Next Article in Journal
Exploring Progression: A Case Study on Student Performance Using the National Clinical Assessment Tool in Emergency Medicine
Previous Article in Journal
Impact of Global Health Scholarship Programs in the Faculty of Medicine at Mbarara University of Science and Technology
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Validity Evidence for Using the Situational Motivation Scale to Assess Pre-Clerkship Medical Student Motivation

1
Frank H. Netter MD School of Medicine, Quinnipiac University, Hamden, CT 06518, USA
2
The University of Connecticut Health Center, University of Connecticut, Farmington, CT 06030, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. Med. Educ. 2024, 3(3), 212-228; https://doi.org/10.3390/ime3030018
Submission received: 20 May 2024 / Revised: 19 June 2024 / Accepted: 21 June 2024 / Published: 26 June 2024

Abstract

:
Motivation is essential in education, with highly motivated learners engaging more deeply with content and more ably transferring knowledge to new contexts. However, the validity of scales to measure motivation has been underexplored in pre-clerkship medical education. This study evaluates the validity of the Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS) for measuring motivation among pre-clerkship medical students in post-situational and short-term contexts. Using a sample of n = 156 students from the Frank H. Netter MD School of Medicine at Quinnipiac University, SIMS was tested to construct validity, with attention paid to content, response process, internal structure, relationships to other variables, and consequences of use evidence. Small modifications from present to past tense in English were made for clarity following focus-group feedback, and content validity was ensured via expert consultation. The SIMS demonstrated strong internal consistency, with a satisfactory Cronbach’s alpha for all subscales and anticipated patterns of correlations. The factor analysis confirmed appropriate factor loadings, with a stronger model fit for the short-term context, and no observed adverse effects on student engagement. These findings support the robustness of the SIMS in capturing intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in pre-clerkship medical students, highlighting its applicability for short-term and situational motivational assessment.

1. Introduction

Medical school begins the challenging journey of equipping students for the demanding profession of being a physician, a career where the stakes are high—involving people’s lives and well-being. As such, it requires discipline, perseverance, and motivation. The latter is crucial for medical students because of the rigorous and demanding nature of medical school curricula. Put simply, motivation is the general desire or willingness of someone to do something [1]. It energizes, directs, and sustains behavior.
Self-determination theory (SDT) is a well-established framework in the field of educational psychology and is regarded as the macro theory of human motivation [2]. SDT’s application across diverse fields, like education, healthcare, and organizational behavior, has refined its understanding and validated its concepts in various contexts [3]. While SDT continues to evolve, incorporating new insights from recent research, the current theoretical background of SDT continues to underscore the critical role of its three foundational principles—autonomy, competence, and relatedness—in fostering optimal motivation [4,5]. SDT posits that motivation is the fundamental reason underlying individuals’ thoughts, actions, and behaviors [6,7,8]. Motivation is of particular concern in education and is something that should be paid attention to from a learning-design and curriculum-evaluation perspective because, when learners are motivated, they try harder to understand, they process information at a deeper level, and they are more apt to apply what they have learned to novel situations [9]. In the context of well-being and burnout, students embarking on their medical-school journey in the United States initially exhibit lower levels of burnout, diminished rates of depression, and an enhanced quality of life compared to their peers graduating from college into different professions. However, this trend inverts by the second year of medical school. At this point, burnout—characterized by emotional exhaustion, heightened stress, a reduced sense of personal achievement, and thoughts of suicide—intensifies, reaching its zenith during the residency period [10,11,12]. The expectations placed upon medical students are continually increasing, which amplifies the imperative to support them in developing self-help strategies [13,14,15,16]. Being able to authentically measure and track motivation should assist medical educators and curriculum designers with this.
How can we measure motivation? To answer that question, we have to appreciate that motivation is a construct. Constructs are conceptual ideas or abstractions used to facilitate an understanding of something intangible [17]. All scientific fields are founded upon frameworks consisting of constructs and the interplay between them. The natural sciences employ constructs like gravity, phylogenetic dominance, and tectonic pressure. Similarly, the behavioral sciences utilize constructs such as conscientiousness, sportsmanship, and resilience. Motivation, like these, is a real albeit abstract human experience. Researchers often use questionnaires with multiple items to measure behavioral constructs. Item responses are added up to a score, and it is hypothesized that this score represents a person’s position on the construct. Behavioral constructs differ in regard to the ease with which they can be measured. Some are straightforward enough to be evaluated with just a single question or a few inquiries. In contrast, others are multifaceted and complex, comprising various dimensions unified by a central theme that collectively defines the construct. Such constructs may necessitate an extensive series of questions to be fully operationalized [18].
Motivation is a complex construct that contains four dimensions: intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, external regulation, and amotivation [6]. SDT perceives these motivational dimensions as existing on a continuum, ranging from self-directed (autonomous) to those driven by external forces [6,7,8,9,10] (Figure 1). In the context of SDT, “regulation” refers to the processes that govern why and how a person initiates and maintains a particular behavior over time, especially in the face of challenges and obstacles. Intrinsically motivated students learn because they derive pleasure and satisfaction, or get a sense of accomplishment, from learning. They enjoy engaging in the behavior for its own sake (e.g., “I enjoy running; it makes me feel good”). Identified regulation happens when an individual acknowledges the value and importance of a behavior, even if it does not provide enjoyment or immediate gratification (e.g., “I run because I know running is good for me”). External regulation arises when behavior is motivated by the pursuit of rewards or the avoidance of adverse outcomes, with the learner feeling compelled to act in a certain manner due to external pressures (e.g., “I run because I will be given a race medal”). Amotivation represents a total lack of motivation, characterized by a learner’s reluctance to engage or exert effort, often accompanied by an absence of purpose or anticipation of reward. This state is akin to learned helplessness, where individuals are beset by a sense of ineffectuality and perceive no opportunity to alter the trajectory of events (e.g., “I’ve stopped running. I can’t see the point anymore—it just tires me out”). These four dimensions can and do vary from person to person over time. Educators aim for medical students to be guided by intrinsic motivation and identified regulation instead of relying on external rewards and punishments or succumbing to a lack of motivation.
The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS) is an instrument informed by self-determination theory (SDT) to measure motivation [19]. It was developed in 2000 by Frederic Guay and colleagues, who published a series of studies that support its use within a French-Canadian college context (in Quebec students may enroll in a two-year college program between high school and university) [19] and has since been used by many researchers to study academic motivation including within the biomedical sciences [20]. It consists of 16 questions (Appendix A), 4 for each of the four dimensions that make up the construct of motivation. It was designed to be used situationally in order to capture a measure of student motivation in situ at one point in time.
In the United States and Canada, the medical school operates as a graduate-level program. Pre-clerkship curricula often blend classroom instruction in integrated basic sciences with early practical exposure to real patients and the practice of medicine. Therefore, pre-clerkship medical students have to sustain a desire or willingness (i.e., be motivated) to participate authentically during learning events, as well as to study further and pursue mastery after learning events conclude [21]. These two sets of circumstances represent two different but related contexts: the former, “situational”; and the latter, “short-term”. However, interrupting educational events to have learners complete in situ the 16-items that make up SIMS would be disruptive and impractical for educators or curriculum designers to sustain on a routine basis; Guay et al. interrupted the French-Canadian students while they were studying alone in a college library. A “post-situational” assessment (i.e., proximal to the conclusion of a learning event) is more realistic for medical students. We define the post-situational context to mean within an hour of a learning event ending and the short-term context to mean later that day or within a week of a learning event ending. Since measuring motivation in these contexts represents a novel use of the SIMS instrument, fresh validity evidence to support it must be obtained. Messick (1989) defined validity as “an integrated evaluative judgment of the degree to which empirical evidence and theoretical rationales support the adequacy and appropriateness of inferences and actions based on test scores and other modes of assessment” [22]. In other words, validity refers to data presented to support or refute the interpretation assigned to SIMS scores [23]. Without evidence of validity, the SIMS scores obtained from medical student learners have little or no intrinsic meaning.
Thus, the objective of this study was to answer the question of whether SIMS is supported by validity evidence to measure the true state of affairs with regard to learner motivation in medical student pre-clerkship post-situational and short-term contexts.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Conceptual Approach to Construct Validity Evidence

