Phonology and the Problems of Learning to Read and Write. Remedial and Special Education 6, 6: 8–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liberman, I. Y., D. P. Shankweiler, and A. M. Liberman. 1990. The alphabetic principle and learning to read. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research SR-101/102: 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Lovett, M. W., M. De Palma, J. Frijters, K. Steinbach, M. Temple, N. Benson, and L. Lacerenza. 2008. Interventions for Reading Difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities 41, 4: 333–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyon, G. R., S. E. Shaywitz, and B. A. Shaywitz. 2003. A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 53, 1: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marzocchi, G. M., J. Oosterlaan, A. Zuddas, P. Cavolina, H. Geurts, D. Redigolo, C. Vio, and J. A. Sergeant. 2008. Contrasting deficits on executive functions between ADHD and reading disabled children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 5: 543–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mathers, M. E. 2006. Aspects of Language in Children With ADHD: Applying Functional Analyses to Explore Language Use. Journal of Attention Disorders 9, 3: 523–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDougall, J. P., and R. Donohoe. 2002. No Title. Reading and Writing 15, 3/4: 359–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGrath, L. M., B. F. Pennington, M. A. Shanahan, L. E. Santerre-Lemmon, H. D. Barnard, E. G. Willcutt, J. C. DeFries, and R. K. Olson. 2011. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52, 5: 547–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menghini, D., A. Finzi, G. A. Carlesimo, and S. Vicari. 2011. Working Memory Impairment in Children With Developmental Dyslexia: Is it Just a Phonological Deficity? Developmental Neuropsychology 36, 2: 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Metsala, J. L., D. Stavrinos, and A. C. Walley. 2009. Children’s spoken word recognition and contributions to phonological awareness and nonword repetition: A 1-year follow-up. Applied Psycholinguistics 30, 1: 101–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, R. D., M. W. Lovett, M. Wolf, R. A. Sevcik, K. A. Steinbach, J. C. Frijters, and M. B. Shapiro. 2012. Multiple-Component Remediation for Developmental Reading Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 45, 2: 99–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moura, O., J. Moreno, M. Pereira, and M. R. Simões. 2015. Developmental Dyslexia and Phonological Processing in European Portuguese Orthography. Dyslexia 21, 1: 60–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moura, O., M. Pereira, C. Alfaiate, E. Fernandes, B. Fernandes, S. Nogueira, J. Moreno, and M. R. Simões. 2017. Neurocognitive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Multiple deficits and diagnostic accuracy. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 39, 3: 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moura, O., M. R. Simões, and M. Pereira. 2014. Executive Functioning in Children With Developmental Dyslexia. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 28, sup1: 20–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moura, O., M. R. Simóes, and M. Pereira. 2014. WISC-III cognitive profiles in children with developmental dyslexia: Specific cognitive disability and diagnostic utility. Dyslexia 20, 1: 19–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moura, O., M. R. Simões, and M. Pereira. 2015. Working Memory in Portuguese Children With Developmental Dyslexia. Applied Neuropsychology: Child 4, 4: 237–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, J., G. J. Benner, and J. Gonzalez. 2003. Learner Characteristics that Influence the Treatment Effectiveness of Early Literacy Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice 18, 4: 255–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nevo, E., and Z. Breznitz. 2011. Assessment of working memory components at 6years of age as predictors of reading achievements a year later. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 109, 1: 73–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panagiotidi, M., P. Overton, and T. Stafford. 2017. Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits. Journal of Eye Movement Research 10, 1: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parrila, R., J. R. Kirby, and L. McQuarrie. 2004. Articulation Rate, Naming Speed, Verbal Short-Term Memory, and Phonological Awareness: Longitudinal Predictors of Early Reading Development? Scientific Studies of Reading 8, 1: 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B. F. 2006. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 101, 2: 385–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B. F., L. Santerre-Lemmon, J. Rosenberg, B. MacDonald, R. Boada, A. Friend, D. R. Leopold, S. Samuelsson, B. Byrne, E. G. Willcutt, and R. K. Olson. 