The Effect of Physical-Activity-Based Programs on School Children’s Cognitive Competence-Related Variables: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Experimental Approach to the Problem
2.2. Information Sources
2.3. Search Strategy
2.4. Eligibility Criteria
2.5. Data Extraction
2.6. Risk of Bias Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Identification and Selection of Studies
3.2. Quality Assessment
3.3. Study Characteristics
- Attention: Six studies evaluated the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s attention. Among them, four studies reported positive effects of intervention programs on children’s attention. Specifically, the Active-Start program revealed significant improvement in attention and concentration when cardiorespiratory fitness was at least, between 3.05 and 0.70 mL/kg/min, respectively [18]; the Movement Games program (based on MVPA) improves attention ability [19]; the external focus-based program showed more effects on attentional focus than internal focus-based exercises [32]; and a nuanced prediction of individual DMA indices spatial attention ability [33]. However, two studies did not find statistically significant effects of exercises based on aerobic efforts or cognitively demanding tasks when compared to usual PE classes [40] or acute PA on lapses of attention [37].
- Inhibitory control: Six studies evaluated the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s inhibition. Among them, one study demonstrated positive effects of the Movement Games intervention program compared with usual PE classes [19], while three studies demonstrated the effects of PA on children’s inhibition but did not find differences between groups. Specifically, Schmidt et al. [20] did not find more positive effects in different types of exercises (high cognitive engagement + high physical exertion, high cognitive engagement + low physical exertion, or low cognitive engagement + low physical exertion); Oppici et al. [29] did not find differences between high cognitive physical activity, low cognitive physical activity, or usual PE classes; and Kolovelonis & Goudas [31] did not find different effects of PA games highlighting contextual interference, mental control, or discovery. However, two studies did not demonstrate any effects on inhibition function [28,37].
- Memory: Six studies evaluated the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s memory capacity. Among them, three studies showed positive effects, while the other three did not demonstrate between-group differences. Specifically, high cognitive PA intervention improves memory better than low cognitive or habitual PE classes [29]; children performing PACER task intervention achieved better recall of words after a brief delay than children without treatment [26]; and children in physical activity games highlighting both contextual interference and mental control significantly enhanced working memory [31]. Meanwhile, other studies demonstrate how aerobic exercise or cognitively demanding exercises did not demonstrate evidence for effects on visuospatial short-term memory and verbal working memory [21]; external and internal focus instruction-based programs did not find the prediction of learning based on working memory capacity [32]; and integrating juggling with math lessons did not show better results than children performing sedentary math lessons on math memorization performance [35].
- Cognitive flexibility: Eight studies evaluated the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s cognitive flexibility. Among them, three studies found positive effects of evaluated programs. Specifically, the Active Recess program has better positive effects than intervention without physical exercise [23]; the Movi-Kids intervention showed better outcomes than usual PE classes on cognitive changes [34]; and martial-arts-based intervention showed better results than habitual PE classes on children’s cognitive self-regulation [38]. Another two studies found positive effects, although no differences between groups were found. In this case, PA games highlighting contextual interference, mental control, or discovery seem equally effective for improving cognitive flexibility when comparing with the no PE intervention group [31], In addition, Oppici et al. [29] support that high cognitive intervention could enhance cognitive flexibility, but they did not find statistical differences between groups (high cognitive or low cognitive PA). However, ball-skill-based intervention [22] and high-intensity interval training did not show better strategy than usual PE classes [30]; and the Active Smarter Kids program [28] did not show any effects on children’s cognitive flexibility.
- Academic performance: Twelve studies evaluated the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s academic performance. Among them, the Active-Start program showed positive effects on language and mathematics [18]; karate-based intervention improved children’s academic achievement [41]; an intervention program improved mathematics scores and language scores more than children performing habitual PE classes [30]; an enriched PE program improves better fluency and flexibility better than conventional PE classes [33]; karate-based intervention seems particularly effective for children with psychosocial difficulties and low academic performance [41]; PE classes taught by specialists are more effective on children’s numeracy and writing [36]; and a martial-arts-based program improves mental math performance [38]. However, other programs did not report significant effects after conducting their intervention programs. Among them, there are no effects of the ball-skill-based intervention program on learning lags in reading and mathematics [22]; there are no effects of aerobic, cognitive engaging, or habitual PE classes on academic achievement, although children with lower performance in reading at baseline performed better in reading at post-test after performing cognitive engaging intervention program [25]; after SPARK program intervention the authors highlighted that engaging children in more time of physical not necessarily influence academic performance [27]; the Active Smarter Kids intervention did not significantly improve verbal fluency [28]; and activity bursts in the classroom seem no more effective than normal curriculum activities for improving reading and mathematics [39]. However, it is of interest to analyze depending on the areas of the measured variables: general academic achievement, mathematics, speech, and literacy skills.
- Brain activation: One study evaluated the brain activation [21]. The authors found that greater brain activation was greater after both aerobic-based and cognitive-engaging interventions, although no statistical differences were found after them.