Current perspectives advocate that all forms of validity be unified within a singular concept of “construct validity”, which refers to the extent to which a score accurately reflects the intended underlying construct [24,25]. Messick recognized five pillars of evidence that underpin construct validity: content, response processes, internal structure, relationships to other variables, and the implications of assessment outcomes (i.e., consequences) [22] (Table 1). It is important to note that, “these are not different types of validity but rather they are categories of evidence that can be collected to support the construct validity of inferences made from instrument scores” [24]. We followed this approach as a scheme to collect and evaluate psychometric data pertaining to SIMS in post-situational and short-term medical student contexts.
Content validity focuses on the construct’s dimensions and how well they are represented [23]. Response-process validity is the extent to which the actions and thought processes of respondents demonstrate that they understand the instrument questions and how to go about answering them the same way as the researchers do [23]. It seeks to identify and eliminate sources of error related to the instrument and how it is administered. Evidence of internal structure seeks to determine whether a group of questions designed to assess the same dimension exhibit higher correlations with one another compared to groups of questions aimed at measuring different dimensions [23]. In other words, do the 4 questions that are trying to measure intrinsic motivation “hang together” better with each other than they do with the other 3 question sets that are not trying to measure intrinsic motivation? Relationship-to-other-variables evidence asks if external correlations (i.e., correlations to entirely different instruments) are confirmatory or counter-confirmatory [23]? And consequences evidence asks, what is the impact on respondents of being appraised by the instrument [23]? Could someone who agrees to answer the 16 questions be harmed or substantially benefited in any way? It seeks to identify whether the appeal of any benefits or the fear of any harm could introduce sources of bias into the data collected.

2.2. Study Design and Sample

This single institution validation study invited n = 186 first- and second-year medical students from the Frank H. Netter MD School of Medicine at Quinnipiac University to participate. Participation in the study was voluntary and de-identified.

2.3. Procedures

All participants were asked to what extent they agreed on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all; 7 = completely agree) with the items on the following four survey instruments: Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS), Academic Motivation Scale (AMS), Multidimensional Crowdworker Motivation Scale (MCMS), and Maslach Burnout Inventory Student Survey Exhaustion component (MBI-SSEE) [19,26,27,28,29,30]. The post-situation and short-term versions of these surveys were administered on two separate occasions to all 156 study participants (Appendix B and Appendix C). The 83 first-year medical students completed the four surveys with regard to a two-hour small-group learning event (each small group comprised 8 students + 1 faculty facilitator) on the central nervous system’s sensory systems (i.e., somatosensory, visual, auditory, olfactory, and gustatory) from a post-situational and short-term perspective, whereas the 73 second-year medical students completed them with regard to a two-hour small-group learning event (each small group comprised 8 students + 1 faculty facilitator) on the pathology of kidney disorders from a post-situational and short-term perspective. All versions of the SIMS, MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE were administered as paper forms. Survey packets were marked with anonymous numerical identification. Participants received verbal instructions before the survey administration commenced. Data were entered independently by the authors B.W. and D.M. and then compared to ensure that data entry was accurate.

2.4. Scale Adaptation

To adapt SIMS to both post-situational and short-term contexts, minor grammatical changes were made to both the initial prompt and to the individual items (Table 2). This was performed to convert the language from present- to past-tense English. For example, the prompt, “Why are you engaged in this activity?” became “Why were you engaged in this activity?”; and item 1, “Because I think that the activity is interesting” became “Because I thought that the activity was interesting”.

2.5. Construct Validity—Content Analysis

Four subject-matter experts experienced with psychometric data analysis and familiar with self-determination theory (1 PhD statistician and professor of medical sciences, 1 PhD educator and professor of learning sciences, 1 PhD educator and associate dean of medical education, and 1 EdD educator and assistant professor of education) reviewed Guay et al. (2000)’s original content validity evidence and our grammatical edits to determine if they represented substantial content changes [19].

2.6. Construct Validity—Response Process Analysis

Six focus groups were held, with n = 7 medical students per session, to review what they understood the 16 SIMS questions to mean and to evaluate the extent to which our proposed survey administration process could be confusing to respondents. This was performed in order to maximize response integrity and avoid data collected being skewed or in error because questions were misunderstood or instructions on how to fill out the SIMS survey were unclear or misleading. Standardized survey administration and data collection procedures were finalized at the conclusion of this aspect of the study.