2012. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121, 1: 212–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pereira, N., M. A. Costa, and M. Guerreiro. 2022. Effects of word length and word frequency among dyslexic, ADHD-I and typical readers. Journal of Eye Movement Research 15, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez, T. M., S. Majerus, and M. Poncelet. 2012. The contribution of short-term memory for serial order to early reading acquisition: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 111, 4: 708–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, C., J. C. Ziegler, and M. Zorzi. 2007. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review 114, 2: 273–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, S. J. 2004. Edited by M. Turner and J. Rack. Verbal memory in the learning of literacy. In The study of dyslexia. Kluwer Academic: pp. 131–156. [Google Scholar]
- Ramus, F., C. R. C. R. Marshall, S. Rosen, and H. K. J. J. Van Der Lely. 2013. Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain 136, 2: 630–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramus, F., S. Rosen, S. C. Dakin, B. L. Day, J. M. Castellote, S. White, and U. Frith. 2003. Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 126, 4: 841–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, M., and D. Besner. 2006. Reading aloud is not automatic: Processing capacity is required to generate a phonological code from print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32, 1303–1323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, B. A., M. M. Martel, and J. T. Nigg. 2017. Are There Executive Dysfunction Subtypes Within ADHD? Journal of Attention Disorders 21, 4: 284–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scarborough, H. S. 1998. Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia 48, 1: 115–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shanahan, M. A., B. F. Pennington, B. E. Yerys, A. Scott, R. Boada, E. G. Willcutt, R. K. Olson, and J. C. DeFries. 2006. Processing Speed Deficits in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Reading Disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34, 5: 584–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegel, L. S. 1989. IQ Is Irrelevant to the Definition of Learning Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 22, 8: 469–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegel, L. S. 1992. An Evaluation of the Discrepancy Definition of Dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities 25, 10: 618–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smyrnakis, I., V. Andreadakis, A. Rina, N. Bοufachrentin, and I. Aslanides. 2021. Silent versus reading out loud modes: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, 2: 1–16. Available online: https://bop.unibe.ch/JEMR/article/view/7769. [CrossRef]
- Soares, A. P., J. C. Medeiros, A. Simões, J. Machado, A. Costa, Á. Iriarte, J. J. de Almeida, A. P. Pinheiro, and M. Comesaña. 2014. ESCOLEX: A grade-level lexical database from European Portuguese elementary to middle school textbooks. Behavior Research Methods 46, 1: 240–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinbrink, C., and M. Klatte. 2008. Phonological working memory in German children with poor reading and spelling abilities. Dyslexia 14, 4: 271–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stuebing, K. K., J. M. Fletcher, J. M. LeDoux, G. R. Lyon, S. E. Shaywitz, and B. A. Shaywitz. 2002. Validity of IQ-Discrepancy Classifications of Reading Disabilities: A Meta-Analysis. American Educational Research Journal 39, 2: 469–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sucena, A., and S. L. Castro. 2011. ALEPE—Bateria e Avaliação da Leitura em Português Europeu. CEGOC-TEA Edições.
- Swanson, H. L., and M. H. Ashbaker. 2000. Working memory, short-term memory, speech rate, word recognition and reading comprehension in learning disabled readers: Does the executive system have a role?11The research was supported by Peloy Endowment Funds awarded by the first author. This work is. Intelligence 28, 1: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swanson, H. L., and O. Jerman. 2007. The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 96, 4: 249–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swanson, H. L., X. Zheng, and O. Jerman. 2009. Working Memory, Short-Term Memory, and Reading Disabilities: A Selective Meta-Analysis of the Literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities 42, 3: 260–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takács, Á., A. Kóbor, Z. Tárnok, and V. Csépe. 2014. Verbal fluency in children with ADHD: Strategy using and temporal properties. Child Neuropsychology 20, 4: 415–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Test, P., and G. Bubble. 2012. BeGaze Manual Table of Contents How to Read this Document, July.
