Visual Direction

A special issue of Vision (ISSN 2411-5150).

Deadline for manuscript submissions: closed (30 September 2018) | Viewed by 13219

Special Issue Editors


E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Interests: visual direction; motion parallax; history of visual sciences

E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Interests: visual direction; eye movements; eye-hand coordination

Special Issue Information

Dear Colleagues,

In this Special Issue on “Visual Direction”, we would cover any topics related to Visual Direction: A historical review, a review of experimental work from the past 50 years, new experimental work, theoretical analysis of mechanisms for visual direction, short commentaries, a retrospective of published work.

Subjects may include the following:

  • Auditory and visual direction
  • Concept of the visual direction center (reference point, cyclopean eye, egocenter, binoculus, etc.)
  • Cyclopean illusion
  • Eye dominance and the visual direction center
  • Factors affecting visual direction
  • Laws, rules or principles of visual direction
  • Location of the visual direction center
  • Ocular parallax
  • Processing visual direction with depth perception
  • Relative and absolute visual direction
  • Single and double vision
  • The effect of phoria
  • Visual direction during and/or around the time of eye movements
  • Visual direction of disparate images
  • Visual direction within monocular zones

Prof. Dr. Hiroshi Ono
Prof. Dr. Alistair P. Mapp
Guest Editors

Manuscript Submission Information

Manuscripts should be submitted online at www.mdpi.com by registering and logging in to this website. Once you are registered, click here to go to the submission form. Manuscripts can be submitted until the deadline. All submissions that pass pre-check are peer-reviewed. Accepted papers will be published continuously in the journal (as soon as accepted) and will be listed together on the special issue website. Research articles, review articles as well as short communications are invited. For planned papers, a title and short abstract (about 100 words) can be sent to the Editorial Office for announcement on this website.

Submitted manuscripts should not have been published previously, nor be under consideration for publication elsewhere (except conference proceedings papers). All manuscripts are thoroughly refereed through a single-blind peer-review process. A guide for authors and other relevant information for submission of manuscripts is available on the Instructions for Authors page. Vision is an international peer-reviewed open access quarterly journal published by MDPI.

Please visit the Instructions for Authors page before submitting a manuscript. The Article Processing Charge (APC) for publication in this open access journal is 1600 CHF (Swiss Francs). Submitted papers should be well formatted and use good English. Authors may use MDPI's English editing service prior to publication or during author revisions.

Benefits of Publishing in a Special Issue

  • Ease of navigation: Grouping papers by topic helps scholars navigate broad scope journals more efficiently.
  • Greater discoverability: Special Issues support the reach and impact of scientific research. Articles in Special Issues are more discoverable and cited more frequently.
  • Expansion of research network: Special Issues facilitate connections among authors, fostering scientific collaborations.
  • External promotion: Articles in Special Issues are often promoted through the journal's social media, increasing their visibility.
  • e-Book format: Special Issues with more than 10 articles can be published as dedicated e-books, ensuring wide and rapid dissemination.

Further information on MDPI's Special Issue polices can be found here.

Published Papers (3 papers)

Order results
Result details
Select all
Export citation of selected articles as:

Research

Jump to: Review

13 pages, 1671 KiB  
Article
Slant of a Surface Shifts Binocular Visual Direction
by Tsutomu Kusano and Koichi Shimono
Vision 2018, 2(2), 20; https://doi.org/10.3390/vision2020020 - 6 May 2018
Viewed by 3972
Abstract
We demonstrate how the slant of a surface affects the relative visual direction between binocular stimuli. In two experiments, we measured the visual direction of a binocular stimulus at different distances in the mid-sagittal plane or in the transverse plane at eye level [...] Read more.
We demonstrate how the slant of a surface affects the relative visual direction between binocular stimuli. In two experiments, we measured the visual direction of a binocular stimulus at different distances in the mid-sagittal plane or in the transverse plane at eye level relative to the center of the stimulus field. Experiment 1 showed that when a binocular stimulus (a vertical bar) was presented in front of or behind a surface slanted along the vertical center of the surface, its visual direction shifted toward the surface. Experiment 2 showed that when a binocular stimulus (a horizontal bar) was presented in front of or behind a surface slanted along the horizontal center of the surface, its visual direction also shifted toward the surface. These results indicate that the slant of a surface should be listed among the variables that contribute to the binocular visual direction, as well as the retinal loci of the stimulus, binocular eye position, the location of the visual egocenter, and stimulus properties. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Visual Direction)
Show Figures

Graphical abstract

10 pages, 4353 KiB  
Article
Differential Angular Expansion in Perceived Direction in Azimuth and Elevation Are Yoked to the Presence of a Perceived Ground Plane
by Frank H. Durgin and Umi I. Keezing
Vision 2018, 2(2), 17; https://doi.org/10.3390/vision2020017 - 24 Mar 2018
Cited by 2 | Viewed by 3935
Abstract
It has been proposed that perceived angular direction relative to straight-ahead is exaggerated in perception, and that this exaggeration is greater in elevation (or declination) than in azimuth. Prior research has suggested that exaggerations in elevation may be tied to the presence of [...] Read more.
It has been proposed that perceived angular direction relative to straight-ahead is exaggerated in perception, and that this exaggeration is greater in elevation (or declination) than in azimuth. Prior research has suggested that exaggerations in elevation may be tied to the presence of a visual ground plane, but there have been mixed results across studies using different methods of dissociation. In the present study, virtual environments were used to dissociate visual from gravitational upright while human participants (N = 128) made explicit angular direction judgments relative to straight ahead. Across these experimental manipulations, observers were positioned either upright (Experiments 1A and 1B) or sideways (Experiment 2), so as to additionally dissociate bodily orientation from gravitational orientation. In conditions in which a virtual environment was perceived as containing a level ground plane, large-scale exaggerations consistent with the visually-specified orientation of the ground plane were observed. In the absence of the perception of a level ground plane, angular exaggerations were relatively small. The ground plane serves as an important reference frame for angular expansion in the perceived visual direction. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Visual Direction)
Show Figures

Graphical abstract

Review

Jump to: Research

15 pages, 4164 KiB  
Review
Early Studies of Binocular and Binaural Directions
by Nicholas J. Wade
Vision 2018, 2(1), 13; https://doi.org/10.3390/vision2010013 - 1 Mar 2018
Cited by 1 | Viewed by 4465
Abstract
Understanding how the eyes work together to determine the direction of objects provided the impetus for examining integration of signals from the ears to locate sounds. However, the advantages of having two eyes were recorded long before those for two ears were appreciated. [...] Read more.
Understanding how the eyes work together to determine the direction of objects provided the impetus for examining integration of signals from the ears to locate sounds. However, the advantages of having two eyes were recorded long before those for two ears were appreciated. In part, this reflects the marked differences in how we can compare perception with one or two organs. It is easier to close one eye and examine monocular vision than to “close” one ear and study monaural hearing. Moreover, we can move our eyes either in the same or in opposite directions, but humans have no equivalent means of moving the ears in unison. Studies of binocular single vision can be traced back over two thousand years and they were implicitly concerned with visual directions from each eye. The location of any point in visual or auditory space can be described by specifying its direction and distance, from the vantage point of an observer. From the late 18th century experiments indicated that binocular direction involved an eye movement component and experimental studies of binaural direction commenced slightly later. However, these early binocular and binaural experiments were not incorporated into theoretical accounts until almost a century later. The early history of research on visual direction with two eyes is contrasted to that on auditory direction with two ears. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Visual Direction)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Back to TopTop