2.7. Construct Validity—Internal Structure Analysis

The internal structure of the SIMS instrument for both post-situational and short-term contexts was assessed via four statistical analyses. The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 27) and Mplus (version 8.2) software [29].
Initially, to estimate reliability, Cronbach’s alpha was computed for each of the four dimensions of the motivation construct. Cronbach’s alpha assesses internal consistency by indicating the extent to which a set of items is interrelated. A “good” coefficient of Cronbach’s alpha is generally one that is substantially high, implying that the items consistently measure the same underlying concept or construct. Methodologists frequently suggest a threshold of 0.7 or above as an acceptable value [18,30,31,32].
Secondly, we assessed internal consistency through the calculation of Pearson’s r, analyzing the patterns of correlations. Given that the four dimensions of motivation are situated sequentially along a continuum, the interrelationships among the four item sets of the SIMS should display a simplex pattern. This pattern dictates that items closer to each other on the motivational continuum will exhibit stronger positive correlations than those that are further apart.
Thirdly, to affirm the configurational structure of the 16-item SIMS scale, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted utilizing Mplus (version 8.2). Factor is an alternative term for dimension. Following the statistical analyses reported by Guay (2000) [19], each subset of four questions was loaded onto each of the four factors, and then the Geomin rotation allowed the factors to correlate [33]. Model goodness-of-fit was assessed, and the adequacy of the model’s fit was evaluated using several indices: the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), the ratio of the chi-square to degrees of freedom (χ2/df), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) indices [34]. A χ2/df value of <2.0 [35], a CFI value > 0.95 [36], an RMSEA value < 0.06 [37], and an SRMR value of ≤0.08 [38] were deemed to be indicative of an excellent model fit. The BIC is a criterion for model selection among a set of models [35].
Fourthly, the Wald test assessed the significance of parameter estimates, and modification index (MI) values assessed how a model could be improved by allowing additional paths to be estimated. Modification indices help us answer “what if?” questions about how to improve construct models. Values bigger than 3.84 (p < 0.05) or 10.83 (p < 0.01) indicate how the model could be improved [37].

2.8. Construct Validity—Relationship to Other Variables Analysis

Relationship to other variables was assessed by calculating the Pearson’s r for each combination of factors within the SIMS and the other three instruments (AMS, MCMS, and MBI-SSEE). The patterns of correlational relationships between the theoretical constructs measured in each instrument were then examined for predicted simplex-like patterns. Data analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 27.

2.9. Construct Validity—Relationship to Other Variables Analysis

The subject-matter experts and focus-group participants were asked if they considered a respondent completing or declining to complete the SIMS instrument to have any negative repercussions or to possibly be harmful to learners.

2.10. Ethical Approval

The study received approval from the Quinnipiac University Institutional Review Board (protocol #06617). Informed consent was acquired from all participants in the study.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Of the 186 medical students invited, 156 agreed (84% response rate): 83 first-year students (57% female, 43% male, mean age = 25) and 73 s-year students (49% female, 51% male, mean age = 26). The data from five participants were excluded from the analysis because their surveys were either incomplete, improperly completed (e.g., circling multiple response numbers for a single item), or displayed straightlining (e.g., consistently circling the same response number for all items across all instruments).

3.2. Construct Validity—Content Evidence

The four subject-matter experts concurred that the subtle linguistic modifications from present to past tense did not negatively impact the substance of the SIMS instrument. They affirmed that the items remained appropriately aligned with the conceptual definitions of intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, external regulation, and amotivation.

3.3. Construct Validity—Response Process Evidence

During the focus groups, survey wording and instructions were examined for potential confusion or misunderstandings. Focus-group participants were unanimously comfortable with the format of the proposed procedures for survey administration and data collection. They agreed that survey volume was not onerous, and data collection was not prone to systematic errors. It was confirmed that they understood the 16 SIMS questions to mean the same thing as the study authors B.W. and D.M.

3.4. Construct Validity—Internal Structure Evidence

The internal structure of the SIMS was assessed by examining simplex patterns of internal Pearson’s r correlations and reliability as reflected by Cronbach’s alpha. SIMS Cronbach’s alpha values were 0.98 (IM), 0.98 (IR), 0.95 (ER), and 0.93 (AM) for the post-situational context; and 0.98 (IM), 0.82 (IR). 0.99 (ER), and 0.91 (AM) for the short-term context (Table 3). The internal consistency of SIMS was acceptable given that the lowest Cronbach’s alpha value was 0.82, which exceeds the ≥0.7 minimum criterion [18,30,31,32]. These measures of reliability indicated that questions purporting to measure the same dimension of the motivation construct scored similarly to one another.
The internal Pearson’s r correlations of SIMS followed an ordered simplex pattern in both the short-term and post-situational contexts. Theoretically proximal dimensions (e.g., IM to IR) showed positive correlations, while distal dimensions (e.g., IM to AM) showed negative correlations (Table 4 and Table 5).
Concerning the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) models for the post-situational and short-term context data, all items demonstrated significant loadings on their intended factors in both models (all p-values < 0.01) (Appendix D). Additionally, the strength of these loadings was consistent across the post-situational and short-term models (Table 6 and Table 7).
Correlations between factors were mostly in the low-to-moderate range, supporting the discriminant validity of the factors (Table 6 and Table 7). However, the magnitude of the correlations between factors appears to differ between the post-situational and short-term contexts. The post-situational context model showed a strong correlation between IM and IR (r = 0.69, p < 0.01), while the correlation was smaller for the short-term context model (r = 0.25, p < 0.01). This is not surprising. Medical students engaging in self-directed study in the short-term (e.g., one week after a learning event) might reasonably be expected to exemplify the overlap of intrinsic motivation—driven by genuine interest and satisfaction in learning medicine—and identified regulation, as they recognize and value the importance of this knowledge for their future role as a competent and effective physician. The AM factor significantly correlated with IM (r = −0.30, p < 0.01) and ER (r = 0.28, p < 0.01) in the post-situational context model, but not in the short-term context model (IM: r = −0.12, p = 0.16; ER: r = −0.12, p = 0.21). Furthermore, there was a moderately strong positive correlation between ER and IR in the short-term context model (r = 0.44, p < 0.01), but there was a small negative correlation in the post-situational context model (r = −0.07, p = 0.47).
The model goodness-of-fit indices (Table 8 and Table 9) indicated that the short-term model achieved a good fit (BIC = 8025.6, χ2/df = 1.49, CFI = 0.954, RMSEA= 0.056, SRMR = 0.082), thus supporting the configural structure of the SIMS scale for that context. Meanwhile, the goodness-of-fit indices for the post-situational model suggested room for enhancement in its configurational structure (BIC = 8235.9, χ2/df = 2.5, CFI = 0.894, RMSEA= 0.099, and SRMR = 0.079). In keeping with this, there were two MI suggestions to improve the model fit for the post-situational context. The first MI (24.7) indicated that having SIMS Q1 (an IM question) reassigned to the IR factor would improve the fit. The second MI (10.5) indicated that Q2 (an IR question) might cross-load onto the IM factor. This helps explain the large correlation found between IM and IR in the CFA post-situational context model. The lower BIC value for the short-term model relative to the post-situational model also indicates that SIMS has a better fit for short-term contexts (Table 8 and Table 9).