- Kibby, M. Y., and M. J. Cohen. 2008. Memory Functioning in Children with Reading Disabilities and/or Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical Investigation of their Working Memory and Long-Term Memory Functioning. Child Neuropsychology 14, 6: 525–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tiu, R. D., L. A. Thompson, and B. A. Lewis. 2003. The Role of IQ in a Component Model of Reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities 36, 5: 424–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tobia, V., and G. M. Marzocchi. 2014. Cognitive Profiles of Italian Children With Developmental Dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly 49, 4: 437–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torgesen, J. K., R. K. Wagner, C. a. Rashotte, S. Burgess, and S. Hecht. 1997. Contributions of Phonological Awareness and Rapid Automatic Naming Ability to the Growth of Word-Reading Skills in Second-to Fifth-Grade Children. Scientific Studies of Reading 1, 2: 161–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varvara, P., C. Varuzza, A. C. P. Sorrentino, S. Vicari, and D. Menghini. 2014. Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vellutino, F. R., J. M. Fletcher, M. J. Snowling, and D. M. Scanlon. 2004. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 45, 1: 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vellutino, F. R., D. M. Scanlon, and G. Reid Lyon. 2000. Differentiating Between Difficult-to-Remediate and Readily Remediated Poor Readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities 33, 3: 223–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, R. K., J. K. Torgesen, and C. A. Rashotte. 1994. Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 30, 1: 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, R. K., J. K. Torgesen, P. Laughon, K. Simmons, and C. A. Rashotte. 1993. Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology 85, 1: 83–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wassenberg, R., J. G. M. Hendriksen, P. P. M. Hurks, F. J. M. Feron, J. S. H. Vles, and J. Jolles. 2010. Speed of Language Comprehension is Impaired in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 13, 4: 374–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. 1991. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (WISC-III): Manual. The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. 2003. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III)—Portuguese Version (M. R. Simões, A. M. Rocha, and C. Ferreira). CEGOC-TEA Edições. [Google Scholar]
- Willcutt, E. G., R. S. Betjemann, L. M. Mcgrath, and B. F. Pennington. 2011. Etiology and neurophysiology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple deficit models. Cortex 46, 10: 1345–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E. G., R. S. Betjemann, L. M. McGrath, N. A. Chhabildas, R. K. Olson, J. C. DeFries, and B. F. Pennington. 2010. Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex 46, 10: 1345–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E. G., B. F. Pennington, R. K. Olson, N. Chhabildas, and J. Hulslander. 2005. Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 1: 35–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M., P. G. Bowers, and K. Biddle. 2000. Naming-Speed Processes, Timing, and Reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities 33, 4: 387–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M., A. G. O’Rourke, C. Gidney, M. Lovett, P. Cirino, and R. Morris. 2002. The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing 15, 1–2: 43–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziegler, J. C., and U. Goswami. 2005. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin 131, 1: 3–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziegler, J. C., D. Bertrand, D. Tóth, V. Csépe, A. Reis, L. Faísca, N. Saine, H. Lyytinen, A. Vaessen, and L. Blomert. 2010. Orthographic Depth and Its Impact on Universal Predictors of Reading. Psychological Science 21, 4: 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Measures | Control ni = 19 Mean SD | Dyslexia ni = 21 Mean SD | ADHD-I ni = 19 Mean SD | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 9.26 | 0.15 | 8.95 | 0.12 | 9.05 | 0.18 |
Verbal IQ | 102.8 | 3.7 | 98.9 | 3.0 | 83.5 | 2.9 |
Performance IQ | 103.0 | 4.5 | 98.2 | 2.4 | 80.9 | 1.9 |
Full-Scale IQ | 102.5 | 4.0 | 97.5 | 2.5 | 78.5 | 1.9 |
