Infrequent Cooperative Breeding in a Short-Lived Migratory Songbird, the Wilson’s Warbler
Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Study Area
2.2. Procedures
3. Results
3.1. The Behavior of a Single Color-Banded Territorial Helper, HM, and Its Interactions with the Host Pair, in the Host Breeding Territory of RM and RF
3.2. Behaviors Related to HM’s Singing
3.3. Behaviors Related to HM’s Interactions with RF
3.4. Helper Behavior of HM in Satellite Territories
3.5. HM’s Helper Behavior Related to Brood Care
3.6. Observations Based on Frequency of Territorial Helpers, and of Hosting Pairs, in the Total Study Population
3.7. Singing by Helper Males in Host Territories
3.8. An Apparent Attempt to Expel a Helper Male from a Host Territory
4. Discussion
4.1. Relevant Findings Related to Infrequent Cooperative Breeding
- (1)
- Findings Related to Helping During Nest Building—Helping behavior in birds is stated to occur in the nest construction, incubation, nestling, and fledgling breeding stages [27]. However, most reports of infrequent helping behavior in parulids have been based on observations made during nestling care [13,14,15,16]. In this study, although I observed helping behavior by one helper male during fledgling care, all other helping behavior I observed was during host female nest building. Helping during nest building is of special interest, since host females are fertile at that stage [25], and there is enhanced potential for sexual helper male/host female interactions. This study found no evidence of sexual helper male/host female interactions. There is also enhanced potential for aggressive host male/helper male interactions, based on host males protecting their paternity. This study found no evidence of host male/helper male interactions that might be attributed to protection of paternity. However, a few times, this study did document host males attacking helpers when the helpers sang in the presence of the host males. No such attacks occurred when the host males were absent from their territories, however, and helper males then appeared to sing with impunity. Finally, observations made during nest building allowed for detection of interactions between helper males and intruding males. This study confirmed that helpers sometimes actively drove out intruders, which were apparently seeking EPCs with nest building host females. I am not aware of a prior study of infrequent helping behavior that primarily was based on observations made during nest building.
- (2)
- Helper Male Solicitation—A helper male entering a host territory during nest building, when females are fertile, is superficially problematic. Most intruding Wilson’s Warbler males were vigorously attacked and driven away by resident males [25,26]. Thus, how helper males gain access to resident males’ territories, especially when resident females are fertile, is said to have perplexed researchers since Skutch’s early studies of helping behavior [13]. However, this study shows that helper male Wilson’s Warblers do gain acceptance in host territories, even during nest building when females are fertile. To my knowledge, there is no prior empirical or speculative evidence showing or hypothesizing how this might happen. In this study, I observed an intruding Wilson’s Warbler male, when attacked by the resident males in two different breeding territories, solicit those attacking males, rather than fleeing. In both cases, the resident males immediately ceased their attacks and flew away. I observed one such solicitation in the principal host territory of RM and RF and the other in a satellite host territory. The resident females in both territories were nest-building at the time and thus were fertile. I subsequently observed that a helper male had become established in each territory. I confirmed that the soliciting male and the helper male that then became established in the satellite territory were the same male, color-banded HM. The male seen to solicit in the principal territory was unbanded at the time. However, a helper male subsequently became established in the territory, and I banded it Bk/O(s), coded it 9830, and called it HM. It seems likely that the unbanded soliciting bird and the subsequent helper were the same bird, that is, HM. Based on these two observations of solicitation, I speculate that solicitation by prospective helpers is at least one behavior, and possibly the only behavior, allowing intruding males to become accepted as helpers in host territories. I know of no other study providing observations on how infrequent helper males might become established in host territories.
- (3)
- Defense of Host Pair Territories by Helper Males—I recorded helper male HM attacking intruding males and chasing them out of the territory of the host pair RM and RF. I also observed HM attacking intruders in one of the three satellite territories. I also observed territorial defense behavior by two of the three other helper males detected in this study. I am not aware of other observations of infrequent helper males defending host territories, or fertile host females, against intruding males.
- (4)
- Helping in Multiple Territories—A male bird that becomes an infrequent helper in a host territory could be presumed to remain attached to that single territory, and to no other territory. In this study I documented a helper male, HM, additionally flying into three adjacent breeding territories and becoming accepted as a helper there. I am not aware of prior reports of a single infrequent helper male simultaneously becoming established in multiple host breeding territories.
- (5)
- Helper Males’ Singing—In this study, three infrequent helper males observed to sing when host males were present were vigorously attacked by the host males, as common intruders would be. However, the same three helper males sang with impunity when the resident males were temporarily absent, one due to a banding operation and the other two for reasons unknown. Thus, infrequent helpers’ singing in host territories may present both positive and negative consequences for the host and helper males, depending on when it occurs. When a host male is absent, helpers’ singing may guard a territory from intrusion and possible attempted takeovers by foreign males. This might allow a host male more time to pursue EPCs with a reduced risk of a foreign male taking over its territory or engaging in EPC with its mate. However, a helper male singing when a host male is present could present a threat of territorial takeover by the helper. I had some evidence suggesting that the host male I observed chasing a likely helper for at least an hour was doing so due to the helper having sung in the host territory when the host male was present. I had heard two males singing repeatedly in a distant corner of the host territory the day before the chase, although I did not confirm their identities. In any case, however, the extended chase indicated the difficulty and energy expenditure that apparently is associated with trying to expel a helper from a host territory. I am not aware of similar observations of infrequent helpers’ singing from other studies.
- (6)
- Frequency of Wilson’s Warbler Helping Behavior—This study found helping behavior in just 6.1% of the principal breeding territories in the study population, although I also found helping in an additional 4.5% of the satellite territories. I detected just four different helpers during the study. These figures indicate that helping is relatively infrequent in Wilson’s Warblers. This paucity of helpers has also been found in other studied North American parulid species, and in some species helping is even more infrequent than in Wilson’s Warblers. For example, Tarof and Stutchbury [13] found cooperative breeding in just 1.5% (1/60) of monitored Hooded Warbler (Setophaga citrina) pairs, and Peak and Kendrick [15] found helping in just 0.25% (1/400) of the monitored nests of Golden-Cheeked Warblers (S. chrysoparia). These and other observations of helping in many short-lived migratory species would seem to justify the terminology of “infrequent helpers” for such species.
4.2. From Which of Two Behavioral Subpopulations Do Helper Male Wilson’s Warblers Derive?
4.3. Possible Reasons for Infrequent Helping in Wilson’s Warblers: Kinship and Inclusive Fitness
4.4. Polyandry
4.5. Mutualism and Reciprocity
4.6. Infrequent Helping and Population Ecology
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Skutch, A.F. Helpers at the nest. Auk 1935, 52, 257–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woolfenden, G.E. Florida Scrub Jay helpers at the nest. Auk 1975, 92, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenig, W.D.; Mumme, R.L. Population Ecology of the Cooperatively-Breeding Acorn Wookpecker; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Burke, T.; Davies, N.B.; Bruford, M.W.; Hatchwell, B.J. Parental care and nesting behaviour of polyandrous dunnocks (Prunella modularis) related to paternity by DNA fingerprinting. Nature 1989, 338, 249–251. [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton, W.D. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 1964, 7, 17–52. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Riehl, C. Living with strangers: Direct benefits favour non-kin cooperation in a community-nesting bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 2011, 278, 1728–1735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riehl, C. Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proc. R. Soc. B 2013, 280, 2245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kingma, S.A. Direct benefits explain interspecific variation in helping behavior among cooperatively breeding birds. Nat. Commun. 2017, 8, 1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cockburn, A. Evolution of helping behavior in cooperatively breeding birds. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1998, 29, 141–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cockburn, A.R.; Mulder, A.; Green, D.J.; Double, M.C. Divorce, dispersal, and incest avoidance in the Superb Fairy-wren, Malurus cyaneus. J. Anim. Ecol. 2003, 72, 189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Earl, A.D.; Carter, G.G.; Berlinger, A.G.; Korir, E.; Shah, S.S.; Watetu, W.N.; Rubenstein, D.R. A cryptic role for reciprocal helping in a cooperatively breeding bird. Nature 2025, 7, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camerlenghi, E.; McQueen, A.; Delhey, K.; Cook, C.N.; Kingma, S.A.; Farine, D.R.; Peters, A. Cooperative breeding and the emergencc of multilevel societies in birds. Ecol. Lett. 2022, 25, 766–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tarof, S.A.; Stutchbury, B.J. A case of cooperative breeding in the Hooded Warbler. Wilson Bull. 1996, 108, 382–384. Available online: https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/wilson_bulletin/vol108/iss2/20/ (accessed on 2 August 2025).
- King, D.L.; Champlin, T.B.; Champlin, P.J. An observation of cooperative breeding in the Ovenbird. Wilson Bull. 2000, 112, 287–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peak, R.G.; Kendrick, S.W. Cooperative breeding in the golden-cheeked warbler (Setophaga chrysoparia). Southwest. Nat. 2013, 58, 486–488. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/24643736 (accessed on 2 August 2025). [CrossRef]
- Collins, M.L. Area Utilization, Evidence of Cooperative Breeding, Sex Ratio Bias, and Nest-Site Selection in Black-and-White Warblers (Mniotilta varia). Master’s Thesis, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, J.L. Helping and Communal Breeding in Birds: Ecology and Evolution; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1987; p. 374. [Google Scholar]
- Woolfenden, G.E.; Fitzpatrick, J.W. Florida Scrub Jays: A Synopsis after 18 years of study. In Cooperative Breeding in Birds: Long-Term Studies of Ecology and Behavior; Stacey, P.B., Koenig, W.D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990; pp. 240–266. [Google Scholar]
- Walters, J.R.; Copeyon, C.K.; Carter Iii, J.H. Test of the ecological basis of cooperative breeding in red-cockaded woodpeckers. Auk 1992, 109, 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trombino, C. Helping behavior within sapsuckers (Sphyrapicus spp.). Wilson Bull. 2000, 112, 273–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ben Mocha, Y.; Scemama de Gialluly, S.; Griesser, M.; Markman, S. What is cooperative breeding in mammals and birds? Removing definitional barriers for comparative research. Biol. Rev. 2023, 98, 1845–1861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weatherhead, P.J.; Forbes, M.R.L. Natal philopatry in passerine birds: Genetic or ecological influences? Behav. Ecol. 1994, 5, 426–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilbert, W.M. Late breeding season definitive prebasic molt in males, and late breeding season brood care by females, in central California Wilson’s warblers. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 12, e8689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilbert, W.M. Territorial and non-territorial subpopulations of Wilson’s Warblers return to central California breeding grounds in two migratory waves. Ecol. Evol. 2024, 14, e70672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ammon, E.M.; Gilbert, W.M. Wilson’s Warbler (Carlellina pusilla), version 1.0. In Birds of the World; Rodewald, R.G., Ed.; Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology: Ithica, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, W.M. Territorial intrusions by resident male Wilson’s Warblers: Breeding stages they intrude and distances they travel. West. N. Am. Nat. 2024, 83, 472–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stacey, P.B.; Koenig, W.D. (Eds.) Cooperative Breeding in Birds: Long Term Studies of Ecology and Behaviour; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Beason, R.C.; Trout, L.L. Cooperative breeding in the Bobolink. Wilson Bull. 1984, 96, 709–710. [Google Scholar]
- Shy, W.M. Interspecific feeding among birds: A review. J. Field Ornithol. 1992, 53, 370–393. [Google Scholar]
- Myers, G.R.; Waller, D.W. Helpers at the nest in Barn Swallows. Auk 1977, 94, 31. [Google Scholar]
- Dexter, R.W. Extra-parental cooperation in the nesting of Chimney Swifts. Wilson Bull. 1952, 64, 133–139. [Google Scholar]
- Clutton-Brock, T. Breeding together: Kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science 2002, 296, 69–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kokko, H.; Johnstone, R.A.; Wright, J. The evolution of parental and alloparental effort in cooperatively breeding groups: When should helpers pay to stay? Behav. Ecol. 2002, 13, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emlen, S.T.; Vehrencamp, S.L. Cooperative breeding strategies among birds. In Perspectives in Ornithology; Bush, A.H., Clark, J.A., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983; pp. 93–120. [Google Scholar]
- Hatchwell, B.J.; Russel, A.F.; Fowlie, M.K.; Ross, D.J. Reproductive success and nest site selection in a cooperative breeder: Effect of experience and a direct benefit of helping. Auk 1999, 116, 355–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Downing, P.A.; Griffin, A.S.; Cornwallis, C.K. Hard-working helpers contribute to long breeder lifespans in cooperative birds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2021, 376, 20190742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
(1) | Number of monitoring sessions in RM/RF’s territory. | 10 |
(2) | Number of times HM was sighted in RM/RF’s territory. | 42 |
(3) | Frequency of HM sightings per monitoring session. | 4.2 |
(4) | Times intruding males other than HM were seen to be attacked. | >50 |
(5) | Number of intruding males specifically seen to be attacked by RM. | 44 |
(6) | Number of intruding males specifically seen to be attacked by HM. | 7 |
(7) | Number of times HM was seen to be attacked by RM after singing in RM’s territory when RM was present. | 2 |
(8) | Number of times HM was seen not to be attacked by RM after singing in RM’s territory when RM was absent. | 2 |
(9) | Closest distance between RM and HM. | <2 m |
(10) | Number of times RF was seen to be attacked, chased, or solicited by intruding foreign males. | >10 |
(11) | Number of times RF was seen to be attacked, chased, or solicited by HM. | 0 |
(12) | Number of times RF solicited HM. | 0 |
(13) | Closest distance between RF and HM. | <2 m |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gilbert, W. Infrequent Cooperative Breeding in a Short-Lived Migratory Songbird, the Wilson’s Warbler. Birds 2025, 6, 49. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds6030049
Gilbert W. Infrequent Cooperative Breeding in a Short-Lived Migratory Songbird, the Wilson’s Warbler. Birds. 2025; 6(3):49. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds6030049
Chicago/Turabian StyleGilbert, William. 2025. "Infrequent Cooperative Breeding in a Short-Lived Migratory Songbird, the Wilson’s Warbler" Birds 6, no. 3: 49. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds6030049
APA StyleGilbert, W. (2025). Infrequent Cooperative Breeding in a Short-Lived Migratory Songbird, the Wilson’s Warbler. Birds, 6(3), 49. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds6030049