3.5. Construct Validity—Relationship to Other Variables Evidence

MCMS and AMS are grounded in the same theoretical framework as SIMS (self-determination theory) and also seek to measure IM, IR, ER, and AM, which facilitated direct comparisons between these instruments and SIMS. MBI-SSEE seeks to measure emotional exhaustion, which is similar albeit distinct from amotivation.
The relationships of SIMS variables to the variables of AMC, MCMS, and MBI-SSEE were assessed by examining simplex patterns of internal Pearson’s r correlations and reliability, as reflected by Cronbach’s alpha.
In the post-situational context, Cronbach’s alpha values were 0.93 (IM), 0.98 (IR), 0.89 (ER), and 0.98 (AM) for MCMS; 0.96 (IM), 0.96 (IR), 0.95 (ER), and 0.31 (AM) for AMS; and 0.98 (AM) for MBI-SSEE (Table 10). In the short-term context, Cronbach’s alpha values were 0.98 (IM), 0.96 (IR), 0.88 (ER), and 0.99 (AM) for MCMS; 0.91 (IM), 0.97 (IR), 0.96 (ER), and 0.84 (AM) for AMS; and 0.93 (AM) for MBI-SSEE (Table 11), indicating that the internal consistency of MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE was acceptable given that all but one of the Cronbach’s alpha values exceed the ≥0.7 minimum criterion [18,30,31,32]. The notable exception is for the AM dimension of the AMS scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.31).
The Pearson’s r correlations between SIMS and the MCMS and AMS scales demonstrated simplex trends in both post-situational and short-term contexts. Dimensions of the motivation construct that were theoretically close (e.g., like IM and IR) had positive correlations, whereas those that were farther apart (e.g., like IM and AM) exhibited negative correlations (Table 10 and Table 11). Comparison of SIMS to the MBI-SSEE likewise revealed a pattern of correlation, with IM and IR showing a negative correlation with emotional exhaustion, and ER and AM demonstrating a positive correlation (Table 10 and Table 11).

3.6. Construct Validity—Consequences Evidence

No direct negative repercussions of the SIMS instrument being used for its intended purpose in a medical school context were identified by the authors (B.W. and D.M.), the focus-group participants, or the subject-matter experts. Student participants understood through informed consent that their participation was entirely anonymous and optional, and that participation or lack thereof would have no impact on their progression through and completion of medical school.

4. Discussion

Greater emphasis is being placed on the interplay between student motivation and educational methods across a range of health professions, including medicine [38,39,40,41,42,43]. For example, motivational pedagogy refers to teaching methods and educational practices that aim to enhance students’ intrinsic motivation to learn. This method is based on the conviction that motivation is an essential component of the learning process, and educators can profoundly influence student engagement and success by nurturing an educational atmosphere that encourages motivational growth [7,44]. As an educational strategy, it necessitates a reliable means to measure and track student motivation to ensure that teaching methods are effectively tailored to enhance engagement and learning, allowing for iterative refinements that address individual motivational needs, promote optimal educational outcomes, and reduce the risk of medical student burnout. SIMS is an instrument suited to the measurement motivation within the context for which it was designed [19]. However, when survey instruments such as SIMS are moved to different contexts, the underlying assumptions, population characteristics, validity evidence, and relevance of the construct it was originally designed to measure may no longer hold true. Accordingly, this study sought validity evidence for the use of the SIMS in accurately measuring learner motivation among medical students in pre-clerkship short-term and post-situational contexts.
In this investigation, we evaluated the validity evidence for applying SIMS to gauge medical student motivation across two distinct contexts. The first context, referred to as “post-situational”, adapted the survey’s intended real-time assessment to a retrospectively proximal one, enabling the gathering of information on a student’s motivation after the occurrence of a learning event, rather than during. This approach provides a more feasible alternative for collecting motivational data; it minimizes disruption to the learning experience and allows for more reflective and accurate self-assessment by the respondents of their motivational states, free from the immediate pressures or engagements of the learning activity. The second context, designated as “short-term”, assessed a student’s motivation to study over a one-week period. Medical students must engage in self-directed studying after learning events in order to consolidate their understanding, challenge their mastery of the material, and bridge the gap between theoretical knowledge and practical application, a crucial step in developing the competence and confidence required for clinical practice [45,46].
Expecting perfect validity evidence for a survey instrument is impractical due to several factors: validity is multi-dimensional, and it is influenced by the variability in respondents’ behaviors and the characteristics of the sample used for validation. Additionally, no survey can entirely escape measurement error, and the statistical methods used to assess validity are themselves imperfect. Thus, while high-quality validity evidence is the goal, perfection in validity is an ideal rather than an attainable standard [22,47,48,49]. With this in mind, we gathered a considerable body of construct validity evidence supporting the use of SIMS to measure motivation in pre-clerkship medical students in post-situational and short-term contexts. The content, response process, and consequences evidence were acceptable for both use case scenarios. For relationship-to-other-variables evidence, this was also substantially true. The Pearson’s r correlation values and simplex patterns were acceptable for both contexts, but the post-situational Cronbach’s alpha for the measurement of amotivation (AM) with the AMS survey tool was 0.31, indicating the low internal reliability of the measurement of this dimension of motivation with this particular instrument. This is more of a reflection on AMS in the two medical-student contexts rather than SIMS. Lastly, for internal structure evidence, the Cronbach’s alpha and Pearson’s r correlational data and simplex patterns were acceptable for both use case scenarios, indicating good internal consistency and reliability. All CFA hypothesized factor loadings were found to be significant (p < 0.01) for both contexts. For the short-term context, the CFA model fit indices data were excellent. However, for the post-situational context, the CFA model fit indices data fell short of excellent. χ2/df (χ2/df = 2.52) was higher than the ideal fit value threshold of <2.0, but it was indicative of the model fitting reasonably well; the CFI value (CFI = 0.894) was also below the preferred threshold, suggesting that the model may not fit as well as it could; the RMSEA value (RMSEA = 0.099) was also a bit higher than what we would want for a good fit, whereas the SRMS (SRMR = 0.079) was just at the cutoff of what is generally considered good, suggesting an adequate model fit. In summary, the post-situational CFA data represented a model that had an acceptable fit in some respects (i.e., factor loadings and SRMS), but indicated that it might be worth examining the model to see if it could be improved (i.e., χ2/df, CFI, RMSEA). This was consistent with the MI data for the post-situational context that proposed that one IM question be reassigned to the IR cohort of questions, and another IR question might cross-load onto the IM dimension.
Hence, we can deduce that the SIMS instrument is effective in differentiating among the four dimensions of motivation delineated by SDT when administered to pre-clerkship medical students in a short-term context. The instrument’s application in a post-situational context should be approached more cautiously due to its limited capacity to discriminate between identified regulation (IR) and intrinsic motivation (IM). However, that said, a less-than-ideal CFA model fit in distinguishing between IR and IM within the framework of SDT is not particularly concerning, given their conceptual adjacency and that both represent self-determined forms of motivation [8].