Sex | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control ni | Dyslexia ni | ADHD-I ni | |||
Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female |
4 | 15 | 8 | 11 | 11 | 10 |
Stimuli | ||
---|---|---|
Length | Short (S) | [4–6] Letters |
Medium (M) | [7–10] Letters | |
Long (L) | [11–14] Letters | |
Frequency | Low (LF) | 0–1000 Token |
Medium (MF) | 1001–10,000 Token | |
Length × Frequency | (S + LF) | Corais (corals) |
(S + MF) | Equipa (team) | |
(M + LF) | Marinhas (marine) | |
(M + MF) | Conhecer (to know) | |
(L + LF) | Mergulhadores (sea divers) | |
(L + MF) | Investigação (research) |
Measures | Groups | Mean (SD) | (Q3-Q1) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal IQ | Control | 102.85 (13.20) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.000) 1,2 Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.002) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 98.89 (13.14) | |||
ADHD-I | 83.45 (13.02) | |||
Performance IQ | Control | 106.00 (115.50–87.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.000) 5 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 95.00 (105.00–90.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 79.50 (89.00–73.00) | |||
Full IQ | Control | 98.00 (117.00–89.50) | ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.000) 5 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 98.00 (105.00–88.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 78.00 (84.75–74.25) | |||
Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) | Control | 104.08 (13.43) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.000) 1,2 Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.001) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 100.58 (11.72) | |||
ADHD-I | 84.50 (13.93) | |||
Perceptual Organization Index (VSI) | Control | 106.00 (118.00–91.50) | ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.009) 5 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.001) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 98.00 (103.00–86.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 87.00 (91.00–72.00) | |||
Processing Speed Index (PSI) | Control | 102.31 (21.67) | Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.002) 3,4 | |
Dyslexia | 102.68 (13.78) | |||
ADHD-I | 87.37 (10.89) | |||
Digit Span Total | Control | 10.62 (2.43) | Control ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.023) 1,2 Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.027) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 8.42 (2.06) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.40 (2.26) | |||
Forward Digit Span | Control | 7.62 (1.12) | Control ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.041) 1,2 Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.008) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 6.47 (1.31) | |||
ADHD-I | 6.20 (1.32) | |||
Backwards Digit Span | Control | 5.00 (5.00–4.00) | ||
Dyslexia | 3.00 (5.00–3.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.042) 3 | ||
ADHD-I | 3.00 (4.00–3.00) | |||
Vocabulary | Control | 10.00 (14.00–9.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.000) 5 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 10.00 (12.00–9.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 6.00 (8.00–5.00) | |||
Information | Control | 9.00 (11.00–7.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.036) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 8.00 (11.00–6.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 6.00 (9.00–6.00) | |||
Similarities | Control | 13.00 (14.00–9.50) | ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.036) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 12.00 (13.00–11.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 9.00 (11.50–8.00) | |||
Comprehension | Control | 10.46 (2.47) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 9.94 (1.95) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.39 (2.45) | |||
Block design | Control | 10.92 (2.53) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.000) 1,2 Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.000) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 9.59 (2.15) | |||
ADHD-I | 6.44 (2.75) | |||
Object assembly | Control | 9.00 (11.50–7.50) | ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.041) 5 ADHD-I ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.006) 5 | |
Dyslexia | 9.00 (10.00–8.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.50 (9.00–4.25) | |||