4. Discussion
4.1. Effects of EF on Attention in Primary School Students
4.2. Effects of PE on the Inhibitory Control of Primary School Students
4.3. Effects of PE on the Memory of Primary School Students
4.4. The Effects of PE on the Cognitive Flexibility of Primary School Students
4.5. Effects of PE on the Academic Performance of Primary School Students
4.5.1. Academic Performance in Mathematics
4.5.2. Academic Performance in Speaking
4.5.3. Academic Performance in Reading and Writing
4.6. The Effects of PE on the Creativity of Primary School Students
5. Limitations
6. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Miller, K.R.; McClave, S.A.; Jampolis, M.B.; Hurt, R.T.; Krueger, K.; Landes, S.; Collier, B. The Health Benefits of Exercise and Physical Activity. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2016, 5, 204–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Z.; Chen, W. A Systematic Review of the Relationship Between Physical Activity and Happiness. J. Happiness Stud. 2019, 20, 1305–1322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rico-González, M. The Effect of Primary School-Based Physical Education Programs: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. J. Phys. Act. Health 2023, 20, 317–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manzano, J. Educación física y desarrollo integral. Isla de Arriarán 2006, 28, 275–294. [Google Scholar]
- Shi, P.; Feng, X. Motor Skills and Cognitive Benefits in Children and Adolescents: Relationship, Mechanism and Perspectives. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 1017825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carbonell Ventura, T.; Antoñanzas Laborda, J.L.; Lope Álvarez, Á. La educación física y las relaciones sociales en educación primaria. Int. J. Dev. Educ. Psychol. Rev. INFAD Psicol. 2018, 2, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rico-González, M.; Martín-Moya, R.; Giles-Girela, F.J.; Ardigò, L.P.; González-Fernández, F.T. The Effects of Cardiopulmonary Fitness on Executive Functioning or Academic Performance in Students from Early Childhood to Adolescence? A Systematic Review. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2025, 10, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berrios Aguayo, B.; Latorre-Román, P.A.; Salas Sánchez, J.; Pantoja Vallejo, A. Effect of Physical Activity and Fitness on Executive Functions and Academic Performance in Children of Elementary School. A Systematic Review. Cult. Cienc. Deporte 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shields, G.S.; Sazma, M.A.; Yonelinas, A.P. The Effects of Acute Stress on Core Executive Functions: A Meta-Analysis and Comparison with Cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 68, 651–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Festa, F.; Medori, S.; Macrì, M. Move Your Body, Boost Your Brain: The Positive Impact of Physical Activity on Cognition across All Age Groups. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 1765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rico-González, M.; González-Devesa, D.; Gómez-Carmona, C.D.; Moreno-Villanueva, A. Exercise as Modula-tor of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) in Children: A Systematic Review of Randomized Con-trolled Trials. Life 2025, 15, 1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Latomme, J.; Calders, P.; Van Waelvelde, H.; Mariën, T.; De Craemer, M. The Role of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) in the Relation between Physical Activity and Executive Functioning in Children. Children 2022, 9, 596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhattacharya, P.; Chatterjee, S.; Roy, D. Impact of Exercise on Brain Neurochemicals: A Comprehensive Review. Sport Sci. Health 2023, 19, 405–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernal, D. Educación Física: Una asignatura para mejorar el rendimiento académico, la cognición y los valores. Rev. Infancia Educ. Aprendiz. 2016, 2, 96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 Statement: An Updated Guideline for Reporting Systematic Reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rico-González, M.; Pino-Ortega, J.; Clemente, F.M.; Los Arcos, A. Guidelines for Performing Systematic Reviews in Sports Science. Biol. Sport 2022, 39, 463–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sterne, J.A.C.; Savović, J.; Page, M.J.; Elbers, R.G.; Blencowe, N.S.; Boutron, I.; Cates, C.J.; Cheng, H.-Y.; Corbett, M.S.; Eldridge, S.M.; et al. RoB 2: A Revised Tool for Assessing Risk of Bias in Randomised Trials. BMJ 2019, 366, l4898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- García-Hermoso, A.; Hormazábal-Aguayo, I.; Fernández-Vergara, O.; González-Calderón, N.; Russell-Guzmán, J.; Vicencio-Rojas, F.; Chacana-Cañas, C.; Ramírez-Vélez, R. A Before-school Physical Activity Intervention to Improve Cognitive Parameters in Children: The Active-Start Study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2020, 30, 108–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chou, C.-C.; Chen, K.-C.; Huang, M.-Y.; Tu, H.-Y.; Huang, C.-J. Can Movement Games Enhance Executive Function in Overweight Children? A Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2020, 39, 527–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, M.; Jäger, K.; Egger, F.; Roebers, C.M.; Conzelmann, A. Cognitively Engaging Chronic Physical Activity, But Not Aerobic Exercise, Affects Executive Functions in Primary School Children: A Group-Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 37, 575–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Bruijn, A.G.M.; Van Der Fels, I.M.J.; Renken, R.J.; Königs, M.; Meijer, A.; Oosterlaan, J.; Kostons, D.D.N.M.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Smith, J.; et al. Differential Effects of Long-Term Aerobic versus Cognitively-Engaging Physical Activity on Children’s Visuospatial Working Memory Related Brain Activation: A Cluster RCT. Brain Cogn. 2021, 155, 105812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Westendorp, M.; Houwen, S.; Hartman, E.; Mombarg, R.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. Effect of a Ball Skill Intervention on Children’s Ball Skills and Cognitive Functions. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2014, 46, 414–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Latorre-Román, P.; Berrios-Aguayo, B.; Aragón-Vela, J.; Pantoja-Vallejo, A. Effects of a 10-Week Active Recess Program in School Setting on Physical Fitness, School Aptitudes, Creativity and Cognitive Flexibility in Elementary School Children. A Randomised-Controlled Trial. J. Sports Sci. 2021, 39, 1277–1286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinto-Escalona, T.; Valenzuela, P.L.; Martin-Loeches, M.; Martinez-de-Quel, O. Individual Responsiveness to a School-based Karate Intervention: An Ancillary Analysis of a Randomized Controlled Trial. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2022, 32, 1249–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Bruijn, A.G.M.; Kostons, D.D.N.M.; Van Der Fels, I.M.J.; Visscher, C.; Oosterlaan, J.; Hartman, E.; Bosker, R.J. Effects of Aerobic and Cognitively-Engaging Physical Activity on Academic Skills: A Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Sports Sci. 2020, 38, 1806–1817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Etnier, J.; Labban, J.D.; Piepmeier, A.; Davis, M.E.; Henning, D.A. Effects of an Acute Bout of Exercise on Memory in 6th Grade Children. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 2014, 26, 250–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sallis, J.F.; McKenzie, T.L.; Kolody, B.; Lewis, M.; Marshall, S.; Rosengard, P. Effects of Health-Related Physical Education on Academic Achievement: Project SPARK. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 1999, 70, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aadland, K.N.; Ommundsen, Y.; Anderssen, S.A.; Brønnick, K.S.; Moe, V.F.; Resaland, G.K.; Skrede, T.; Stavnsbo, M.; Aadland, E. Effects of the Active Smarter Kids (ASK) Physical Activity School-Based Intervention on Executive Functions: A Cluster-Randomized Controlled Trial. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 2019, 63, 214–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oppici, L.; Rudd, J.R.; Buszard, T.; Spittle, S. Efficacy of a 7-Week Dance (RCT) PE Curriculum with Different Teaching Pedagogies and Levels of Cognitive Challenge to Improve Working Memory Capacity and Motor Competence in 8–10 Years Old Children. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2020, 50, 101675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takehara, K.; Togoobaatar, G.; Kikuchi, A.; Lkhagvasuren, G.; Lkhagvasuren, A.; Aoki, A.; Fukuie, T.; Shagdar, B.-E.; Suwabe, K.; Mikami, M.; et al. Exercise Intervention for Academic Achievement Among Children: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics 2021, 148, e2021052808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kolovelonis, A.; Goudas, M. Exploring the Effects of Three Different Types of Cognitively Challenging Physical Activity Games on Students’ Executive Functions and Situational Interest in Physical Education. Cogent Educ. 2022, 9, 2148448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brocken, J.E.A.; Kal, E.C.; Van Der Kamp, J. Focus of Attention in Children’s Motor Learning: Examining the Role of Age and Working Memory. J. Mot. Behav. 2016, 48, 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Fano, A.; Benzing, V.; Condello, G.; Ballester, R.; Tocci, N.; Marchetti, R.; Pesce, C.; Schmidt, M. How Divergent Are Children’s Divergent Movements? The Role of Cognition and Expertise in a Class-Randomized Cross-over Trial. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2023, 65, 102373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sánchez-López, M.; Cavero-Redondo, I.; Álvarez-Bueno, C.; Ruiz-Hermosa, A.; Pozuelo-Carrascosa, D.P.; Díez-Fernández, A.; Gutierrez-Díaz Del Campo, D.; Pardo-Guijarro, M.J.; Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. Impact of a Multicomponent Physical Activity Intervention on Cognitive Performance: The MOVI-KIDS Study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2019, 29, 766–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Den Berg, V.; Singh, A.S.; Komen, A.; Hazelebach, C.; Van Hilvoorde, I.; Chinapaw, M.J.M. Integrating Juggling with Math Lessons: A Randomized Controlled Trial Assessing Effects of Physically Active Learning on Maths Performance and Enjoyment in Primary School Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Telford, R.D.; Cunningham, R.B.; Fitzgerald, R.; Olive, L.S.; Prosser, L.; Jiang, X.; Telford, R.M. Physical Education, Obesity, and Academic Achievement: A 2-Year Longitudinal Investigation of Australian Elementary School Children. Am. J. Public Health 2012, 102, 368–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Der Fels, I.M.J.; Smith, J.; Roel, J.; Königs, M.; Oosterlaan, J.; Visscher, C.; Hartman, E. The Acute Effects of Two Types of Physical Activity in Physical Education on Response Inhibition and Lapses of Attention in Chil-dren Aged 8- 10 Years: A Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2020, 51, 158–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lakes, K.D.; Hoyt, W.T. Promoting Self-Regulation through School-Based Martial Arts Training. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 25, 283–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katz, D.L.; Cushman, D.; Reynolds, J.; Njike, V.; Treu, J.A.; Walker, J.; Smith, E.; Katz, C. Putting Physical Ac-tivity Where It Fits in the School Day: Preliminary Results of the ABC (Activity Bursts in the Classroom) for Fitness Program. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2010, 7, A82. [Google Scholar]
- Meijer, A.; Königs, M.; Pouwels, P.J.W.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Hartman, E.; Oosterlaan, J. Effects of Aerobic versus Cognitively Demanding Exercise Interventions on Brain Structure and Function in Healthy Children—Results from a Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychophysiology 2022, 59, e14034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinto-Escalona, T.; Gobbi, E.; Valenzuela, P.L.; Bennett, S.J.; Aschieri, P.; Martin-Loeches, M.; Paoli, A.; Mar-tinez-de-Quel, O. Effects of a School-Based Karate Intervention on Academic Achievement, Psychosocial Functioning, and Physical Fitness: A Multi-Country Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Sport Health Sci. 2024, 13, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiménez, J.E.; Hernández, S.; García, E.; Díaz, A.; Rodríguez, C.; Martín, R. Test de atención D2: Datos norma-tivos y desarrollo evolutivo de la atención en educación primaria. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 2012, 5, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rico-González, M.; Martín-Moya, R.; Carlos-Vivas, J.; Giles-Girela, F.J.; Ardigò, L.P.; González-Fernández, F.T. Is Cardiopulmonary Fitness Related to Attention, Concentration, and Academic Performance in Different Subjects in Schoolchildren? J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2025, 10, 272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubiales, J.; Bakker, L.; Urquijo, S. Estudio comparativo del control inhibitorio y la flexibilidad cognitiva en niños con TDAH. Cuad. Neuropsicol. Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 2013, 7, 50–69. [Google Scholar]
- Ballesteros, S. Memoria humana: Investigación y teoría. Psicothema 1999, 11, 705–723. [Google Scholar]
- Maddio, S.L.; Greco, C. Flexibilidad Cognitiva para Resolver Problemas entre Pares ¿Difiere esta Capacidad en Escolares de Contextos Urbanos y Urbanomarginales? Rev. Interam. Psicol. J. Psychol. 2010, 44, 98–109. [Google Scholar]
- Navarro, R.E. El rendimiento académico: Concepto, investigación y desarrollo. REICE Rev. Iberoam. Sobre Calid. Efic. Cambio Educ. 2016, 1, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiménez-Parra, J.F.; Belando-Pedreño, N.; Valero-Valenzuela, A. The Effects of the ACTIVE VALUES Program on Psychosocial Aspects and Executive Functions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2022, 20, 595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruiz-Ariza, A.; Suárez-Manzano, S.; López-Serrano, S.; Martínez-López, E.J. The Effect of Cooperative High-Intensity Interval Training on Creativity and Emotional Intelligence in Secondary School: A Random-ised Controlled Trial. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 2019, 25, 355–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zachopoulou, E.; Trevlas, E.; Konstadinidou, E. Archimedes Project Research Group The Design and Imple-mentation of a Physical Education Program to Promote Children’s Creativity in the Early Years. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2006, 14, 279–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
PICOS | Inclusion | Exclusion | Search Coherence | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | P | Population | Children from elementary school. | Children who do not attend elementary school. | (school OR “primary education” OR “elementary education”) |
2 | I | Intervention or Exposure | Children participating in physical education during school hours. | Children not participating in physical education during school hours. Intervention related to virtual reality. | “physical education” |
3 | C | Comparison | - | - | - |
4 | O | Outcome(s) | Outcomes related to cognition. | Results extracted from non-objective measures: teacher’s opinion, interviews, observations, perceptions, or experiences during a certain program. Program proposals without considering children in their studies. Study protocols. | “executive function*” OR cogniti* OR “academic performance” OR “academic achievement” OR “academic outcome*” OR “academic readiness” OR concentration OR memory OR attention |