4.1. Analytical Insights and Reflections

The findings of this study underscore the complexity of measuring student motivation accurately in varying educational contexts. While SIMS demonstrates robust validity evidence in a short-term context, the nuanced differences observed in the post-situational context highlight the intricate interplay between motivational constructs and the timing of the assessment. Recent research emphasizes that motivational states are highly dynamic and context-dependent, which can significantly impact the reliability of retrospective self-assessments [50,51].
One critical insight from our study is the challenge of distinguishing between intrinsic motivation (IM) and identified regulation (IR) in post-situational contexts. This aligns with recent findings suggesting that these constructs, while theoretically distinct, often overlap in practical educational settings, particularly when students reflect on their motivation after the fact [52]. The marginal fit indices observed for the CFA model in the post-situational context may reflect this overlap and point to the need for more nuanced tools or methods to capture these subtle differences more effectively.
Moreover, the low internal reliability for amotivation (AM) when measured with the AMS survey tool in our study raises important questions about the tool’s suitability across different contexts. This finding is consistent with recent critiques of the AMS, which have called for revisions to better capture the full spectrum of motivational states, especially in high-stakes educational environments like medical school [53,54]. Future research should explore alternative instruments or hybrid approaches that combine real-time and retrospective assessments to provide a more comprehensive picture of student motivation.
Our study also highlights the importance of considering contextual factors when applying and interpreting motivational assessment tools. As educational environments and learner characteristics evolve, it is crucial to continuously validate and adapt these tools to ensure their relevance and accuracy. This adaptive approach is essential for developing targeted interventions that effectively address the unique motivational needs of medical students, thereby enhancing their learning experiences and outcomes [40].

4.2. Relevance to Medical Education

Current research underscores the vital role of motivation in medical education, emphasizing its impact on academic performance and student well-being. The findings from our study highlight the significant relevance of using tools like the SIMS to accurately measure and enhance student motivation within health professions education. By providing robust evidence for the validity of SIMS in measuring motivation in pre-clerkship medical students, our study contributes to the broader understanding of how motivational assessment can be integrated into educational practices to improve learning outcomes.
The ability to measure motivation reliably in both post-situational and short-term contexts allows educators to tailor interventions more effectively to the needs of medical students. For example, the post-situational context offers a practical approach to gather reflective and accurate self-assessments of motivational states without disrupting the learning experience. This can inform strategies to support students in consolidating their understanding and bridging the gap between theoretical knowledge and practical application, which is essential for developing clinical competence.
Moreover, the short-term context assessment highlights the importance of self-directed study and ongoing engagement with learning materials. Understanding students’ motivation to study over a one-week period provides insights into their commitment to mastering the material, which is critical for their success in both academic and clinical settings.
Integrating motivational pedagogy, which includes using validated tools like SIMS, into medical education can foster an environment that promotes intrinsic motivation [55]. This approach aligns with SDT, which posits that motivation driven by internal factors such as personal interest and satisfaction leads to better academic outcomes and overall student well-being [56]. By enhancing intrinsic motivation, educators can reduce negative behaviors such as procrastination and burnout, ultimately supporting students’ academic success and mental health [57,58].
Our study’s findings on the differentiation between intrinsic motivation (IM) and identified regulation (IR) also have important implications for curriculum design. The observed challenges in distinguishing these constructs in post-situational contexts suggest the need for more nuanced assessment tools or methods. This can help educators better understand and support the complex motivational profiles of medical students, leading to more effective and personalized educational interventions.
In summary, the evidence gathered from our study reinforces the critical role of accurate motivational assessment in health professions education. By leveraging tools like SIMS, educators can gain valuable insights into students’ motivational states and tailor their teaching methods to foster a supportive and motivating learning environment. This approach not only enhances academic performance but also contributes to the overall well-being of medical students, preparing them for the demands of their future clinical practice.

4.3. Limitations and Threats to Validity

This study was conducted at a single U.S. medical school, which may limit its generalizability to other domestic and international medical institutions. Nonetheless, the underlying concepts are widely relevant, and tools based on self-determination theory, such as SIMS, have been utilized in diverse research contexts worldwide. Potential validity threats like the Hawthorne effect were mitigated by the fact that students participated in a familiar environment and were accustomed to completing surveys as a part of their routine activities. Furthermore, to reduce response bias that could skew the psychometric survey outcomes, participants were not informed of the anticipated results, and the surveys were conducted anonymously.

5. Conclusions

While it is commonly believed that medical students possess a high level of inherent motivation, even a cursory observation indicates that their motivation, much like that of any other student, fluctuates over time. Gaining a deeper understanding of this dynamic and identifying effective intervention strategies could greatly benefit medical educators in a variety of areas, from curriculum development to lecture design. This investigation adds to the extensive research on self-determination theory in education and enriches the more limited body of work specific to medical education. By evaluating the application of SIMS in two novel contexts, this research not only bolsters the existing validation evidence for SIMS but also enhances its practical applicability in medical education. Considering the results of this study, educators can use SIMS with increased assurance to explore the motivational dynamics of pre-clerkship medical students in short-term academic scenarios.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.M. and B.W.; data curation, D.M. and B.W; formal analysis, D.M. and B.W.; funding acquisition, D.M and B.W.; investigation, D.M. and B.W.; methodology, D.M. and B.W.; project administration, D.M.; resources, D.M. and B.W.; software, D.M. and B.W.; supervision, D.M.; visualization, D.M. and B.W.; writing—original draft, D.M. and B.W.; writing—review and editing, D.M. and D.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the internal “Scholarly Reflection and Concentration/Capstone (SRCC)” capstone project budget of the Frank H. Netter MD School of Medicine at Quinnipiac University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Quinnipiac University (#06617).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available in order to maintain participant confidentiality per IRB protocol.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge Richard Feinn (Professor of Medical Sciences, Frank H. Netter MD School of Medicine at Quinnipiac University), our consulting statistician, for his guidance with the statistical analyses presented in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

This is a reproduction of the original version of SIMS published by Guay et al. (2000) [19].