Pictures completion | Control | 11. 08 (3.20) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.012) 1,2 | |
Dyslexia | 10. 24 (2.73) | |||
ADHD-I | 8.56 (2.41) | |||
Picture arrangement | Control | 9.92 (3.30) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 10.47 (3.20) | |||
ADHD-I | 8.72 (2.08) | |||
Coding | Control | 11.08 (3.88) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.019) 3,4 Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.015) 3,4 | |
Dyslexia | 9.82 (2.63) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.39 (2.25) | |||
Symbol search | Control | 9.77 (4.38) | Dyslexia ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.003) 3,4 | |
Dyslexia | 11.53 (2.94) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.89 (2.14) | |||
Arithmetic | Control | 9.85 (3.11) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 9.18 (2.83) | |||
ADHD-I | 8.17 (1.98) | |||
Mazes | Control | 11.31 (2.98) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 11.56 (2.28) | |||
ADHD-I | 9.68 (3.16) |
Measures | Groups | Mean (SD) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|
Control | 5.85 (0.38) | ||
CV | Dyslexia | 5.59 (0.62) | |
ADHD-I | 5.14 (1.70) | ||
Control | 4.00 (0.00) | ||
nCV | Dyslexia | 3.94 (0.24) | |
ADHD-I | 3.64 (1.08) | ||
Control | 9.85 (0.38) | ||
Total_CV | Dyslexia | 9.53 (0.62) | |
ADHD-I | 8.79 (2.67) | ||
Control | 5.23 (0.93) | n.s. | |
CVC | Dyslexia | 4.94 (1.14) | |
ADHD-I | 4.29 (2.20) | ||
Control | 4.00 (0.00) | ||
nCVC | Dyslexia | 3.88 (0.33) | |
ADHD-I | 3.57 (1.09) | ||
Control | 9.23 (0.93) | ||
Total_CVC | Dyslexia | 8.82 (1.24) | |
ADHD-I | 7.86 (2.96) | ||
Control | 19.08 (1.19) | ||
Total Sum | Dyslexia | 18.35 (1.66) | |
ADHD-I | 16.64 (5.24) |
Measures | Groups | Mean (SD) | (Q3–Q1) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|
Control | 4.62 (1.33) | n.s. | ||
CV | Dyslexia | 4.53 (1.46) | ||
ADHD-I | 4.07 (1.64) | |||
nCV | Control | 4.00 (0.00) | ||
Dyslexia | 3.35 (0.86) | |||
ADHD-I | 3.29 (1.14) | |||
Control | 8.62 (1.33) | |||
Total_CV (CV plus nCV) | Dyslexia | 7.88 (1.54) | ||
ADHD-I | 7.36 (2.65) | |||
Control | 4.85 (1.46) | |||
CVC | Dyslexia | 5.29 (1.31) | ||
ADHD-I | 4.36 (1.55) | |||
Control | a | Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.025) 1 | ||
nCVC | Dyslexia | 4.00 (4.00–3.00) | ||
ADHD-I | 4.00 (4.00–3.00) | |||
Total_CVC (CVC plus nCVC) | Control | 8.77 (1.42) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 8.76 (1.56) | |||
ADHD-I | 7.36 (2.59) | |||
Control | 17.38 (2.40) | |||
Total sum | Dyslexia | 16.71 (2.59) | ||
ADHD-I | 13.86 (5.42) |
Measures | Groups | Mean (SD) | (Q3–Q1) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|
Control | 96.15 (5.23) | n.s. | ||
Percentage of simple words read correctly | Dyslexia | 88.64 (17.09) | ||
ADHD-I | 85.83 (15.86) | |||
Control | 78.85 (16.88) | |||
Percentage of inconsistent words read correctly | Dyslexia | 65.91 (24.85) | ||
ADHD-I | 60.83 (10.43) | |||
Percentile of consistent words read correctly | Control | 99.00 (99.00–99.00) | Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.049) 1 | |
Dyslexia | 25.00 (99.00–10.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 25.00 (99.00–10.00) | |||
Percentile of total words read correctly(Sum of simple, consistent and inconsistent words) | Control | 60.00 (92.50–25.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.023) 1 | |
Dyslexia | 25.00 (60.00–5.00) | |||
ADHD-I | 17.50 (40.00–4.00) | |||
Mean reaction time/ms—simple words | Control | 1237.69 (253.29) | n.s. | |
Dyslexia | 1350.27 (455.25) | |||
ADHD-I | 1236.70 (499.29) | |||
Control | 1267.00 (351.88) | |||
Mean reaction time/ms—consistent words | Dyslexia | 1382.64 (520.46) | ||
ADHD-I | 1223.00 (448.92) | |||
Control | 1629.08 (665.52) | |||
Mean reaction time/ms—inconsistent words | Dyslexia | 1857.09 (1117.02) | ||
ADHD-I | 1741.10 (765.33) | |||
Sum of mean reaction times/ms | Control | 1377.85 (403.77) | ||
Dyslexia | 1530.00 (679.91) | |||
ADHD-I | 1400.20 (533.97) |
Measures | Groups | Mean (SD) | Multiple Measures |
---|---|---|---|
Control | 22.85 (0.38) | n.s. | |
Uppercase letter reading | Dyslexia | 21.64 (2.98) | |
ADHD-I | 22.60 (0.52) | ||
Control | 85.31 (33.42) | ||
Percentile of uppercase letter reading | Dyslexia | 64.18 (48.37) | |
ADHD-I | 63.40 (45.96) |
Measures | Groups | M (SD) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|