5 | S | Study design | Randomized controlled trials. | Non-randomized controlled trials. | “randomized controlled trial*” |
6 | - | Other criteria | Peer-reviewed, original, full-text studies, written in English or Spanish. | Written in another language or without peers, reviewing the complete original text studies. | - |
Study | Random Sequence Generation | Allocation Concealment | Blinding of Participants and Personnel | Blinding of Outcome Assessment | Incomplete Outcome Data | Selective Reporting | Other Bias |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Garcia-Hermoso et al. [18] | |||||||
Chou et al. [19] | |||||||
Schmidt et al. [20] | |||||||
De Bruijn et al. [21] | |||||||
Westendorp et al. [22] | |||||||
Latorre- Román et al. [23] | |||||||
Pinto-Escalona et al. [24] | |||||||
De Bruijn et al. [25] | |||||||
Etnier et al. [26] | |||||||
Sallis et al. [27] | |||||||
Aadland et al. [28] | |||||||
Oppici et al. [29] | |||||||
Takehara et al. [30] | |||||||
Kolovelonis & Goudas [31] | |||||||
Brocken et al. [32] | |||||||
De Fano et al. [33] | |||||||
Sánchez-López et al. [34] | |||||||
Van Den Berg et al. [35] | |||||||
Telford et al. [36] | |||||||
Van Der Fels et al. [37] | |||||||
Lakes & Hoyt. [38] | |||||||
Katz et al. [39] | |||||||
Meijer et al. [40] | |||||||
Pinto-Escalona et al. [41] |
Reference | Aim | Sample | Intervention | Results | Conclusions | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Name/Groups | Intervention | Duration | Test Instrument | Variables | Results | ||||
Attention | |||||||||
García-Hermoso et al. [18] | Analyze the implementation of a before school PE classes in concentration and attention ability. | Nº children: 171. Schools: 3. Country: Chile. Age: 8−10. | EXP (n = 100) Active-Start | EXP did Active-Start program while CON did usual PE classes. | 8 weeks 5 times per week. | d2 Test of Attention. | Selective attention. Concentration capacity. | Although significant changes were not found in attention and concentration, the study revealed significant improvements in these variables when cardiorespiratory fitness was, at least, 3.05 and 0.70 mL/kg/min, respectively. | Purpose a PA program that enhance Vo2max before school classes may foster attention and concentration. |
CON (n = 70) Usual PE. | |||||||||
Chou et al. [19] | Evaluate the effects of a program on cognitive-related variables among overweight children. | Nº children: 84. Schools: 4. Country: Taiwan. Age: 10−12. | EXP (n = 44) Movement Games intervention. | EXP did Movement Games intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 8 weeks 3 times per week. | Stroop test. | Attention function. | Response accuracy rate: significant time effect in EXP (F(1, 43) = 71.45, p 01, partial η2 = 0.62 with moderate ES, whereas no change was noted in the CON, F(1, 39) = 3.24, p > 0.05). Response accuracy rate higher in EXP. Correct on time: significant time effect in EXP (F(1, 43) = 88.94, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.67 with moderate ES, but not in the control group, F(1, 39) = 1.67, p > 0.05). In addition, a significant group effect was noted in the post-test, t(82) = −6.04, p < 0.001, d = 0.84 with moderate to high ES. | Movement Games Intervention, involving children in MVPA, is suitable for improving attention abilities in overweight children. |
CON (n = 40) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Brocken et al. [32] | Explore the effects of age on attentional focus on motor learning. | Nº children: 60. Schools: doesn’t say. Country: Age: 8−12. | EXP1 (n = 30) External focus instructions | EXP1 did external focus instructions. while EXP2 did internal focus instruction. | 2 sessions. | AWMA. | Attentional focus. | Effects of attentional focus instruction on motor learning: the EXP1 achieved better results than EXP2. In detail: the pretest score was a significant covariate, F(1, 55) D 30.96, p < 0.001, h2 D 0.36. A main effect of instruction was found, F(1, 55) D 7.29, p D 0.009, h2 D 0.12, indicating that the EXP1 showed larger improvements in putting performance than the EXP2. No significant main effect of age, F(1, 55) D 1.06, p D 0.31, partial h2 D 0.02, and no significant interaction between age and instruction, F(1, 55) D 0.33, p D.57, partial h2 D 0.006. | External focus-based program is more effective than internal focus-based program. |
EXP2 (n = 30) Internal focus instructions. | |||||||||
De Fano et al. [33] | To assess the attentional predictors of divergent movement ability (DMA) in childhood and the role of sport and enriched PE experience. | Nº children: 242. Schools: 2. Country: Italy. Age: 10−11. | EXP (n = 129) Past enriched PE. | EXP did past enriched PE while CON did usual PE classes. | 6 months 1 time per week 1 h. | Line-bisection task. | Attentional skills. | Through regression analysis, the outcomes showed a nuanced prediction of individual DMA indices spatial attention ability. | The results identify novel cognitive determinants of children’s DMA. |
CON (n = 112) Past conventional PE. | |||||||||
Meijer et al. [40] | To examine the effects of two 14-week school-based exercise interventions on neurocognitive functioning. | Nº children: 93. Schools: 22. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 7−11. | EXP1 (n = 30) Aerobic exercise. | EXP1 did aerobic exercise and EXP2 did cognitively demanding exercise while CON did habitual PE classes. | 14 weeks 4 lesson per week. | WISC-III. | Alerting attention. Information processing. Spatial attention. | The results did not show statistical significative effects after both EXP1 or EXP2. | 14-week PA program based on aerobic exercise or cognitive-demanding exercise seems not effective enough to influence brain structure and brain function. |
EXP2 (n = 32) Cognitively demanding exercise. | |||||||||
CON (n = 31) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Van Der Fels et al. [37] | To assess the effectiveness of a program on children’s lapses of attention. | Nº children: 89. Schools: 24. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 8−10. | EXP (n = 43) Aerobic PA and Cognitively engaging PA. | EXP did aerobic PA and cognitively engaging PA intervention while CON did regular classroom lessons. | 2 days. | Performed pretest. Performed post-test. | Lapses of attention. | After PA intervention, the results did not show improvements in lapses of attention. | Acute PA did not have positive effects on lapses of attention. |
CON (n = 47) Regular classroom lessons. | |||||||||
Inhibition | |||||||||
Chou et al. [19] | To examine the effects of a program on overweight children’s executive function. | Nº children: 84. Schools: 4. Country: Taiwan. Age: 10−12. | EXP (n = 44) Movement Games intervention. | EXP did Movement Games intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 8 weeks 3 times per week. | Determination test. | Inhibitory control. | Mainly in the interference tendency condition, children that performed program of movement games showed improvements in inhibitory control, while no performance improvements was noted in the original PE lessons. | The program performance by children in EXP is suitable for improving inhibitory control. |
CON (n = 40) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Schmidt et al. [20] | To compare the effects of two different programs on executive functions. | Nº children: 181. Schools: 12. Country: Switzerland. Age: 10−12. | EXP1 (n = 69) Team games (high cognitive engagement, high physical exertion) | EXP1 did team games program, EXP2 did aerobic exercise program, and CON did a program based on low cognitive and low physical exertion. | 6 weeks 2 times per week. | Flanker task. | Inhibitory control. | Although the results showed that the data significantly better for inhibition, χ2 (1, N = 181) = 4200.36−4195.86 = 4.50, p < 0.05, it did not differ significantly between groups (F(2, 178) = 0.06, p = 0.947). | PA can affect executive function, although the change in inhibition did not differ between three different interventions. |
EXP2 (n = 57) Aerobic exercise (low cognitive engagement, high physical exertion). | |||||||||
CON (n = 55) Control condition (low cognitive engagement, low physical exertion). | |||||||||
Oppici et al. [29] | To evaluate different dance choreography-based different interventions affects executive functions. | Nº children: 74. Schools: 1. Country: Australia. Age: 8−10. | EXP1 (n = 30) High-cognitive intervention. | EXP1 did high-cognitive intervention, EXP2 did low-cognitive, and CON did usual PE classes. | 7 week. 2 lessons per week. | Flanker test. | Inhibitory control. | The results showed a statistically significant time effect (F(1,73) = 10.44, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.13) and no significant effect of group (p = 0.69) nor group time (p = 0.33). Pairwise comparisons showed a significant pre-to-post improvement in the CON group only (T [18] = 3.3, p < 0.01, D = 0.33 ± 0.21, d = 0.41). | Although initially the results showed that high-cognitive intervention support that it could improve inhibition control, not statistically significant differences between groups highlighted the importance of future research. |
EXP2 (n = 29) Low-cognitive group. | |||||||||
CON (n = 19) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Van Der Fels et al. [37] | To analyze the effects of acute exercise and cognitive engaging exercise on response inhibition and lapses of attention. | Nº children: 89. Schools: 24. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 8−10. | EXP (n = 43) Aerobic PA and cognitively engaging PA. | EXP did aerobic PA and cognitively engaging PA intervention while CON did regular classroom lessons. | 2 days. | Performed pretest. Performed post-test. | Response inhibition. Lapses of attention. | Regarding acute PA vs. seated classroom lesson, the study did not show significant improvements in the covariates model for response inhibition, Δχ2(1) = 0.08, p = 0.77, lapses of attention, Δχ2(1) = 0.97, p = 0.33, mean reaction time, Δχ2(1) = 0.46, p = 0.50, and μ, Δχ2(1) = −0.01, p = 0.91. Regarding aerobic vs. cognitively engaging PA, the study did not show significant improvements in the covariates model for response inhibition, Δχ2(2) = 0.07, p = 0.97, lapses of attention, Δχ2(2) = 0.91, p = 0.63, mean reaction time, Δχ2(2) = 3.69, p = 0.16, and μ, Δχ2(2) = 3.12, p = 0.21. | The acute exercise and these cognitive functions following the PA program did not show positive effects on response inhibition and lapses of attention. |
CON (n = 47). Regular classroom lessons. | |||||||||
Aadlamd et al. [28] | To evaluate the effects of curriculum prescribed program on executive functions. | Nº children: 1202 Schools: 57 Country: Norway Age: 10. | EXP (n = 596) Active Smarter Kids. | EXP did Active Smarter Kids intervention while CON did usual PA classes. | 7 months 135 min per week of PA. | Stroop test. | Inhibition. | The outcomes did not demonstrate any effects on inhibition function (mean group difference 0.2–1.2%, 0.01–0.06 SD units, p = 0.191−0.893) of children involved in EXP. | The intervention did not show that is a suitable option to increase executive functions. |
CON (n = 533) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Kolovelonis & Goudas [31] | To evaluate the effects of different intervention programs on executive functions. | Nº children: 140. Schools: 4. Country: Greece. Age: 11. | EXP1 (n = 29) PA games highlighting contextual interference. | EXP1 did PA games highlighting contextual interference, EXP2 did PA games highlighting mental control, EXP3 PA games highlighting discovery did, and CON did not participate in PE. | 3 45-min sessions per week | Design fluency test. | Inhibition. | Children from all EXP groups showed significative positive effects on inhibition compared to CON. | All intervention programs are effective to improve inhibition functions. |
EXP2 (n = 36) PA games highlighting mental control. | |||||||||
EXP3 (n = 36) Physical activity games highlighting discovery. | |||||||||
CON (n = 39) No PE . | |||||||||
Memory | |||||||||
Maijer et al. [40] | To analyze the effects of an exercise interventions on neurocognitive functioning. | Nº children: 93. Schools: 22. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 7−11. | EXP1 (n = 30) Aerobic exercise. | EXP1 did aerobic exercise and EXP2 did cognitively demanding exercise while CON did habitual PE classes. | 14 weeks 4 lesson per week. | WISC-III. | Visuospatial short-term memory. Verbal working memory. | There is no evidence for effects on visuospatial short-term memory and memory verbal working memory. | This study indicated that aerobic exercise or cognitive demanding exercise not necessarily improve memory. |
EXP2 (n = 32) Cognitively demanding exercise. | |||||||||
CON (n = 31) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Oppici et al. [29] | To evaluate different dance choreography-based different interventions affects executive functions. | Nº children: 74. Schools: 1. Country: Australia. Age: 8−10. | EXP1 (n = 30) High-cognitive intervention. | EXP1 did high-cognitive intervention, EXP2 did low-cognitive, and CON did usual PE classes. | 7 week 2 lessons per week. | Working memory test DCSS. | Memory capacity. | EXP1 children’s working memory significantly improves (p < 0.01; d = 0.51), while the low-cognitive (p = 0.04; d = 16 0.48) and control groups did not (p = 0.32; d = 0.17). However, difference between groups was not statistically significant. | Although outcomes of high-cognitive intervention support that it could improve working memory, not statistically significant differences between groups highlighted the importance of future research. |
EXP2 (n = 29) Low-cognitive group. | |||||||||
CON (n = 19) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Etnier et al. [26] | To compare the effects of an acute bout of exercise on learning, short-term memory, and long-term memory. | Nº children: 43. Schools: 1. Country: United States. Age: 11−12. | EXP (n = 24) PACER task intervention. | EXP did PACER task intervention program while CON did no-treatment control condition exercise. | 2 days. | RAVLT. | Short- and long-term memory. | Checking the word list, children in EXP achieved significantly better recall of the words after a brief delay. | This study supports acute bout of exercise to provide benefits for verbal learning and long-term memory. |
CON (n = 19) No-treatment control condition. | |||||||||
Kolovelonis & Goudas [31] | To evaluate the effects of different intervention programs on executive functions. | Nº children: 140. Schools: 4. Country: Greece. Age: 11. | EXP1 (n = 29) PA games highlighting contextual interference. | EXP1 did PA games highlighting contextual interference, EXP2 did PA games highlighting mental control, EXP3 PA games highlighting discovery did, and CON did not participate in PE. | 3 45-min sessions per week. | Design fluency test. | Memory capacity. | Comparing to CON, children in PA games highlighting both contextual interference (EXP1) and mental control (EXP2) significantly enhanced working memory. These games required students to engage in non-repeating, changing sequences of actions and to hold and manipulate information while reacting to signals. Such conditions likely prompted increased mental effort and rehearsal, contributing to improvements in working memory. | Non-repeating, changing sequences of actions and to hold and manipulate information while reacting to signal are suitable strategies for implementing during PA exercises for improving memory. |
EXP2 (n = 36) PA games highlighting mental control. | |||||||||
EXP3 (n = 36) PA games highlighting discovery. | |||||||||
CON (n = 39) No PE were involved. | |||||||||
Brocken et al. [32] | Explore the effects of age on attentional focus on motor learning. | Nº children: 60. Schools: - Country: - Age: 8−12. | EXP (n = 30) External focus instructions | EXP did external focus instructions while EXP2 did internal focus instruction. | 2 sessions. | AWMA. | Working memory capacity. | Younger children had lower verbal working memory ability than older children younger children, whereas no significant differences between groups existed. Age: a large main effect (F(1, 56) D 10.6, p < 0.01, partial h2 D 0.16), but not of instruction, F(1, 56) D 0.84, p D 0.36, partial h2 D 0.02). No interaction between age and instruction was found, F(1, 56) D 0.05, p D 0.83, partial h2 D 0.001. | In external and internal focus, instruction-based programs do not found the prediction of learning based on working memory capacity. |
EXP2 (n = 30) Internal focus instructions. | |||||||||
Van Den Berg et al. [35] | To assess the effectiveness of integrating juggling with math classes on multiplication memorization performance. | Nº children: 323. Schools: 9. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 10,4. | EXP (n = 170) Juggling intervention program. | EXP did juggling intervention program while CON did control program. | 5 weeks 20 sessions 5 to 8 min | CITO test battery A multiplication tables test. | Math memorization performance. | Children in EXP enjoyed more than children in sedentary math group. However, no significant effect were found on multiplication memorization performance. | Although no significant improvements were found, the increased enjoyment in the math-juggling group can sed light on for structurally incorporating physical activities in the classroom setting. |
CON (n = 153) Control program. | |||||||||
Cognitive flexibility | |||||||||
Westendorp et al. [22] | To evaluate the effects of ball-skills-based program on cognitive parameters of children with learning disorders. | Nº children: 91 Schools: 1 Country: The Netherlands Age: 7−11. | EXP (n = 45) ball skill intervention. | EXP did ball skill intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 16 weeks 2 times per week 40 min each session. | TMT | Cognitive flexibility. | The results did not show any effects on cognitive parameters. No significant correlations (P 9 0.05) between ball skills and cognitive flexibility during the intervention or 6 months after in both groups. | The program performed by children in EXP seem not effective to show improvements on cognitive parameters, at least, in children with learning disorders. |
CON (n = 46) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Latorre-Román et al. [23] | Evaluate the effects of active recess program on children’s cognitive flexibility. | Nº children: 114. Schools: 3. Country: Spain. Age: 8−12. | EXP (n = 58) Active Recess Program. | EXP did Active Recess program while CON did habitual class activities with no physical exercise | 10 weeks 3 times per week 30 min each activity. | TMT | Cognitive flexibility. | The intervention program shows significant improvements on children’s cognitive flexibility. In fact, children in EXP increase their cognitive flexibility greater (p < 0.05) than the children in CON (p < 0.05). | The Active Recess program is suitable to foster cognitive flexibility. |
CON (n = 56) Habitual activities with no physical exercise. | |||||||||
Aadlamd et al. [28] | To evaluate the effects of curriculum prescribed program on executive functions. | Nº children: 1,202 Schools: 57 Country: Norway Age: 10 | EXP (n = 596) Active Smarter Kids. | EXP did Active Smarter Kids intervention while CON did usual physical educational classes. | 7 months 135 min per week of PA. | TMT | Cognitive flexibility. | Following intention-to-treat analysis, children in EXP did not show any effects on their executive functions (mean group difference 0.2–1.2%, 0.01–0.06 SD units, p = 0.191–0.893). However, by conducting supplementary per protocol analyses, the authors obtained small significant effects of the intervention on the composite score of executive functions and cognitive flexibility. | Cognitively engaging and coordinative demanding activities/games could be viable options to improve executive functions and, it is possible that it can improve academic performance. |
CON (n = 533) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Oppici et al. [29] | To evaluate different dance choreography-based different interventions affects executive functions. | Nº children: 74. Schools: 1. Country: Australia. Age: 8−10. | EXP1 (n = 30) High-cognitive intervention. | EXP1 did high-cognitive intervention, EXP2 did low-cognitive, and CON did usual PE classes. | 7 week 2 lessons per week. | DCSS Flanker test. | Cognitive flexibility. | ANOVA showed a statistically significant time effect (F(1, 73) = 9.84, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.13), and no significant effect of group (p = 0.30) nor group time (p = 0.53) in the DCSS score. Pairwise comparisons did not show any statistically significant improvement in the three groups. The exploratory ANOVA showed a significant effect of time (F(1, 73) = 9.70, p < 476 0.01, ηp2 = 0.13). For the within-group pairwise comparisons, ANOVA showed no significant effects in all three groups. T-test showed that the males significantly improved their score (T[1,11] = 2.20, p = 0.015, D = 0.81 ± 0.62, d = 1.04) in EXP2. | Although outcomes of high-cognitive intervention support that it could improve cognitive flexibility, not statistically significant differences between groups highlighted the importance of future research. |
EXP2 (n = 29) Low-cognitive group. | |||||||||
CON (n = 19) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Takehara et al. [30] | To evaluate the effects of an exercise-based intervention on children’s academic achievement | Nº children: 2101. Schools: 10 Country: Mongolia. Age: 9−10. | EXP (n = 1069) High-intensity interval exercise program. | EXP did high-intensity interval exercise program while CON did usual physical educational classes. | 10 weeks 10−25 min per session. | Flanker test. | Cognitive function. | Between group comparison did not reveal any significant difference on cognitive functions. | High-intensity interval training did not show better strategy than usual PE classes. |
CON (n = 1032) Usual PE classes. | |||||||||
Kolovelonis & Goudas [31] | To evaluate the effects of different intervention programs on executive functions. | Nº children: 140. Schools: 4. Country: Greece. Age: 11. | EXP1 (n = 29) PA games highlighting contextual interference. | EXP1 did PA games highlighting contextual interference, EXP2 did PA games highlighting mental control, EXP3 PA games highlighting discovery did, and CON did not participate in physical education. | 3 45-min sessions per week. | Design fluency test. | Cognitive flexibility. | Children from all EXP groups showed significative positive effects on executive functions compared to CON, although no differences between three EXP groups was observed. | The study supports the implementation of cognitive challenging PA games to foster executive functions. |
EXP2 (n = 36) PA games highlighting mental control. | |||||||||
EXP3 (n = 36) Physical activity games highlighting discovery. | |||||||||
CON (n = 39) Without PE. | |||||||||
Sánchez-López et al. [34] | To analyze the effects of a multicomponent intervention on schoolchildren’s cognition. | Nº children: 240. Schools: 9. Country: Spain. Age: 5−7. | EXP (n = 82) Movi-Kids intervention. | EXP did Movi-Kids intervention while CON did usual physical educational classes. | 3 sessions weekly 80 sessions 60 min sessions | Battery of General and Differential Aptitudes. | Cognition changes. | When compared with CON, children in EXP experimented significant higher improvements after intervention than in pretest (p ≤ 0.05) (effect size ranged from 0.33 to 1.48). | The intervention program was more effective than habitual PE classes for improving cognitive-related variables. |
CON (n = 158) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Lakes & Hoyt. [38] | To evaluate the influence of tae kwon do-based program on math performance. | Nº children: 200. Schools: 1. Country: United States. Age: 10−11. | EXP (n = 100) Martial arts intervention (LEAD). | EXP did martial arts intervention (LEAD) while CON habitual PE classes. | 3 months 2−3 times per week. | WISC-III. | Cognitive self-regulation. | Children in the martial arts group showed greater self-regulation in response to a challenge than children in the CON for all three dimensions of self-regulation [Fs(1,174) = 11.18, 7.38, and 3.93, ps < 0.05 for cognitive, affective, and physical self-regulation, respectively]. Boys in EXP improved more their self-regulation than those in CON. | LEAD intervention is suitable for improving self-regulation. |
CON (n = 100) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Academic performance | |||||||||
García-Hermoso et al. [18] | To evaluate before school exercise program on school children academic performance. | Nº children: 171. Schools: 3. Country: Chile. Age: 8−10. | EXP (n = 100) Active-Start. | EXP did Active-Start program while CON did usual PE classes. | 8 weeks 5 times per week. | Children’s grades in the core subjects (mathematics and language). | Academic performance. | The children in EXP experimented improvements in both language (0.63; 95% CI 0.49 to 0.77) and mathematics (0.49; 95% CI 0.32 to 0.66) performance (p < 0.001). | Cardiorespiratory fitness through PA before school may fuel academic performance. |
CON (n = 70) Usual PE. | |||||||||
Westendorp et al. [22] | To evaluate the effects of ball-skills-based program on cognitive parameters of children with learning disorders. | Nº children: 91. Schools: 1. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 7−11. | EXP (n = 45) ball skill intervention. | EXP did ball skill intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 16 weeks 2 times per week 40 min each session. | TMT Dutch World in Numbers test. | Academic achievement. | Regarding cognitive related parameters, no intervention effects were found on the cognitive parameters. Further, no significant correlations were found between the changes in TOL performance (creativity test) and the changes in learning lags on reading and mathematics (all p values 9 0.05) during the intervention or six months later. | The ball skill intervention program did not show positive effects on academic performance |
CON (n = 46) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Pinto-Escalona et al. [41] | To examine the effects of a school-based karate intervention on academic achievement. | Nº children: 721. Schools: 20. Country: Spain, Portugal, France, Poland. Age: 7−8. | EXP (n = 388) School-based karate intervention. | EXP did school-based karate intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 1 year 2 h per week. | Curriculum evaluation criteria. | Academic performance. | Compared to CON, children in EXP improved significantly their academic achievement (d = 0.16; p = 0.003). | The study supports the use of martial arts-based program to enhance academic performance |
CON (n = 333) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
De Bruijn et al. [25] | To evaluate the effects of different school-based PE programs on children’s academic performance-related variables. | Nº children: 891. Schools: 22. Country: The Netherlands Age: 7,4-11,14. | EXP1 (n = 221) Aerobic intervention. | EXP1 did aerobic intervention, EXP2 did cognitively engaging program, and CON did a program based on low cognitive and low physical exertion. | 14 weeks 4 lessons per week. | Reading and mathematics tests provided by the government. Spelling test. | Academic achievement Reading performance Mathematic performance | Children with lower performance in reading at baseline performed better in reading at the post-test in the EXP2 group than in the CON (β = −0.06 (0.03), p = 0.03, 95% CI [−0.11 to −0.01]). No significant relation was found for the interaction between the dummy variable contrasting the EXP2 and the CON and baseline mathematics performance (β = −0.03 (0.04), p =.37, 95% CI [−0.11 to 0.04]), or baseline spelling performance (β = 0.07 (0.04), p = 0.06, 95% CI [−0.01 to 0.14]). | This study found no significant effects of two PA interventions on academic achievement, a conclusion that corroborates existing literature in which mixed findings on the effectiveness of PA are reported. |
EXP2 (n = 240) Cognitively-engaging program. | |||||||||
CON (n = 430) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Sallis et al. [27] | To evaluate a PE program on academic achievement. | Nº children: 754. Schools: 12. Country: United States. Age: 9. | EXP (n = 330) SPARK. | EXP did SPARK program while CON did usual PE classes. | 2 years 4 weeks 3 days per week. | Metropolitan Achievement Tests. | Language performance. Mathematics performance. Reading performance. | Match score: no effects Language score: the decline in percentile ranking was significantly less in the Trained Teacher condition than in the CON. Reading score: students in the EXP significantly increase in percentile ranking while the CON declined. | The results suggested that engaging children in more time of physical not necessarily influence academic performance. |
CON (n = 225) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Aadlamd et al. [28] | To evaluate the effects of curriculum prescribed program on executive functions. | Nº children: 1202. Schools: 57. Country: Norway. Age: 10. | EXP (n = 596) Active Smarter Kids. | EXP did Active Smarter Kids intervention while CON did usual PE classes. | 7 months 135 min per week of PA. | Verbal fluency test WISC-IV. | Verbal fluency. | Children in EXP did not show significant effects on participants verbal fluency (the mean group difference for verbal fluency was 0.17 (−0.31 to 0.65), with a p-value of 0.484 in the completers-only analysis). | The evaluated program is not effective for improving children’s verbal fluency. |
CON (n = 533) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Takehara et al. [30] | To evaluate the effects of an exercise-based intervention on children’s academic achievement. | Nº children: 2101. Schools: 10 Country: Mongolia. Age: 9−10. | EXP (n = 1069) Intervention program. | EXP did intervention program while CON did usual PE classes. | 10 weeks 10–25 min per session. | Mongolian national language scores. Flanker test. | Academic achievement. Cognitive function. | Compared to children in CON, the program performed by children in EXP improves: total examination scores, mathematics scores, and Mongolian language scores. When compared to CON, children in EXP recruited from urban areas showed an 8.36-point improvement (95% CI: 6.06 to 10.66) in academic scores, whereas those in a mixed residential area showed a 9.55-point improvement (95% confidence interval: 6.58 to 12.51). | The program performed by EXP is suitable for fueling academic achievement. |
CON (n = 1032) Control group. | |||||||||
De Fano et al. [33] | To evaluate the effects of enriched PE program on children fluency and flexibility. | Nº children: 242. Schools: 2. Country: Italy. Age: 10−11. | EXP (n = 129) Past enriched PE. | EXP did past enriched PE while CON did usual PE classes. | 6 months 1 time per week. 1 h. | RNG task GPAI. | Fluency. Flexibility. | Fluency The results showed significant interaction between CON and EXP was found for fluency (F(1, 134) = 4.74, p = 0.048) and switching (F(1, 165) = 4.69, p = 0.032). Flexibility Compared to the children in CON at the end of the intervention, children in EXP showed higher scores (Cohen’s d = 0.46) and lower originality values (−0.33). | The results showed positive effects on fluency and flexibility with continued participation in the program. |
CON (n = 112) Past conventional PE. | |||||||||
Pinto-Escalón et al. [41] | To evaluate the effects of Karate-based program on children’s academic achievement. | Nº children: 388. Schools: 20. Country: Spain, Portugal, France, Germany, and Poland. Age: 7−8. | EXP (n = 388) Karate Mind and Movement program. | EXP did Karate Mind and Movement program. | 1 year 2 h per week. | GPA. | Academic performance. | Responders for the SDQ presented higher SDQ scores (i.e., higher psychosocial difficulties) at baseline than non-responders (p < 0.001). Responders for academic performance were mostly males (p = 0.017), with an older age (p = 0.030), and with worse academic performance (p < 0.001) at baseline compared with non-responders, and tended to present higher SDQ scores (p = 0.055). Responders for one outcome obtained greater benefits from the EXP on the other outcome (e.g., responders for SDQ improved academic performance [p < 0.001] compared with non-responders). | Although the results of CON were not included in the statistical analysis, EXP seems particularly effective for children with psychosocial difficulties and low academic performance. |
CON (n = not included in the analysis). | |||||||||
Telford et al. [36] | To evaluate the effects of specialist-taught PE on academic development. | Nº children: 620. Schools: 13. Country: Australia. Age: 8−9. | EXP (n = 312) Specialist-taught PE. | EXP did specialist-taught PE while CON did common-practice PE. | 2 years 150 min per week. | Local government education tests. | Writing proficiency. Numeracy proficiency. Reading proficiency. | While no evidence was found on children’s reading effects, children in EXP improved greater on numeracy (p < 0.03) and writing (p = 0.13) scores. | The results support the role of PE in academic development. |
CON (n = 308) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Lakes & Hoyt. [38] | To evaluate the tae kwon do-based program on math performance. | Nº children: 200. Schools: 1. Country: United States Age: 10−11. | EXP (n = 100) Martial arts intervention (LEAD). | EXP did martial arts intervention (LEAD) while CON habitual PE classes. | 3 months. 2–3 times per week. | WISC-III. | Math performance. | Compared with CON, children in EXP improved performance on a mental math test. | The martial arts based program seems suitable for improving match performance. |
CON (n = 100) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Katz et al. [39] | To assess the effects of a PA-based program on health-related outcomes. | Nº children: 1214. Schools: 5. Country: United States. Age: 7–10. | EXP (n = 655) Activity Bursts in the classroom | EXP did activity bursts in the classroom intervention while CON normal curricular activities. | 1 year 30 min per day. | Missouri Academic Performance scores. | Academic performance. | Based on MAP achievement level scores: no significant differences between groups in reading (p = 0.35) and mathematics (p = 0.15). Based on ISD progress reports: in mathematics (28.6% vs. 20.8%, p < 0.001) and reading (21.1% vs. 16.1%, p = 0.01) a greater proportion of students whose academic performance improved compared to that of the EXP. | The results did not show that the intervention program was more suitable than habitual PE classes for improving academic performance. |
CON (n = 559) Normal curricular activities. | |||||||||
Creativity | |||||||||
Westendorp et al. [22] | Evaluate the effects of a program based on ball skills on primary school children’s problem-solving ability. | Nº children: 91 (with learning disorders). Schools: 1. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 7−11. | EXP (n = 45) ball skill intervention. | EXP did ball skill intervention program while CON did usual PEclasses. | 16 weeks 2 times per week 40 min each session. | Tower of London test. | Problem solving skills. | The intervention group showed changes in problem-solving mainly in those who showed larger improvements in ball skills (r = 0.41, p = 0.007). | Implementing activities for improving ball skills may help for enhancing problem-solving abilities. |
CON (n = 46) Habitual PE classes. | |||||||||
Ángel Latorre-Román et al. [23] | Assess the effects of active recess-based program on schoolchildren ability. | Nº children: 114. Schools: 3. Country: Spain. Age: 8−12. | EXP (n = 58) Active Recess Programme. | EXP did Active Recess program while CON did habitual class activities with no physical exercise. | 10 weeks 3 times per week 30 min each activity. | PIC-N. | Creativity. | The EXP group showed significant improvements showed larger increases in EXP than in CON in creativity (CON = p < 0.05; EXP = p < 0.001). | Active Recess based strategies are suitable for improve creativity. |
CON (n = 56) Habitual activities with no physical exercise. | |||||||||
De Fano et al. [33] | Analyze the effects of a program on schoolchildren’s different abilities. | Nº children: 242. Schools: 2. Country: Italy. Age: 10−11. | EXP (n = 129) Past enriched PE. | EXP did past enriched PE while CON did usual PE classes. | 6 months 1 time per week 1 h. | GPAI. | Originality. | EXP showed significant differences (F(1, 165) = 5.56, p = 0.019; ES of Cohen’s d = −0.33). Children in EXP showed better values in originality than CON. | Enriched PE classes are more suitable for enhancing originality, at least in short term. |
CON (n = 112) Past conventional PE. | |||||||||
Others | |||||||||
De Bruijn et al. [21] | Compare the effects of different programs on brain activation. | Nº children: 92. Schools: 22. Country: The Netherlands. Age: 8−10. | EXP1 (n = 30) Aerobic group. | EXP1 did aerobic program, EXP2 did cognitively engaging program, and CON did a program based on low cognitive and low physical exertion. | 30 min 4 lesson per week 14 week. | The Spatial Span tasks. | Brain activation. | Pretest: no significant differences between groups on the spatial span task (F (2, 59) = 0.15, p = 0.86). Post-test: in general, significant differences between pretest and post-test were found (F (1, 59) = 12.32, p < 0.001). However, brain activation was no significantly differ between both intervention groups. | Both aerobic group and cognitive engaging group are suitable for enhance brain activation. |
EXP1 (n = 31) Cognitively engaging group. | |||||||||
CON (n = 31) Habitual PE classes. |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Brioa Saez, G.; Rico-González, M.; Monge Gómez, N. The Effect of Physical-Activity-Based Programs on School Children’s Cognitive Competence-Related Variables: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Sports 2025, 13, 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080261
Brioa Saez G, Rico-González M, Monge Gómez N. The Effect of Physical-Activity-Based Programs on School Children’s Cognitive Competence-Related Variables: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Sports. 2025; 13(8):261. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080261
Chicago/Turabian StyleBrioa Saez, Gorka, Markel Rico-González, and Natalia Monge Gómez. 2025. "The Effect of Physical-Activity-Based Programs on School Children’s Cognitive Competence-Related Variables: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials" Sports 13, no. 8: 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080261
APA StyleBrioa Saez, G., Rico-González, M., & Monge Gómez, N. (2025). The Effect of Physical-Activity-Based Programs on School Children’s Cognitive Competence-Related Variables: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Sports, 13(8), 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports13080261