Appendix B

This is an example of the four survey instruments used to gather data for the post-situational context: SIMS, MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE.

Appendix C

This is an example of the four survey instruments used to gather data for the short-term context: SIMS, MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE.

Appendix D

These are supplemental figures generated by SPSS Statistics, Version 27 for the post-situational and short-term context CFA.

References

  1. Oxford English and Spanish Dictionary. “Motivation, n.”. 2021. Available online: https://www.lexico.com/en/definition/motivation (accessed on 1 November 2023).
  2. Linnenbrink-Garcia, E.; Patall, E. Motivation. In Handbook of Educational Psychology, 3rd ed.; Corno, L., Anderman, E., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 91–103. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Brick by brick: The origins, development, and future of self-determination theory. Adv. Motiv. Sci. 2019, 6, 111–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness, 1st ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  5. Vansteenkiste, M.; Ryan, R.M.; Soenens, B. Basic psychological need theory: Advancements, critical themes, and future directions. Motiv. Emot. 2020, 44, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Deci, E.; Ryan, R. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ryan, R.; Deci, E. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 25, 54–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior. Psychol. Inq. 2000, 11, 227–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mayer, R.E. Learning and Instruction; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dyrbye, L.N.; Thomas, M.R.; Shanafelt, T.D. Systematic review of depression, anxiety, and other indicators of psychological distress among U.S. and Canadian medical students. Acad. Med. 2006, 81, 354–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Brazeau, C.; Shanafelt, T.D.; Durning, S.J.; Massie, F.S.; Eacker, A.; Moutier, C.; Satele, D.; Sloan, J.A.; Dyrbye, L.N. Distress among matriculating medical students relative to the general population. Acad. Med. 2014, 89, 1520–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Mata, D.A.; Ramos, M.A.; Bansal, N.; Khan, R.; Guille, C.; Di Angelantonio, E.; Sen, S. Prevalence of depression and depressive symptoms among resident physicians. JAMA 2015, 314, 2373–2383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cooke, M.; Irby, D.M.; Sullivan, W.; Ludmerer, K.M. American medical education 100 years after the Flexner report. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006, 355, 1339–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Drolet, B.C.; Rodgers, S. A Comprehensive Medical Student Wellness Program—Design and Implementation at Vanderbilt School of Medicine. Acad. Med. 2010, 85, 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Slavin, S.; Schindler, D.; Chibnall, J.T. Medical Student Mental Health 3.0. Acad. Med. 2014, 89, 573–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dyrbye, L.N.; West, C.P.; Satele, D.; Boone, S.; Tan, L.; Sloan, J.A.; Shanafelt, T.D. Burnout among U.S. medical students, residents, and early career physicians relative to the general U.S. population. Acad. Med. 2014, 89, 443–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Binning, J. “Construct”. Encyclopedia Britannica. 2016. Available online: https://www.britannica.com/science/construct (accessed on 1 November 2023).
  18. DeVellis, R.F. Scale Development: Theory and Applications; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  19. Guay, F.; Vallerand, R.; Blanchard, C. On the Assessment of Situational Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS). Motiv. Emot. 2000, 24, 175–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Jeno, L.M.; Raaheim, A.; Kristensen, S.M.; Kristensen KD, B.; Hole, T.N.; Haugland, M.; Mæland, S. The Relative Effect of Team-Based Learning on Motivation and Learning: A Self-Determination Theory Perspective. CBE-Life Sci. Educ. 2017, 16, ar59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Zimmerman, B.J. Investigating Self-Regulation and Motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2008, 45, 166–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Messick, S. Validity. In Educational Measurement, 3rd ed.; Linn, R.L., Ed.; American Council on Education: New York, NY, USA, 1989; pp. 13–103. [Google Scholar]
  23. Downing, S.M. Validity: On the meaningful interpretation of assessment data. Med. Educ. 2003, 37, 830–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Cook, D.A.; Beckman, T.J. Current Concepts in Validity and Reliability for Psychometric Instruments: Theory and application. Am. J. Med. 2006, 119, 166.e7–166.e16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Goodwin, L.D.; Leech, N.L. The meaning of validity in the new standards for educational and psychological testing. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 2003, 36, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Vallerand, R.J.; Fortier, M.N. Measures of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation in Sport and Physical Activity: A Review and Critique. In Advances in Sport and Exercise Psychology Measurement; Duda, J.L., Ed.; Fitness Information Technology: Morgantown, WV, USA, 1998; pp. 81–101. [Google Scholar]
  27. Schaufeli, W.B.; Martínez, I.; Pinto, A.M.; Salanova, M.; Bakker, A.B. Burnout and engagement in university students. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2002, 33, 464–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Posch, L.; Bleier, A.; Lechner, C.M.; Danner, D.; Flöck, F.; Strohmaier, M. Measuring Motivations of Crowdworkers: The Multidimensional Crowdworker Motivation Scale. arXiv 2017, arXiv:1702.01661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Muthen, L.K.; Muthen, B.O. MPlus Version 8 User’s Guide; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  30. Nunnally, J.; Bernstein, I.H. Psychometric Theory; McGraw-Hill Humanities/Social Sciences/Languages: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  31. Bland, J.M.; Altman, D.G. Statistics notes: Cronbach’s alpha. BMJ 1997, 314, 572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Using and Interpreting Cronbach’s Alpha|UVA Library. (n.d.). Available online: https://data.library.virginia.edu/using-and-interpreting-cronbachs-alpha/ (accessed on 1 November 2023).
  33. Browne, M.W. An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2001, 36, 111–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hooper, D.; Coughlan, J.; Mullen, M.R. Structural Equation Modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods 2008, 6, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed.; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Modification Indices|Just Enough R. (n.d.-b). Available online: https://benwhalley.github.io/just-enough-r/modification-indices.html#:~:text=T%20he%20modification%20index%20is,was%20added%20or%20constraint%20freed (accessed on 1 November 2023).