Literal comprehension | Control | 6.93 (2.09) | |
Dyslexia | 4.73 (2.12) | Control ≠ Dyslexia (p = 0.026) 1,2 | |
ADHD-I | 5.50 (2.31) | ||
Inferential comprehension | Control | 4.71 (1.77) | n.s. |
Dyslexia | 4.33 (2.29) | ||
ADHD-I | 3.38 (1.54) | ||
Critical comprehension | Control | 1.07 (0.62) | |
Dyslexia | 1.27 (0.70) | ||
ADHD-I | 0.81 (0.75) | ||
Reorganization | Control | 2.71 (0.99) | |
Dyslexia | 1.60 (1.30) | ||
ADHD-I | 2.13 (1.20) | ||
Total result | Control | 16.07 (5.08) | |
Dyslexia | 11.93 (4.85) | Control ≠ ADHD-I (p = 0.033) 1,2 | |
ADHD-I | 11.50 (4.75) |
Measures | Groups | (Q3–Q1) | Multiple Comparisons |
---|---|---|---|
Control | 111.00 (125.00–99.50) | Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 | |
Correct words read in 60″ | Dyslexia | 71.00 (80.00–50.50) | |
ADHD-I | 77.00 (85.00–46.50) | ||
Control | 270.00 (274.50–265.50) | Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.001) 1 | |
Correct words read 180″ | Dyslexia | 179.00 (227.50–123.50) | |
ADHD-I | 177.00 (242.50–111.50) | ||
Control | 153.00 (176.50–133.00) | ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.001) 1 Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 | |
Total reading time/seconds | Dyslexia | 256.00 (381.50–211.00) | |
ADHD-I | 235.00 (399.50–186.50) | ||
Control | 90.00 (91.50–88.50) | Dyslexia ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 ADHD-I ≠ Control (p = 0.000) 1 | |
Fluency index—reading speed | Dyslexia | 59.67 (75.83–41.17) | |
ADHD-I | 59.00 (80.83–37.17) |
Model Fitting Criteria | Coefficients | Classification | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Likelihood Ratio Tests | B | Sig. | ÔR | % Correct | |||||
X2 | df | Sig. | Dyslexia | Intercept | 5.558 | 1.523 | 0.217 | 81.4 | |
46.77 | 8 | 0.00 | Backwards digit span | −1.331 | 4.700 | 0.030 | 0.264 | ||
Vocabulary | −0.142 | 0.551 | 0.458 | 0.868 | |||||
Coding | −0.255 | 1.193 | 0.275 | 0.775 | |||||
L+LF_FC | 0.107 | 5.641 | 0.018 | 1.113 | |||||
ADHD-I | Intercept | 16.349 | 8.513 | 0.004 | |||||
Backwards digit span | −1.266 | 3.709 | 0.054 | 0.282 | |||||
Vocabulary | −0.702 | 7.059 | 0.008 | 0.496 | |||||
Coding | −0.794 | 5.763 | 0.016 | 0.452 | |||||
L+LF_FC | 0.077 | 2.692 | 0.101 | 1.081 |
Summary Model | ANOVA | Dependent Variable | Predictors | Non-Standardized Coefficients | T | Sig. | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | |||||||||
R2 | Durbin-Watson | F | Sig. | Number of correct words read in 60″. | (Constant) | 142.467 | 11.823 | 0.000 | |
0.678 | 0.641 | 2.475 | 17.933 | 0.000 | DiagRecDis | −43.781 | −6.435 | 0.000 | |
DiagRecDA | −42.939 | −6.012 | 0.000 | ||||||
Picture Completion | −2.305 | −2.355 | 0.024 | ||||||
M+MF_TFT | −0.001 | −2.192 | 0.035 |
Summary Model | ANOVA | Dependent Variable | Predictors | Non-Standardized Coefficients | T | Sig. | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | |||||||||
R2 | Durbin- Watson | F | Sig. | Number of correct words read in 180 s | (Constant) | 273.858 | 19.951 | 0.000 | |
DiagRecDis | −66.527 | −3.403 | 0.002 | ||||||
0.520 | 0.479 | 2.002 | 12.650 | 0.000 | DiagRecDA | −50.599 | −2.484 | 0.018 | |
S+MF_SPRT | −0.015 | −3.013 | 0.005 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pereira, N.; Costa, M.A.; Guerreiro, M. Integrating Cognitive Factors and Eye Movement Data in Reading Predictive Models for Children with Dyslexia and ADHD-I. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2023, 16, 1-23. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.16.4.6
Pereira N, Costa MA, Guerreiro M. Integrating Cognitive Factors and Eye Movement Data in Reading Predictive Models for Children with Dyslexia and ADHD-I. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2023; 16(4):1-23. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.16.4.6
Chicago/Turabian StylePereira, Norberto, Maria Armanda Costa, and Manuela Guerreiro. 2023. "Integrating Cognitive Factors and Eye Movement Data in Reading Predictive Models for Children with Dyslexia and ADHD-I" Journal of Eye Movement Research 16, no. 4: 1-23. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.16.4.6
APA StylePereira, N., Costa, M. A., & Guerreiro, M. (2023). Integrating Cognitive Factors and Eye Movement Data in Reading Predictive Models for Children with Dyslexia and ADHD-I. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 16(4), 1-23. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.16.4.6