  38. Deci, E.L.; Vallerand, R.J.; Pelletier, L.G.; Ryan, R.M. Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. Educ. Psychol. 1991, 26, 325–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ten Cate, O.; Snell, L.; Carraccio, C. Medical education: Past, present and future. Perspect. Med. Educ. 2010, 29, 320–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kusurkar, R.A.; Croiset, G.; Cate, O.T. Twelve tips to stimulate intrinsic motivation in students through autonomy-supportive classroom teaching derived from Self-Determination Theory. Med. Teach. 2011, 33, 978–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Al-Eraky, M.M.; Marei, H.F. A fresh look at Miller’s pyramid: Assessment at the “Is” levels. Med. Teach. 2016, 38, 830–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mann, K. The role of educational theory in continuing medical education: Has it helped us? J. Contin. Educ. Health Prof. 2004, 24 (Suppl. S1), S22–S30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kusurkar, R.; Ten Cate, T.; Van Asperen, M.; Croiset, G. Motivation as an independent and a dependent variable in medical education: A review of the literature. Med. Teach. 2011, 33, 242–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Stipek, D.J. Motivation to Learn: Integrating Theory and Practice; Allyn & Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  45. Blumberg, P. Evaluating the Evidence That Problem-Based Learners Are Self-Directed Learners: A Review of the Literature. In Problem-Based Learning: A Research Perspective on Learning Interactions; Evensen, D.H., Hmelo, C.E., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 199–226. [Google Scholar]
  46. Murad, M.H.; Varkey, P. Self-directed learning in health professions education. Ann. Acad. Med. Singap. 2008, 37, 580–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Cronbach, L.J.; Meehl, P.E. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol. Bull. 1955, 52, 281–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kane, M.T. Validating the interpretations and uses of test scores. J. Educ. Meas. 2013, 50, 1–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. American Educational Research Association; American Psychological Association; National Council on Measurement in Education. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing; American Educational Research Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  50. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 61, 101860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Vansteenkiste, M.; Mouratidis, A. Emerging trends and future directions for the field of motivation psychology: A special issue in honor of the 40th anniversary of the publication of “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation”. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 28, 375–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Howard, J.L.; Gagné, M.; Bureau, J.S. Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 1346–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Litalien, D.; Morin, A.J.; McInerney, D.M. Achievement goal profiles among adolescent males and females. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 53, 731–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Núñez, J.L.; Jaime, L. Determinants of Classroom Engagement: A Prospective Test Based on Self-Determination Theory. Teach. Teach. 2018, 25, 147–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Pless, A.; Hari, R.; Harris, M. Why are medical students so motivated to learn ultrasound skills? A qualitative study. BMC Med. Educ. 2024, 24, 458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Siqueira, M.A.M.; Gonçalves, J.P.; Mendonça, V.S.; Kobayasi, R.; Arantes-Costa, F.M.; Tempski, P.Z.; Martins, M.d.A. Relationship between metacognitive awareness and motivation to learn in medical students. BMC Med. Educ. 2020, 20, 393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Tisocco, F.; Liporace, M. Structural Relationships Between Procrastination, Academic Motivation, and Academic Achievement Within University Students: A Self-determination Theory Approach. Innov. High. Educ. 2023, 48, 351–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Edgar, S.; Carr, S.E.; Connaughton, J.; Celenza, A. Student motivation to learn: Is self-belief the key to transition and first year performance in an undergraduate health professions program? BMC Med. Educ. 2019, 19, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Visual representation of the continuum of motivation, from self-determined to non-self-determined, according to self-determination theory [2].
Figure 1. Visual representation of the continuum of motivation, from self-determined to non-self-determined, according to self-determination theory [2].
Ime 03 00018 g001
Table 1. Five sources of construct validity evidence and their definitions [23].
Table 1. Five sources of construct validity evidence and their definitions [23].
Validity Evidence SourcesValidity Evidence Definitions
ContentContent validity focuses on the construct’s dimensions and how well they are represented.
Response processResponse-process validity is the extent to which the actions and thought processes of respondents demonstrate that they understand the instrument questions the same way as the researchers do.
Internal structureInternal structure evidence asks whether a group of questions designed to assess the same dimension exhibit higher correlations with one another compared to groups of questions aimed at measuring different dimensions.
Relationship to other variablesRelationship to other variables evidence asks if external correlations (i.e., correlations to entirely different instruments) are confirmatory or counter-confirmatory.
ConsequencesConsequences evidence asks, what is the impact on respondents of being appraised by the instrument?
Table 2. The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS)’s 16 items, which were adapted from present to past tense [19].
Table 2. The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS)’s 16 items, which were adapted from present to past tense [19].
ItemPresent-Tense SIMS VersionPast-Tense SIMS Version
1Because I think that the activity is interestingBecause I thought that the activity was interesting
2Because I am doing it for my own goodBecause I was doing it for my own good
3Because I am supposed to do itBecause I was supposed to do it
4There may be good reasons to do this activity, but personally I don’t see anyThere may have been good reasons to do this activity, but personally I don’t see any
5Because I think that this activity is pleasantBecause I thought that this activity was pleasant
6Because I think that this activity is good for meBecause I thought that this activity was good for me
7Because it is something that I have to doBecause it was something that I had to do
8I do this activity, but I am not sure if it is worth itI did this activity, but I am not sure if it was worth it
9Because this activity is funBecause this activity was fun
10By personal decisionBy personal decision
11Because I don’t have any choiceBecause I didn’t have any choice
12I don’t know; I don’t see what this activity brings me I don’t know; I don’t see what this activity brought me
13Because I feel good when doing this activityBecause I felt good when doing this activity
14Because I believe that this activity is important for meBecause I believe that this activity was important for me
15Because I feel that I have to do itBecause I feel that I had to do it
16I do this activity, but I am not sure it is a good thing to pursue itI did this activity, but I am not sure it was a good thing to pursue it
Table 3. SIMS Cronbach’s alpha values for the four dimensions of the motivation construct calculated from post-situational and short-term context data.
Table 3. SIMS Cronbach’s alpha values for the four dimensions of the motivation construct calculated from post-situational and short-term context data.
SIMS
Context
Intrinsic Motivation (IM)
Qs 1, 5, 9, 13
Identified Regulation (IR)
Qs 2, 6, 10, 14
External Regulation (ER)
Qs 3, 7, 11, 15
Amotivation (AM)
Qs 4, 8, 12, 16
Post-situational
Cronbach’s alpha
0.980.980.950.93
Short-term
Cronbach’s alpha
0.980.820.990.91
Table 4. SIMS internal Pearson’s r correlation values calculated from the post-situational context data form an ordered simplex pattern between the four dimensions of the motivation construct.
Table 4. SIMS internal Pearson’s r correlation values calculated from the post-situational context data form an ordered simplex pattern between the four dimensions of the motivation construct.
SIMS Post-Situational Context
Intrinsic Motivation (IM)Identified Regulation (IR)External Regulation (ER)Amotivation (AM)
Intrinsic motivation (IM)1.00.63−0.25−0.3
Identified regulation
(IR)
0.631.0−0.16−0.43
External regulation (ER)−0.25−0.161.00.3
Amotivation
(AM)
−0.3−0.430.31.0
Table 5. SIMS internal Pearson’s r correlation values calculated from the short-term context data form an ordered simplex pattern between the four dimensions of the motivation construct.
Table 5. SIMS internal Pearson’s r correlation values calculated from the short-term context data form an ordered simplex pattern between the four dimensions of the motivation construct.
SIMS Short-Term Context
Intrinsic Motivation (IM)Identified Regulation (IR)External Regulation (ER)Amotivation (AM)
Intrinsic motivation (IM)1.00.27−0.19−0.23
Identified regulation
(IR)
0.271.00.18−0.29
External regulation (ER)−0.190.181.00.16
Amotivation
(AM)
−0.23−0.290.161.0
Table 6. SIMS standardized factor loadings from CFA models of the post-situational context data. Note: * p < 0.01.
Table 6. SIMS standardized factor loadings from CFA models of the post-situational context data. Note: * p < 0.01.
SIMS Post-Situational Context
ItemIntrinsic Motivation (IM)Identified Regulation (IR)External Regulation (ER)Amotivation (AM)
10.76 *
50.87 *
90.85 *
130.77 *
2 0.83 *
6 0.85 *
10 0.35 *
14 0.77 *
3 0.76 *
7 0.84 *
11 0.68 *
15 0.85 *
4 0.83 *
8 0.78 *
12 0.86 *
16 0.66 *
Factor
correlation
IMIRER
IR0.69 *
ER−0.25 *−0.07
AM−0.30 *−0.48 *0.28 *
Table 7. SIMS standardized factor loadings from CFA models of the short-term context data. Note: † = p < 0.05; * = p < 0.01.
Table 7. SIMS standardized factor loadings from CFA models of the short-term context data. Note: † = p < 0.05; * = p < 0.01.
SIMS Short-Term Context
ItemIntrinsic Motivation (IM)Identified Regulation (IR)External Regulation (ER)Amotivation
(AM)
10.56 *
50.83 *
90.98 *
130.68 *
2 0.60 *
6 0.79 *
10 0.47 *
14 0.79 *
3 0.71 *
7 0.90 *
11 0.50 *
15 0.77 *
4 0.77 *
8 0.75 *
12 0.75 *
16 0.81 *
Factor
correlation
IMIRER
IR0.25 *
ER−0.18 †0.44 *
AM−0.12−0.36 *0.12
Table 8. SIMS goodness-of-fit indices from CFA models of the post-situational context data.
Table 8. SIMS goodness-of-fit indices from CFA models of the post-situational context data.
SIMS Post-situational Context
Model Fit IndicesBIC8235.9
χ2/df247.2/98 = 2.52
CFI0.894
RMSEA (95% CI)0.099 (0.084–0.114)
SRMR0.079
Table 9. SIMS goodness-of-fit indices from CFA models of the short-term context data.
Table 9. SIMS goodness-of-fit indices from CFA models of the short-term context data.
SIMS Short-Term Context
Model Fit IndicesBIC8025.6
χ2/df146.2/98 = 1.49
CFI0.954
RMSEA (95% CI)0.056 (0.036–0.074)
SRMR0.082
Table 10. MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE Cronbach’s alpha values and Pearson’s r correlation values relative to SIMS calculated from the post-situational context data.
Table 10. MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE Cronbach’s alpha values and Pearson’s r correlation values relative to SIMS calculated from the post-situational context data.
Post-SituationalSIMSIMIRERAMCronbach’s Alpha
MCMSIM0.850.65−0.21−0.290.93
IR0.170.230.06−0.080.98
ER−0.09−0.010.300.230.89
AM−0.40−0.550.320.760.98
AMSIM0.640.58−0.21−0.160.96
IR0.440.51−0.11−0.200.96
ER0.140.080.100.210.95
AM−0.34−0.490.290.810.31
MBI-SSE−0.40−0.330.230.370.998
Table 11. MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE Cronbach’s alpha values and Pearson’s r correlation values relative to SIMS calculated from the short-term context data.
Table 11. MCMS, AMS, and MBI-SSEE Cronbach’s alpha values and Pearson’s r correlation values relative to SIMS calculated from the short-term context data.
Short-TermSIMSIMIRERAMCronbach’s Alpha
MCMSIM0.860.30−0.22−0.180.98
IR−0.090.300.24−0.150.96
ER−0.070.010.320.050.88
AM−0.32−0.290.210.590.99
AMSIM0.640.31−0.120.020.91
IR0.180.390.25−0.180.97
ER−0.140.110.350.120.96
AM−0.1−0.310.130.650.84
MBI-SSEE−0.45−0.100.230.310.93
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wasicek, B.; McHugh, D. Validity Evidence for Using the Situational Motivation Scale to Assess Pre-Clerkship Medical Student Motivation. Int. Med. Educ. 2024, 3, 212-228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ime3030018

AMA Style

Wasicek B, McHugh D. Validity Evidence for Using the Situational Motivation Scale to Assess Pre-Clerkship Medical Student Motivation. International Medical Education. 2024; 3(3):212-228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ime3030018

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wasicek, Brian, and Douglas McHugh. 2024. "Validity Evidence for Using the Situational Motivation Scale to Assess Pre-Clerkship Medical Student Motivation" International Medical Education 3, no. 3: 212-228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ime3030018

APA Style

Wasicek, B., & McHugh, D. (2024). Validity Evidence for Using the Situational Motivation Scale to Assess Pre-Clerkship Medical Student Motivation. International Medical Education, 3(3), 212-228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ime3030018

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop