Next Article in Journal
Moderate-to-Severe Malnutrition Identified by the Controlling Nutritional Status (CONUT) Score Is Significantly Associated with Treatment Failure of Periprosthetic Joint Infection
Next Article in Special Issue
Untargeted Metabolome Analysis of Alcohol-Exposed Pregnancies Reveals Metabolite Differences That Are Associated with Infant Birth Outcomes
Previous Article in Journal
Genome-Wide Association Analysis of Over 170,000 Individuals from the UK Biobank Identifies Seven Loci Associated with Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) Diet
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder and Iron Homeostasis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Mediating and Moderating Effects of Iron Homeostasis Alterations on Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits

by
R. Colin Carter
1,2,*,
Neil C. Dodge
3,
Christopher D. Molteno
4,
Ernesta M. Meintjes
2,
Joseph L. Jacobson
2,3 and
Sandra W. Jacobson
2,3,4
1
Departments of Emergency Medicine and Pediatrics, Institute of Human Nutrition, Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY 10032, USA
2
Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town 7925, South Africa
3
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, MI 48201, USA
4
Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town 7925, South Africa
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(20), 4432; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204432
Submission received: 1 September 2022 / Revised: 3 October 2022 / Accepted: 17 October 2022 / Published: 21 October 2022

Abstract

:
We have previously demonstrated prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE)-related alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis. Given that early iron deficiency and PAE both lead to growth restriction and deficits in recognition memory and processing speed, we hypothesized that PAE-related iron homeostasis alterations may mediate and/or moderate effects of PAE on growth and neurobehavior. We examined this hypothesis in a prenatally recruited, prospective longitudinal birth cohort [87 mother-infant pairs with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (mean = 7.2 drinks/occasion on 1.4 days/week); 71 controls], with serial growth measures and infant neurobehavioral assessments. PAE was related to growth restriction at 2 weeks and 5 years, and, in infancy, poorer visual recognition memory, slower processing speed, lower complexity of symbolic play, and higher emotionality and shyness on a parental report temperament scale. Lower maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio, which we have shown to be associated with PAE, appeared to exacerbate PAE-related 2-week head circumference reductions, and elevated maternal ferritin, which we have shown to be associated with PAE, appeared to exacerbate PAE-related visual recognition memory deficits. In causal inference analyses, PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin and hemoglobin:log(ferritin) appeared to statistically mediate 22.6–82.3% of PAE-related growth restriction. These findings support potential mechanistic roles of iron homeostasis alterations in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD).

1. Introduction

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) is the most common preventable cause of developmental disability worldwide. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) affect 2.0–7.1% of the school age population in the US and Western Europe and 13.6–30.6% in South Africa [1,2,3,4]. FASD are characterized by a range of deficits in neurobehavior and cognition; fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) and partial FAS (PFAS) are also characterized by specific facial dysmorphology, small head circumference, and/or growth restriction [5,6]. Teratogenic effects of alcohol have been reported in every organ system [6,7,8,9,10].
Both human and animal studies have demonstrated alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis induced by PAE. In our prospective, longitudinal birth cohort study in Cape Town, South Africa, infants exposed to prenatal binge drinking were 3.6 times more likely to have iron deficiency anemia (IDA) at 12 months than infant whose mothers did not binge drink [11]. The mechanisms underlying this PAE-related increase in IDA were unclear, as data regarding maternal and fetal iron status were not available. More recently, in a subsequent Cape Town prospective longitudinal birth cohort, we replicated our earlier finding of PAE-related increases in infant IDA and elucidated potential maternal-fetal mechanisms [12]. In the mother, alcohol consumption was associated with elevations in prenatal ferritin and hepcidin, as well as reductions in hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio, signifying a shift of iron into storage at the expense of other biologic processes such as erythropoiesis and, possibly, placental iron transfer. Indeed, in analyses of placental tissue samples stained for iron transport proteins, PAE was associated with decreases in the ratio of ferroportin-1 to transferrin-receptor-1, which is seen when iron on the maternal side is insufficient to meet the needs of the placenta and fetus [13]. PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin and reductions in hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio partially mediated a similar shift of iron into storage at the expense of hematopoeisis in the neonate, as evidenced by PAE-related decreases in hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio and increases in ferritin at age 2 weeks. PAE-related decreases in neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio, in turn, partially mediated the relation of PAE to increased IDA prevalence at 6.5 months. Notably, all of the PAE-related alterations in maternal, placental, and infant iron homeostasis seen were independent of maternal dietary iron intake and use of prenatal iron supplements, which were similar between heavy drinking women and abstainers/light-drinkers. Smith and colleagues demonstrated similar findings in a series of experimental rat models, including PAE-related increases in maternal and fetal anemia and hepcidin, as well as decreased fetal brain iron despite increased iron stores in the fetal liver [14,15,16,17].
Since infant iron deficiency (ID) has been associated with a range of long-term neurodevelopmental deficits [18,19,20], the alterations in iron homeostasis we have found to be related to PAE may have important implications in FASD. Miller and colleagues found that PAE decreased total brain tissue iron despite increases in tissue ferritin [21]. PAE and ID in early infancy are both associated with: growth restriction; poorer recognition memory, information processing speed, and eyeblink conditioning; and higher emotionality and lower activity on temperament measures [9,19,20,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32]. Furthermore, in our original human birth cohort, PAE-related growth restriction was more severe among children who had had IDA in infancy [7,11]. Similarly, in Smith and colleagues’ rat models [14,17,33], PAE-related infant ID exacerbated teratogenic effects of PAE on growth and behavior. We thus hypothesized that PAE-related alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis mediate and/or moderate effects of PAE on growth, neurobehavior, and temperament. We tested these hypotheses in data from our more recent prospective longitudinal birth cohort.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

From 2011–2015 we recruited women from two antenatal clinics in Cape Town, South Africa, to participate in a prospective, longitudinal cohort study examining developmental effects of PAE [34,35]. Alcohol consumption at recruitment was ascertained in timeline follow-back interviews [36,37]. If a woman averaged at least 1.0 oz (30 mL) absolute alcohol (AA)/day (≈1.67 standard drinks) or reported binge drinking (≥2.0 oz (60 mL) AA/drinking occasion), she was invited to participate. Women who abstained or drank only minimally (with no binge episodes) were invited to participate as controls. Maternal exclusion criteria at recruitment included age <18 years, HIV infection, multiple gestation pregnancy, and pharmacologic treatment for medical conditions. Infant exclusion criteria were chosen prior to study initiation due to potential developmental consequences of such conditions; they included major chromosomal anomalies, seizures, neural tube defects, very low birthweight (<1500 g), and extreme prematurity (weeks gestation <32 weeks). We have previously reported that infants with missing data were similar to those seen at 2 weeks and 6.5 months in terms of maternal demographics, alcohol and drug use, weeks gestation at study entry, and maternal dietary iron intake, iron supplement use, ferritin, and hemoglobin [12]. The final sample included 158 mother-infant pairs (87 with heavy PAE, 71 controls). Consents and interviews were conducted in the mother’s preferred language (Afrikaans or English). Women reporting alcohol or drug use were counseled by our staff about the risks of prenatal alcohol and drug use and were encouraged to stop or reduce their alcohol and/or drug use. Women were also offered referrals to substance use treatment programs (South African National Council on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence, Belleville, South Africa).

2.2. Ascertainment of Maternal Alcohol, Cigarette Smoking, and Drug Use

At recruitment and 4 and 12 weeks later, women were interviewed about their alcohol consumption on a day-by-day basis during the previous 2 weeks, with recall linked to specific times of daily activities in timeline follow-back interviews [36,37]. Women were also interviewed regarding cigarette and drug use (cocaine, methamphetamine, opiates, methaqualone, and marijuana); drug use reports were validated by urine ELISA drug testing [8]. Three summary alcohol measures were calculated by averaging across pregnancy: oz AA/day, oz AA/drinking occasion, and frequency of drinking (1 oz = 30 mL). Based on prior studies in this cohort [36,38], drinking days/week was used as the PAE predictor variable except where otherwise noted.

2.3. Hematological and Biochemical Iron Indices

Venous blood samples were obtained from mothers at recruitment (mean weeks gestation = 25.6; visit 1 in Table 1) and 12 weeks later (mean weeks gestation = 34.0; visit 2 in Table 1) and from infants at 2 weeks and 6.5 months postpartum (corrected for prematurity) and analyzed for complete blood count and serum ferritin. Ferritin is a measure of iron in storage. C-reactive protein and serum soluble transferrin receptor concentrations were assayed on maternal samples and infant samples at 6.5-months. Maternal values from the two visits were averaged. Anemia of inflammation for mothers was defined as normocytic (mean corpuscular volume (MCV) ≥ 80 fL) anemia with ferritin > 30 µg/L [39]. Hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio was calculated to assess the balance of iron available for erythropoiesis (hemoglobin) vs. iron in storage (ferritin) [12]. Infant ID at age 6.5 months was defined as (1) serum ferritin < 12 µg/L or (2) in cases with CRP > 5 mg/L, elevated sTfR (>8.5 mg/L for mothers; >8.3 mg/L at 6.5 months) [40,41,42]. Anemia was defined as hemoglobin < 11.0 g/dL [42]. Iron deficiency anemia was defined as the presence of both iron deficiency and anemia.

2.4. Demographics and Control Variables

Women were interviewed at prenatal visits regarding their demographic background, including age, gravidity, and education. Weeks gestation at delivery was calculated based on early pregnancy ultrasound; when this was not available, last menstrual period was used. Infant sex and birthweight were obtained from medical records.

2.5. Infant Anthropometric Outcomes

Weight, length, and head circumference were measured at ages 2 weeks and 5 years by research assistants (trained by RCC) using standard WHO protocols [43]. Age/sex-specific z-scores were calculated for length/height, weight, at both ages and head circumference at 2 weeks using WHO norms. Raw values (cm) were used for analyses of head circumference at age 5 years since WHO reference norms are not available for this measure at that age.

2.6. Infant Neurobehavioral and Temperament Outcomes

Visual recognition memory, and processing time, an inverse measure of processing speed, were assessed at 6.5 months using the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence [44]. In this assessment, the infant is seated on the mother’s lap and shown two identical photos, followed by a novel photo paired with the familiar one. At this age, the normative response is to look at the novel stimulus for a longer time period, which indicates that the infant is able to recall the familiar stimulus and discriminate it from the novel stimulus. Infant fixation time was recorded on a computer, and visual recognition memory, assessed as percent novelty preference, was computed by dividing duration of time looking at the novel stimulus by total time looking at the paired familiar and novel stimuli for each of 10 novel stimuli problems. In addition, as described in Colombo et al. [45], mean fixation duration (processing time) was computed for each problem by dividing the total duration looking time by the number of looks. A pattern of shorter looks reflects faster processing of information and has been found to relate to faster infant reaction time [46]. Symbolic play with objects emerges during development in a hierarchical sequence of stages, with increasing complexity at each stage [47]. We assessed complexity of play using the procedure developed by Belsky and colleagues [48] and adapted by Jacobson and colleagues [49]. First, 10 min of free play with a standard set of toys is observed. Suggestion and modeling are subsequently utilized to elicit progressively higher levels of play complexity than those spontaneously demonstrated by the infant. Complexity of elicited play is scored as the highest of 14 levels successfully imitated by the infant. Infant temperament was assessed by maternal report at 12 months postpartum using the Emotionality, Activity and Sociability (EAS) Temperament Survey, a 20-item questionnaire, which assesses emotionality, activity, sociability, and shyness [50]. Each of these dimensions of temperament is measured by five items (e.g., tends to be shy, cries easily, likes to be with people), each of which is rated on a 5-point scale.

2.7. Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (v.24; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) and Stata (v.16.1; Statcorp, College Station, TX, USA). All variables were examined for normality of distribution; oz AA/day and ferritin values were log-transformed due to skewness (>3.0). All missing data were treated with list-wise deletion. Analyses were two-sided (α = 0.05). Linear regression models were constructed to examine associations of PAE and iron indices to infant outcomes, first in univariate models and then in multivariable models adjusting for potential confounders. Potential confounders for growth outcomes were those associated with PAE-related growth restriction in previous studies [7]: maternal age, smoking (no. prenatal cigarettes/day), maternal education, and weeks gestation at delivery. Potential confounders for neurobehavioral and temperament outcomes were those related to a given outcome at p ≤ 0.10 in univariate regression models (see Table S1). Interaction effects between PAE and iron indices were examined by constructing linear regression models regressing the infant outcome on PAE, a given iron measure, a PAE by iron measure interaction term, and potential confounders. Given the low power of interaction terms in observational studies, interaction effects were inferred when the p-value for the interaction term was ≤0.10 [51]. For any model with evidence of a PAE by iron measure interaction effect, two sets of stratified results were then examined: (1) the relation of the iron measure to the given outcome stratified by alcohol exposure (controls vs. heavy exposure); (2) the relation of PAE to the outcome stratified by the given iron measure (tertiles for continuous measures, yes vs. no for categorical variables (e.g., IDA)). To examine the hypothesis that relations of PAE to infant growth and neurobehavioral outcomes are mediated by alterations in iron indices, mediation analysis was conducted based on marginal structural models and the product method (paramed package, Stata) for variable triads where PAE (exposure) and a previously identified [12] PAE-related iron measure (potential mediator) were both related to an infant outcome [52,53]. This method allows for estimation of natural direct effects (exposure→outcome, independent of the potential mediator), natural indirect effects (exposure→outcome, through the potential mediator), and total effects with exposure-mediator interactions, with control for covariates from the relevant regression models described above. PAE-mediator interaction was modeled with drinking frequency increasing from 0 to 3 days/week to mimic the weekend drinking pattern seen in this cohort.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Women were in their 3rd decade on average, and heavy drinking mothers were 2.4 years older than controls (Table 1). Both heavy drinking and control mothers were poorly educated with heavy drinking women averaging 0.7 fewer years of school than controls. Heavy drinking mothers averaged 4.3 oz (129.0 mL) absolute alcohol (~7.2 standard drinks) per occasion on 1.4 days per week across pregnancy. Among drinking mothers, binge drinking was very common, with 93.1% averaging ≥2 oz (60 mL) AA/occasion. Controls abstained from drinking during pregnancy except for 8 women. These 8 all reported light drinking with no binges; 4 women reported 1–3 drinking occasions across pregnancy; 2 women reported monthly drinking occasions; 1 reported drinking once a week; and 1 drank 2.1 oz (63 mL) absolute alcohol (~3.5 drinks) on one occasion. Although cigarette smoking was more common among heavy drinking women than controls, number of cigarettes/day was similarly low in both groups (86.7% < 0.5 pack/day; 1.9% ≥ 1.0 pack/day). Co-exposures were common; 0.7% of heavy drinking women reported marijuana use vs. 11% of controls; 13.8% reported methamphetamine use vs. 15.5% of control women; methaqualone, cocaine, and opiate use were not reported by any participant. PAE was not associated with weeks gestation at delivery. Most women (86.7%) delivered at term. Infants born to heavy drinking mothers were more likely to be small for gestational age and were smaller than control infants across almost every anthropometric measure in the neonatal period and at 5 years.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Controls
(n = 71)
Heavy Drinking Mothers
(n = 87)
nM (SD) or n (%)nM (SD) or n (%)p a
Maternal characteristics
 Maternal age at conception (years)7125.5 (4.9)8727.9 (6.1)0.003
 Gravidity (no. live births (%))711.5 (1.3)871.8 (1.5)0.090
 Education (years school completed)709.9 (1.7)879.3 (1.6)0.007
 Marital status, no. married (%)7130 (42.3)8725 (28.7)0.076
 Weeks gestation at maternal visit 17126.4 (5.2)8725.0 (5.6)0.056
 Weeks gestation at maternal visit 24434.6 (3.8)5533.5 (4.1)0.083
 Iron intake from diet only (mg/day) b7112.4 (3.7)8711.9 (2.9)0.188
  n (%) with inadequate intake c7167 (94.4)8786 (98.9)0.109
 Iron/folic acid supplementation, n (%)7058 (82.9)8572 (84.7)0.755
  Took supplement on “most days”5856 (96.6)7270 (97.2)0.826
Alcohol and drug use
 Alcohol consumption:71 87
  oz AA/day 0.0 (0.0) 0.9 (1.2)<0.001
  oz AA/drinking day 0.1 (0.4) 4.3 (2.4)<0.001
  Drinking days/week 0.0 (0.1) 1.4 (1.1)<0.001
n (%) reporting cigarette smoking7149 (69.0)8771 (81.6)0.065
  Cigarettes/day among smokers495.8 (3.8)716.7 (4.4)0.121
n (%) reporting marijuana use718 (11.3)8718 (20.7)0.026
  Marijuana days/month among users84.0 (4.7)189.7 (9.0)0.047
n (%) reporting methamphetamine use7111 (15.5)8712 (13.8)0.763
  Methamphetamine days/month among users1110.2 (8.9)124.3 (5.4)0.033
Infant characteristics
 Weeks gestation at delivery7139.1 (1.8)8738.8 (2.1)0.223
  Preterm delivery (<37 weeks), n (%) 9 (12.7) 12 (13.8)0.837
 Infant sex, no. female (%)7133 (46.5)8745 (51.7)0.512
 Infant birthweight (g)703078.8 (534.5)872885.4 (609.5)0.019
  No. small for gestational age (%) d7013 (18.6)8430 (35.7)0.018
 2-week z-scores e
  Length61−1.3 (1.3)69−1.7 (1.4)0.083
  Weight61−0.8 (1.2)69−1.3 (1.4)0.029
  Head circumference61−0.4 (1.2)69−0.9 (1.5)0.016
 5-year z-scores e
  Height67−0.5 (0.9)80−1.0 (0.9)0.005
  Weight67−0.2 (1.0)80−0.9 (0.8)<0.001
 5-year head circumference (cm)6750.3 (1.6)8048.7 (3.4)<0.001
AA = absolute alcohol; 1 oz AA = 30 mL AA = 1.67 standard drinks. a From independent samples t-tests for continuous outcomes, chi-square for binary outcomes. b From three multipass 24-h recall dietary interviews. c Defined as dietary iron intake <23 mg/day if age <19 years; <22 mg/day if age 19 years and older [54]. d Defined as birthweight <10th percentile for gestational age [55]. e Age- and sex-specific z-scores from World Health Organization norms [43].

3.2. Relation of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure and Maternal and Infant Iron Indices to Neonatal and Postnatal Anthropometric Measures

As we have previously reported [12], PAE was related to smaller length- and weight-for-age z-scores at age 2 weeks (Table 2A). Each additional day/week of prenatal drinking was associated with a reduction of 0.28 SD for length and 0.21 SD for weight. PAE was associated with smaller head circumference-for-age z-scores at age 2 weeks in univariate analyses, but this relation was no longer statistically significant after adjusting for potential confounders. At age 5 years (Table 2B), PAE was related to smaller length and weight z-scores as well as smaller head circumference (cm). For length, the magnitude of effect was slightly larger at 2 weeks when compared to age 5 years (B = −0.28 and −0.19, respectively) and remarkably similar at the two ages for weight (B = −0.21 and −0.22, respectively). Each day/week of prenatal drinking was associated with a 5-year head circumference reduction of 0.73 cm.
Higher maternal prenatal ferritin (an indicator of iron in storage) and lower hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio [a measure of iron available for erythropoiesis (hemoglobin) vs. iron in storage (ferritin)] were both associated with smaller weight, length, and head circumference z-scores at age 2 weeks (Table 2A). A trend (p < 0.10) was seen for smaller length among infants whose mothers had anemia of inflammation during pregnancy. Higher infant hemoglobin at age 2 weeks was associated with larger weight, length, and head circumference z-scores in multivariable models. Higher maternal prenatal ferritin and lower hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio were associated with smaller height and weight z-scores at age 5 years (Table 2B). Neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio was positively associated with 5-year height z-scores. No other infant iron indices were related to infant anthropometric measures.

3.3. Relation of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure and Maternal and Infant Iron Indices to Infant Neurobehavior and Temperament

PAE was related to poorer visual recognition memory at age 6.5 months in univariate analyses and multivariable models adjusting for potential confounders (Table 3A); this association was not seen at age 12 months. At age 12 months PAE was associated with longer processing time, i.e., slower processing speed, and lower complexity of symbolic play. PAE was also associated with greater emotionality and shyness on the EAS temperament scales (Table 3B).
Higher maternal prenatal hemoglobin was associated with shorter 6.5-month processing time, i.e., faster processing speed. Higher neonatal ferritin was associated with poorer visual recognition memory at 12 months (Table 3A). Lower 6.5-month ferritin and the presence of ID at 6.5 months were both associated with longer 12-month processing time. Univariate associations between presence of maternal anemia of inflammation, neonatal hemoglobin, and 6.5-month ferritin ID, and IDA with symbolic play performance were no longer seen after adjustment for potential confounders. Higher maternal prenatal hemoglobin was associated with decreased sociability and increased shyness (Table 3B). A trend (p < 0.10) was seen for higher neonatal ferritin relating to increased emotionality. Higher 6.5-month ferritin was associated with increased activity. ID at age 6.5 months was associated with increased emotionality, decreased activity, and increased shyness; IDA was also associated with decreased activity.

3.4. Interaction Effects between Prenatal Alcohol Exposure and Maternal and Infant Iron Indices on Growth, Neurobehavior, and Temperament

Results from regression analyses examining potential interaction effects for PAE by maternal and infant iron indices on growth and neurobehavioral outcomes are presented in Supplementary Table S2. Table 4 displays stratified models for iron indices with evidence of interaction effects with PAE. Since interaction effects are 2-way, two sets of stratified results are presented: (1) the relation of the iron measure to the given outcome stratified by alcohol exposure (controls vs. heavy exposure); (2) the relation of PAE to the outcome stratified by the given iron measure (tertiles for continuous measures, yes vs. no for categorical outcomes (e.g., IDA). Higher maternal and neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratios were both associated with larger neonatal length z-scores among exposed infants but not controls. Higher neonatal hemoglobin and hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio were both associated with higher neonatal weight z-scores among exposed infants but not controls. A trend for lower weight z-scores among exposed infants vs. controls was seen among infants in the lower third for neonatal hemoglobin that was not seen in the other groups. A trend for higher weight z-scores among exposed infants vs. controls was seen among infants in the upper third for neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio that was not seen in the other groups. Among exposed infants but not controls, higher maternal ferritin and lower maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio were associated with smaller neonatal head circumference z-scores. PAE was related to smaller neonatal head circumference z-scores among infants whose mothers were in the upper third for maternal ferritin but not in the other groups. At age 5 years, the relation of PAE to lower height z-scores was seen only among infants whose mothers were in the lowest third for maternal hemoglobin, and paradoxically, higher maternal hemoglobin was associated with lower height z-scores among controls. Higher neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio was related to larger 5-year height z-scores among exposed but not control infants; PAE was related to smaller height z-scores among children in the lowest third for neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio but not among the other infants.
A trend (p < 0.10) association between higher maternal ferritin and better 6.5-month visual recognition memory was seen among control but not exposed infants (Table 4). A negative association between PAE and 6.5-month visual recognition memory was only seen among infants whose mothers were in the highest third for ferritin. A trend (p < 0.10) association between higher maternal hemoglobin and better 12-month visual recognition memory was seen among exposed infants but not controls, and the association of PAE to poorer 12-month visual recognition memory was only seen among infants whose mothers were in the lower third for hemoglobin. Higher maternal hemoglobin was associated with shorter processing time (i.e., faster processing speed) at 6.5 months among exposed but not control infants, and a trend (p < 0.10) association between PAE and longer processing time at this age was only seen among infants whose mothers were in the lower third for hemoglobin. An association between ID at 6.5 months and longer processing time (i.e., slower processing speed) at 6.5 months was seen among exposed but not control infants.

3.5. Alterations in Maternal and Infant Iron Homeostasis as Potential Mediators in Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits

Table 5 presents the results of causal inference analyses using marginal structural models and the product method to examine the degree to which PAE-related alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis statistically mediate the relations between PAE and growth and neurobehavioral outcomes. These analyses were conducted for variable triads that consisted of PAE, an iron homeostasis outcome we have previously shown to be related to PAE [12], and a growth or neurobehavioral outcome related to both PAE and the given iron homeostasis outcome. When examining the PAE-related elevation in maternal ferritin as a potential mediator of the relation of PAE to neonatal length z-scores, the indirect effect of PAE through maternal ferritin comprised 33.3% of the total effect of PAE on neonatal length. Similarly, the indirect effect of PAE through maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio comprised 82.3% of the total effect of PAE on neonatal length z-scores. When examining neonatal weight z-scores, a trend (p < 0.10) for an indirect effect of PAE through maternal ferritin comprised 32.8% of the total effect of PAE on weight. A trend (p < 0.10) for an indirect effect of PAE through maternal ferritin comprised 43.5% of the total effects of PAE on head circumference z-scores. At age 5 years, the indirect effect of PAE through maternal ferritin comprised 27.1% of the total effects of PAE on height z-scores. The indirect effect of PAE through maternal ferritin comprised 29.4% of the total effects of PAE on weight z-scores, and the indirect effect of PAE through maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio comprised 22.6% of total effects on weight z-scores. Evidence of statistical mediation was not seen for neurobehavioral outcomes.

4. Discussion

In this prospective longitudinal birth cohort with serial measures of maternal, neonatal, and postnatal iron measures as well as serial child growth and neurobehavioral outcomes, PAE and maternal and infant iron measures had separate and overlapping associations with growth and neurobehavioral outcomes. Among the overlapping associations, PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin and hemoglobin:log(ferritin) appeared to statistically mediate 22.6–82.3% of the effects of PAE on neonatal and postnatal anthropometric measures. Furthermore, interaction effects were also evident between PAE and PAE-related maternal and infant iron measures on both growth and neurobehavioral deficits, indicating that PAE-related alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis appear to exacerbate the teratogenic effects of alcohol. These findings are also consistent with previous animal studies [17,33,57]. To our knowledge, this is the first examination of the effects of PAE and iron measures on both growth and neurobehavior in a human study.
As we and others have previously demonstrated [7,58,59], PAE is related to both fetal and postnatal growth restriction. We have previously reported that maternal alcohol consumption is associated with elevated ferritin [12]. Given that PAE was also related to a lower hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio, we posited that PAE-related elevations in ferritin were not an indication of better overall iron status but rather represented iron being sequestered into storage at the expense of other physiologic processes, such as erythropoiesis and placental iron transport, as is seen in an inflammatory state. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found that higher maternal ferritin and lower maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio were related to smaller neonatal length, weight, and head circumference and smaller 5-year height and weight. We have previously reported that PAE was associated with lower hemoglobin in this cohort, and in the analyses presented here, lower hemoglobin was related to smaller neonatal length, weight, and head circumference. Lower neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio, which we have shown to be associated with PAE, was related to lower weight at 5 years. In causal inference analyses, PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin and reductions in maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio appeared to partially statistically mediate effects of PAE on pre- and postnatal growth, indicating that these alterations in maternal iron homeostasis may play possible mechanistic roles in PAE-related pre- and postnatal growth restriction. These statistical mediation findings are consistent with previous animal and human studies demonstrating that reduced fetal iron stores may result in growth restriction [31,32]. Furthermore, interaction effects between PAE and maternal ferritin and maternal and neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio were seen, indicating that the detrimental effects of these iron indices on fetal growth are only seen in the setting of PAE. Additionally, PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin appeared to exacerbate PAE-related neonatal head circumference reductions, and PAE-related reductions in neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio appeared to exacerbate PAE-related 5-year weight reductions. We have previously shown that PAE-related postnatal height restriction is a biomarker for the severity of FASD neurocognitive deficits [10].
As we have demonstrated in birth cohorts in Detroit, USA [9], and Cape Town, South Africa [60], PAE was related to poorer visual recognition memory and slower processing speed. Recognition memory has been shown to be negatively affected by inadequate iron in early infancy [19,25,28]. In this cohort, higher neonatal ferritin, which we have shown to be associated with PAE [12], was related to poorer visual recognition memory at 12 months. Although higher ferritin may be an indicator of better overall iron status, in this cohort, we found that PAE was associated with a lower neonatal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio that partially mediated later development of IDA at age 6.5 months. These findings suggest that, as seen in the mothers, the higher ferritin likely represented iron sequestered into storage, making it unavailable for other processes like erythropoiesis, and, possibly, brain development. PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin appeared to exacerbate the detrimental effects of PAE on recognition memory in that the relation of PAE to visual recognition memory was only seen among infants whose mothers were in the highest tertile for ferritin. Maternal hemoglobin appeared to alter the timing of PAE-related neurobehavioral deficits. In the whole cohort, effects on visual recognition memory at 6.5 months were no longer evident at 12 months, suggesting a PAE-related developmental delay for this outcome. However, among infants whose mothers were in the lowest third for hemoglobin, PAE was also related to poorer visual recognition memory at 12 months, indicating that low maternal hemoglobin may prolong PAE-related visual recognition memory deficits.
Our finding that higher maternal hemoglobin was related to faster processing speed is consistent with previous studies demonstrating slower processing speed among infants born to anemic mothers [31,61]. Similarly, as seen in previous human studies of infant ID [29,62], ID at age 6.5 months was associated with slower processing speed. Interaction effects between PAE and alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis on processing speed were also seen. The relation of PAE to slower processing speed seen at 12 months in the whole cohort was also evident at 6.5 months among infants whose mothers were in the lowest tertile for maternal hemoglobin, indicating that lower maternal hemoglobin may make this deficit evident earlier in infancy. Additionally, the relation of IDA at 6.5 months to slower processing speed was only seen among infants with PAE.
As we have previously demonstrated [24,49], PAE was associated with lower complexity of symbolic play, a known precursor to language [63], and higher emotionality and shyness scores on the EAS temperament assessment. ID at 6.5 months was associated with higher emotionality, lower activity, and increased shyness. These findings confirm data from our prospective longitudinal cohort of infants in inner-city Detroit, where ID at 9 months was associated with increased shyness and decreased activity on the EAS and increased latency to engage with the examiner and decreased orientation-engagement, soothability, and positive affect on the Bayley Behavior Rating Scale [30]. Higher maternal hemoglobin was related to lower sociability and greater shyness; the significance of this finding is unclear and may be a chance finding, as infants with better iron status would be expected to be more socially engaged than infants with iron deficiency.
Our findings that higher maternal ferritin and lower maternal hemoglobin-to-log(ferritin) ratio partially statistically mediated PAE-related fetal and postnatal growth restriction suggest that correcting these alterations in maternal iron homeostasis may ameliorate some of the effects of PAE on growth; as this study was an observational study, true causation cannot be inferred, and randomized controlled intervention trials are needed to evaluate this hypothesis. In this cohort, we recently demonstrated that the effects of PAE on neonatal weight and length and postnatal head circumference were only apparent among children whose mothers were in the lowest and middle tertiles for dietary iron intake [38]. Our findings demonstrating that low maternal hemoglobin and PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin may exacerbate PAE-related deficits in recognition memory and/or processing speed suggest that improving maternal iron status may also mitigate some of the detrimental effects of PAE on neurobehavior. In Smith and colleagues’ FASD rat model studies, PAE-related alterations in maternal and offspring iron status mediated PAE-related growth and neurobehavioral deficits, and prenatal iron supplementation resulted in amelioration of these deficits [15,17]. Notably, in our pilot feasibility clinical trial demonstrating beneficial effects of prenatal choline supplementation on growth and neurobehavior among heavy drinking pregnant women, no treatment effects were seen on iron or hematological measures [64], suggesting that choline alone is likely to be insufficient to ameliorate these teratogenic effects of PAE.
Although clear evidence of mediation in neurobehavioral and temperament outcomes was not seen in causal inference analyses in this study, we cannot conclude that alterations in maternal and/or infant iron homeostasis do not partially mediate PAE effects on these outcomes. While effects of PAE on neurobehavioral outcomes were seen in linear regression models (Table 3), the total effects of PAE (direct + indirect) on these outcomes in causal inference analyses were significant for only two outcomes. This study may, therefore, have been underpowered to utilize marginal structural models for these outcomes. Other less robust approaches for examining statistical mediation that are less sensitive to power, such as the Sobel or Clogg tests, could not be used here, as they are not valid when interaction effects between the predictor and mediator are present [65]. Furthermore, although results from causal inference analyses with marginal structural models in observational studies more closely approximate true causation than other approaches, true causation cannot be inferred from mediation findings in observational studies such as this one. Our relatively small sample size, measurement error surrounding exposure estimates, and potential model misspecification and residual confounding may have led to Type 2 (false negative) errors. To minimize the risk of Type 1 (false positive) errors, we have focused particularly on the overall pattern of findings seen when examining multiple iron and infant outcome measures.

5. Conclusions

In summary, our findings add to the growing evidence in animal and human studies that fetal Alcohol-Related alterations in iron homeostasis in the mother, fetus, and postnatal infant may play mechanistic roles in FASD. PAE-related elevations in maternal ferritin appeared to partially statistically mediate pre- and postnatal fetal alcohol growth restriction. Furthermore, interaction effects between PAE and PAE-related maternal and infant iron measures on both growth and neurobehavioral deficits indicated that PAE-related alterations in maternal and infant iron homeostasis may exacerbate the teratogenic effects of alcohol. These findings provide strong justification for conducting randomized clinical trials in humans to examine the potential for prenatal iron supplementation to mitigate the teratogenic effects of alcohol on growth and neurobehavior. Future experimental animal and/or cell culture models examining the molecular mechanisms by which PAE and iron homeostasis interact in FASD may also yield novel, important insights into how PAE disrupts fetal development.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14204432/s1: Table S1. Relations of potential confounders to neurobehavioral outcomes; Table S2. Potential interaction effects between prenatal alcohol exposure and iron indices on infant growth, neurobehavior, and temperament.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.C.C., S.W.J., J.L.J. and C.D.M.; methodology, R.C.C., S.W.J., J.L.J. and C.D.M.; formal analysis, R.C.C.; resources, R.C.C., S.W.J., J.L.J. and C.D.M.; data curation, R.C.C., N.C.D., S.W.J. and J.L.J.; writing—original draft preparation, R.C.C.; writing—review and editing, R.C.C., S.W.J., J.L.J. and E.M.M.; funding acquisition, R.C.C., S.W.J., J.L.J. and E.M.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by NIH/NIAAA (R01AA016781, R21AA022203, K23AA020516; R01AA027916) and the Lycaki-Young Fund (State of Michigan).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Boards/Ethics Committees of Wayne State University (026708B3F, approved August 2011 and annually thereafter), University of Cape Town Faculty of Health Sciences, and Columbia University Irving Medical Center.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

De-identified, individual participant data that underlie the results reported in this article and the study protocol, statistical analysis plan, and analytic code will be available for sharing to journal editors for any reason either before or after publication for checking and to researchers who provide a methodologically sound proposal, as determined by the authors of this article. Proposals from interested parties should be directed to Sandra W. Jacobson, PhD ([email protected]). Data will be stored in a data repository at Wayne State University and transmitted electronically in encrypted form to requestors. Data requestors will need to sign a data access agreement prior to access.

Acknowledgments

We thank our UCT and WSU research staffs including Maggie September, Beverley Arendse, Patricia O’Leary, and Patricia Solomon for their work on subject recruitment and maintenance; Catherine Lewis, Nadine Lindinger, and Stacey Hall, who performed the infant anthropometry measurements and neurobehavioral assessments; and Renee Sun, for her assistance with data processing. We also thank Susan Fawcus, Mowbray Maternity Hospital; the nursing and records department staff at the Hanover Park and Retreat Midwife Obstetric Units, Mowbray Maternity Hospital, Somerset Hospital, and Groote Schuur Hospital, where the mothers were recruited and the infants were born. Lastly, we extend our deep appreciation to our Cape Town study participants for their contributions to this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

AA = absolute alcohol; EAS = Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability; FAS = fetal alcohol syndrome; FASD = fetal alcohol spectrum disorders; ID = iron deficiency; IDA = iron deficiency anemia; PAE = prenatal alcohol exposure; PFAS = partial fetal alcohol syndrome.

References

  1. May, P.A.; Baete, A.; Russo, J.; Elliott, A.J.; Blankenship, J.; Kalberg, W.O.; Buckley, D.; Brooks, M.; Hasken, J.; Abdul-Rahman, O.; et al. Prevalence and characteristics of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2014, 134, 855–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. May, P.A.; Chambers, C.D.; Kalberg, W.O.; Zellner, J.; Feldman, H.; Buckley, D.; Kopald, D.; Hasken, J.M.; Xu, R.; Honerkamp-Smith, G.; et al. Prevalence of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders in 4 US Communities. JAMA 2018, 319, 474–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. May, P.A.; Marais, A.S.; De Vries, M.M.; Buckley, D.; Kalberg, W.O.; Hasken, J.M.; Stegall, J.M.; Hedrick, D.M.; Robinson, L.K.; Manning, M.A.; et al. The prevalence, child characteristics, and maternal risk factors for the continuum of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A sixth population-based study in the same South African community. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2021, 218, 108408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. May, P.A.; Hasken, J.M.; Hooper, S.R.; Hedrick, D.M.; Jackson-Newsom, J.; Mullis, C.E.; Dobyns, E.; Kalberg, W.O.; Buckley, D.; Robinson, L.K.; et al. Estimating the community prevalence, child traits, and maternal risk factors of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) from a random sample of school children. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2021, 227, 108918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Hoyme, H.E.; Kalberg, W.O.; Elliott, A.J.; Blankenship, J.; Buckley, D.; Marais, A.-S.; Manning, M.A.; Robinson, L.K.; Adam, M.P.; Abdul-Rahman, O.; et al. Updated clinical guidelines for diagnosing fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20154256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Hoyme, H.E.; May, P.A.; Kalberg, W.O.; Kodituwakku, P.; Gossage, J.P.; Trujillo, P.M.; Buckley, D.G.; Miller, J.H.; Aragon, A.S.; Khaole, N.; et al. A practical clinical approach to diagnosis of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: Clarification of the 1996 Institute of Medicine criteria. Pediatrics 2005, 115, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Carter, R.C.; Jacobson, J.L.; Molteno, C.D.; Jiang, H.; Meintjes, E.M.; Jacobson, S.W.; Duggan, C. Effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure and iron deficiency anemia on child growth and body composition through age 9 years. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2012, 36, 1973–1982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Carter, R.C.; Wainwright, H.; Molteno, C.D.; Georgieff, M.K.; Dodge, N.C.; Warton, F.; Meintjes, E.M.; Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Alcohol, methamphetamine, and marijuana exposure have distinct effects on the human placenta. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 753–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jacobson, S.W. Specificity of neurobehavioral outcomes associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1998, 22, 313–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Carter, R.C.; Jacobson, J.L.; Molteno, C.D.; Dodge, N.C.; Meintjes, E.M.; Jacobson, S.W. Fetal alcohol growth restriction and cognitive impairment. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20160775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Carter, R.C.; Jacobson, S.W.; Molteno, C.D.; Jacobson, J.L. Fetal alcohol exposure, iron-deficiency anemia, and infant growth. Pediatrics 2007, 120, 559–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Carter, R.C.; Georgieff, M.K.; Ennis, K.M.; Dodge, N.C.; Wainwright, H.; Meintjes, E.M.; Duggan, C.P.; Molteno, C.D.; Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Prenatal alcohol-related alterations in maternal, placental, neonatal, and infant iron homeostasis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021, 114, 1107–1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sangkhae, V.; Fisher, A.L.; Wong, S.; Koenig, M.D.; Tussing-Humphreys, L.; Chu, A.; Lelic, M.; Ganz, T.; Nemeth, E. Effects of maternal iron status on placental and fetal iron homeostasis. J. Clin. Invest. 2019, 130, 625–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Huebner, S.M.; Blohowiak, S.E.; Kling, P.J.; Smith, S.M. Prenatal alcohol exposure alters fetal iron distribution and elevates hepatic hepcidin in a rat model of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Nutr. 2016, 146, 1180–1188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Huebner, S.M.; Helfrich, K.K.; Saini, N.; Blohowiak, S.E.; Cheng, A.A.; Kling, P.J.; Smith, S.M. Dietary iron fortification normalizes fetal hematology, hepcidin, and iron distribution in a rat model of prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2018, 42, 1022–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Saini, N.; Helfrich, K.K.; Kwan, S.T.C.; Huebner, S.M.; Abazi, J.; Flentke, G.R.; Blohowiak, S.E.; Kling, P.J.; Smith, S.M. Alcohol’s dysregulation of maternal-fetal IL-6 and p-STAT3 is a function of maternal iron status. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2019, 43, 2332–2343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rufer, E.S.; Tran, T.D.; Attridge, M.M.; Andrzejewski, M.E.; Flentke, G.R.; Smith, S.M. Adequacy of maternal iron status protects against behavioral, neuroanatomical, and growth deficits in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lozoff, B.; Beard, J.; Connor, J.; Barbara, F.; Georgieff, M.; Schallert, T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr. Rev. 2006, 64, S34–S43. discussion S72–S91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Carter, R.C.; Jacobson, J.L.; Burden, M.J.; Armony-Sivan, R.; Dodge, N.C.; Angelilli, M.L.; Lozoff, B.; Jacobson, S.W. Iron deficiency anemia and cognitive function in infancy. Pediatrics 2010, 126, e427–e434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Algarín, C.; Peirano, P.; Garrido, M.; Pizarro, F.; Lozoff, B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy: Long-lasting effects on auditory and visual system functioning. Pediatr. Res. 2003, 53, 217–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Miller, M.W.; Roskams, A.J.; Connor, J.R. Iron regulation in the developing rat brain: Effect of in utero ethanol exposure. J. Neurochem. 1995, 65, 373–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jacobson, S.W.; Stanton, M.E.; Dodge, N.C.; Pienaar, M.; Fuller, D.S.; Molteno, C.D.; Meintjes, E.M.; Hoyme, H.E.; Robinson, L.K.; Khaole, N.; et al. Impaired delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in school-age children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 250–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. McEchron, M.D.; Alexander, D.N.; Gilmartin, M.R.; Paronish, M.D. Perinatal nutritional iron deficiency impairs hippocampus-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning in rats. Dev. Neurosci. 2008, 30, 243–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Molteno, C.D.; Jacobson, J.L.; Carter, R.C.; Dodge, N.C.; Jacobson, S.W. Infant emotional withdrawal: A precursor of affective and cognitive disturbance in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2014, 38, 479–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Burden, M.J.; Westerlund, A.J.; Armony-Sivan, R.; Nelson, C.A.; Jacobson, S.W.; Lozoff, B.; Angelilli, M.L.; Jacobson, J.L. An event-related potential study of attention and recognition memory in infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Pediatrics 2007, 120, e336–e345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Geng, F.; Mai, X.; Zhan, J.; Xu, L.; Georgieff, M.; Shao, J.; Lozoff, B. Timing of iron deficiency and recognition memory in infancy. Nutr. Neurosci. 2022, 25, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Siddappa, A.M.; Georgieff, M.K.; Wewerka, S.; Worwa, C.; Nelson, C.A.; Deregnier, R.A. Iron deficiency alters auditory recognition memory in newborn infants of diabetic mothers. Pediatr. Res. 2004, 55, 1034–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Fretham, S.J.B.; Carlson, E.S.; Georgieff, M.K. The role of iron in learning and memory. Adv. Nutr. 2011, 2, 112–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Amin, S.B.; Orlando, M.; Eddins, A.; MacDonald, M.; Monczynski, C.; Wang, H. In utero iron status and auditory neural maturation in premature infants as evaluated by auditory brainstem response. J. Pediatr. 2010, 156, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Lozoff, B.; Clark, K.M.; Jing, Y.; Armony-Sivan, R.; Angelilli, M.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Dose-response relationships between iron deficiency with or without anemia and infant social-emotional behavior. J. Pediatr. 2008, 152, 696–702, 631–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Georgieff, M.K. Iron deficiency in pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2020, 223, 516–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kemppinen, L.; Mattila, M.; Ekholm, E.; Pallasmaa, N.; Torma, A.; Varakas, L.; Makikallio, K. Gestational iron deficiency anemia is associated with preterm birth, fetal growth restriction, and postpartum infections. J. Perinat. Med. 2021, 49, 431–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Huebner, S.M.; Tran, T.D.; Rufer, E.S.; Crump, P.M.; Smith, S.M. Maternal iron deficiency worsens the associative learning deficits and hippocampal and cerebellar losses in a rat model of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2015, 39, 2097–2107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Carter, R.C.; Senekal, M.; Dodge, N.C.; Bechard, L.; Meintjes, E.M.; Molteno, C.D.; Duggan, C.; Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Maternal alcohol use and nutrition during pregnancy: Diet and anthropometry. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2017, 41, 2114–2127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L.; Molteno, C.D.; Warton, C.M.R.; Wintermark, P.; Hoyme, H.E.; De Jong, G.; Taylor, P.; Warton, F.; Lindinger, N.M.; et al. Heavy prenatal alcohol exposure is related to smaller corpus callosum in newborn MRI scans. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2017, 41, 965–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Jacobson, S.W.; Chiodo, L.M.; Sokol, R.J.; Jacobson, J.L. Validity of maternal report of prenatal alcohol, cocaine, and smoking in relation to neurobehavioral outcome. Pediatrics 2002, 109, 815–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sokol, R.; Martier, S.; Ernhart, C. Identification of alcohol abuse in the prenatal clinic. In Early Identification of Alcohol Abuse; Chang, N., Chao, M., Eds.; Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration Research: Rockville, MD USA, 1983; Volume 17. [Google Scholar]
  38. Carter, R.C.; Senekal, M.; Duggan, C.P.; Dodge, N.C.; Meintjes, E.M.; Molteno, C.D.; Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Gestational weight gain and dietary energy, iron, and choline intake predict severity of fetal alcohol growth restriction in a prospective birth cohort. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2022, 116, 460–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ganz, T. Anemia of Inflammation. N. Engl. J. Med. 2019, 381, 1148–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Beard, J.L. Iron deficiency: Assessment during pregnancy and its importance in pregnant adolescents. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1994, 59, 502S–508S. discussion 508S–510S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Carriaga, M.T.; Skikne, B.S.; Finley, B.; Cutler, B.; Cook, J.D. Serum transferrin receptor for the detection of iron deficiency in pregnancy. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1991, 54, 1077–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. United Nations University; World Health Organization/United Nations Children’s Fund/United Nations University. Iron Deficiency Anemia: Prevention, Assessment and Control—Report of a Joint WHO/UNICEF/UNU Consultation; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  43. de Onis, M.; Garza, C.; Victora, C.G.; Onyango, A.W.; Frongillo, E.A.; Martines, J. The WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study: Planning, study design, and methodology. Food Nutr. Bull. 2004, 25, S15–S26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Fagan, J.F.; Singer, L.T. Infant recognition memory as a measure of intelligence. In Advances in Infancy Research; Lipsett, L.P., Ed.; Ablex: Norwood, NJ, USA, 1983; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
  45. Colombo, J.; Mitchell, D.W.; Coldren, J.T.; Freeseman, L.J. Individual differences in infant visual attention: Are short lookers faster processors or feature processors? Child Dev. 1991, 62, 1247–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L.; Sokol, R.J. Effects of fetal alcohol exposure on infant reaction time. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1994, 18, 1125–1132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. McCune-Nicholic, L. Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Dev. 1981, 52, 785–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Belsky, J.; Garduque, L.; Hrncir, E. Assessing performance, competence, and executive capacity in infant play. Dev. Psychol. 1984, 20, 406–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Molteno, C.D.; Jacobson, J.L.; Carter, R.C.; Jacobson, S.W. Infant symbolic play as an early indicator of fetal alcohol-related deficit. Infancy 2010, 15, 586–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Buss, A.H.; Plomin, R. Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  51. McClelland, G.H.; Judd, C.M. Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 376–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pearl, J. Direct and indirect effects. In Proceedings of The 17th Conference on Uncertainty and Artificial Intelligence, San Francisco, CA, USA, 2–5 August 2001; pp. 411–420. [Google Scholar]
  53. Robins, J.M.; Greenland, S. Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology 1992, 3, 143–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Institute of Medicine. Choline. In Dietary Reference Intakes for Folate, Thiamin, Riboflavin, Niacin, Vitamin B12, Panthothenic Acid, Biotin, and Choline; National Academy Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  55. Oken, E.; Kleinman, K.P.; Rich-Edwards, J. A nearly continuous measure of birth weight for gestational age using a United States national reference. BMC Pediatr. 2003, 3, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L.; O’Neill, J.M.; Padgett, R.J.; Frankowski, J.J.; Bihun, J.T. Visual expectation and dimensions of infant information processing. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 711–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Rufer, E.S.; Tran, T.D.; Attridge, M.E.; Andrzejewski, M.E.; Smith, S.M. Subclinical maternal iron inadequacy exacerbates neurobehavioral deficits caused by developmental ethanol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2010, 34, 292a. [Google Scholar]
  58. Day, N.L.; Richardson, G.A.; Geva, D.; Robles, N. Alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco: Effects of prenatal exposure on offspring growth and morphology at age six. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1994, 18, 786–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W.; Sokol, R.J. Effects of prenatal exposure to alcohol, smoking, and illicit drugs on postpartum somatic growth. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1994, 18, 317–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Riley, E.P.; Mattson, S.N.; Li, T.K.; Jacobson, S.W.; Coles, C.D.; Kodituwakku, P.W.; Adnams, C.M.; Korkman, M.I. Neurobehavioral consequences of prenatal alcohol exposure: An international perspective. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2003, 27, 362–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Thomas, D.G.; Kennedy, T.S.; Colaizzi, J.; Aubuchon-Endsley, N.; Grant, S.; Stoecker, B.; Duell, E. Multiple Biomarkers of Maternal Iron Predict Infant Cognitive Outcomes. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2017, 42, 146–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Georgieff, M.K. Nutrition and the developing brain: Nutrient priorities and measurement. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 85, 614S–620S. [Google Scholar]
  63. Orr, E.; Geva, R. Symbolic play and language development. Infant. Behav. Dev. 2015, 38, 147–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Jacobson, S.W.; Carter, R.C.; Molteno, C.D.; Stanton, M.E.; Herbert, J.; Lindinger, N.M.; Lewis, C.E.; Dodge, N.C.; Hoyme, H.E.; Zeisel, S.H.; et al. Efficacy of maternal choline supplementation during pregnancy in mitigating adverse effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on growth and cognitive function: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2018, 42, 1327–1341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Clogg, C.C. Latent Class Models; Arminger, G., Clogg, C.C., Sobel, M.E., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 311–359. [Google Scholar]
Table 2. Relation of prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices to infant anthropometric measures.
Table 2. Relation of prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices to infant anthropometric measures.
 A. 2-week Neonatal Measures (n = 61 Controls, 69 Heavily Exposed)
Length-for-Age z-score aWeight-for-Age z-score aHead Circumference-for-Age z-score a
B1B2B1B2B1B2
Maternal alcohol consumption
 Drinking days/week−0.36 **
(−0.58, −0.14)
−0.28 **
(−0.47, −0.09)
−0.30 **
(−0.51, −0.08)
−0.21 *
(−0.39, −0.03)
−2.84 ***
(−4.36, −1.33)
−2.40
(−3.66, −1.14)
Maternal iron indices
 Dietary iron (mg/day)0.05
(−0.03, 0.12)
0.04
(−0.02, 0.11)
0.05
(−0.02, 0.12)
0.05
(−0.01, 0.10)
0.05
(−0.03, 0.12)
0.04
(−0.03, 0.10)
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.69 ***
(−1.02, −0.37)
−0.54 ***
(−0.83, −0.24)
−0.56 ***
(−0.87, −0.25)
−0.39 **
(−0.67, −0.11)
−0.63 ***
(−0.95, −0.30)
−0.46 **
(−0.75, −0.17)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.15
(−0.38, 0.08)
−0.03
(−0.23, 0.17)
−0.14
(−0.36, 0.08)
−0.01
(−0.20, 0.18)
−0.13
(−0.36, 0.11)
0.01
(−0.18, 0.21)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)0.45 ***
(0.19, 0.71)
0.34 **
(0.11, 0.57)
0.37 **
(0.12, 0.62)
0.26 *
(0.05, 0.48)
0.46 ***
(0.20, 0.72)
0.35 **
(0.12, 0.57)
 Anemia of inflammation b−0.68
(−1.68, 0.32)
−0.72
(−1.56, 0.11)
−0.38
(−1.35, 0.59)
−0.42
(−1.22, 0.38)
0.30
(−0.40, 1.01)
−0.51
(−1.32, 0.30)
Neonatal iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)0.03
(−0.35, 0.42)
−0.02
(−0.32, 0.29)
0.04
(−0.34, 0.41)
−0.04
(−0.33, 0.26)
0.17
(−0.23, 0.56)
0.13
(−0.17, 0.44)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)0.10 (−0.01, 0.21)
0.11 *
(0.02, 0.20)
0.10
(−0.01, 0.20)
0.10 *
(0.02, 0.19)
0.08
(−0.03, 0.19)
0.09 *
(0.00, 0.17)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)0.31
(−0.17, 0.79)
0.33
(−0.04, 0.70)
0.32
(−0.15, 0.79)
0.34
(−0.02, 0.71)
0.14
(−0.34, 0.62)
0.17
(−0.17, 0.51)
 B. 5-year Postnatal Measures (n = 67 Controls, 80 Heavily Exposed)
Height-for-Age z-score aWeight-for-Age z-score aHead Circumference (cm)
B1B2B1B2B1B2
Maternal alcohol consumption
 Drinking days/week−0.24 ***
(−0.38, −0.10)
−0.19 **
(−0.33, −0.05)
−0.29 ***
(−0.43, −0.15)
−0.22 **
(−0.36, −0.08)
−0.70 ***
(−1.13, −0.28)
−0.73 ***
(−1.17, −0.29)
Maternal iron indices
 Dietary iron (mg/day)−0.02
(−0.06, 0.03)
−0.02
(−0.07, 0.02)
0.01
(−0.04, 0.05)
0.00
(−0.05, 0.04)
−0.06
(−0.20, 0.08)
−0.09
(−0.24, 0.05)
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.37 ***
(−0.58, −0.17)
−0.33 **
(−0.55, −0.11)
−0.45 ***
(−0.66, −0.25)
−0.40 ***
(−0.62, −0.19)
−0.43
(−1.09, 0.22)
−0.29
(−1.03, 0.44)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.14 *
(−0.29, 0.00)
−0.10
(−0.25, 0.04)
−0.15 *
(−0.30, 0.00)
−0.09
(−0.23, 0.05)
0.14
(−0.31, 0.58)
−0.29
(−1.03, 0.44)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)0.23 **
(0.07, 0.39)
0.19 *
(0.02, 0.35)
0.30 ***
(0.14, 0.46)
0.25 **
(0.09, 0.41)
0.36
(−0.14, 0.85)
0.26
(−0.28, 0.79)
 Anemia of inflammation b−0.42
(−1.01, 0.17)
−0.21
(−0.80, 0.38)
−0.57
(−1.18, 0.04)
−0.31
(−0.89, 0.28)
−0.92
(−2.80, 0.96)
−0.89
(−2.86, 1.07)
Neonatal iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.18
(−0.43, 0.07)
−0.23
(−0.47, 0.01)
−0.03
(−0.29, 0.24)
−0.07
(−0.31, 0.18)
0.21
(−0.62, 1.05)
0.15
(−0.70, 1.00)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)0.03
(−0.04, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.04, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.05, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.04, 0.11)
0.08
(−0.15, 0.32)
0.09
(−0.16, 0.35)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)0.30
(−0.01, 0.61)
0.30 *
(0.00, 0.60)
0.13
(−0.20, 0.46)
0.14
(−0.17, 0.46)
0.23
(−0.83, 1.28)
0.26
(−0.82, 1.34)
 6.5-month infant iron indices0.19
(−0.01, 0.39)
0.08
(−0.12, 0.28)
0.14
(−0.07, 0.35)
−0.01
(−0.21, 0.19)
0.20
(−0.44, 0.84)
0.13
(−0.55, 0.81)
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.05
(−0.20, 0.09)
−0.05
(−0.19, 0.09)
−0.06
(−0.21, 0.10)
−0.06
(−0.20, 0.08)
0.13
(−0.32, 0.58)
0.17
(−0.29, 0.63)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.28
(−0.63, 0.07)
−0.13
(−0.48, 0.22)
−0.12
(−0.49, 0.24)
0.08
(−0.26, 0.42)
−0.01
(−1.10, 1.09)
0.13
(−1.01, 1.26)
 Iron deficiency b−0.08
(−0.55, 0.40)
−0.03
(−0.50, 0.45)
0.00
(−0.52, 0.52)
0.08
(−0.26, 0.42)
0.01
(−0.79, 0.80)
0.08
(−0.69, 0.85)
 Iron deficiency anemia b0.19
(−0.25, 0.63)
0.05
(−0.38, 0.48)
0.10
(−0.36, 0.56)
−0.06
(−0.50, 0.38)
0.18
(−0.63, 0.98)
−0.08
(−0.87, 0.72)
B1 = regression coefficients (95% CI) from univariate linear regression models, B2 = regression coefficients (95% CI) from multivariable linear regression models adjusting for potential confounders (maternal age, prenatal cigarettes/day, maternal education, and weeks gestation at delivery for all outcomes, with the addition of age at time of measurement for 5-year head circumference), a Age/sex-specific z-scores from World Health Organization norms [43]. b Yes = 1; no = 0. p ≤ 0.10; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Table 3. Relation of prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices to infant neurobehavior and temperament.
Table 3. Relation of prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices to infant neurobehavior and temperament.
A. Neurobehavior (n = 71 Controls, 83 Heavily Exposed at 6.5 months; 70 Controls, 81 Heavily Exposed at 12 months)
Visual Recognition Memory aInformation Processing Time (ms) aSymbolic Play (12 months) b
6.5 months12 months6.5 months12 months
B1B2B1B2B1B2B1B2B1B2
Maternal alcohol consumption
 Drinking days/week−1.39 **
(−2.43, 0.34)
−1.22 *
(−2.29, −0.14)
−0.50
(−1.60, 0.60)
−0.53
(−1.68, 0.61)
0.01
(−0.07, 0.08)
0.01
(−0.07, 0.08)
0.08 *
(0.01, 0.15)
0.08 *
(0.01, 0.15)
−0.61 *
(−1.12, −0.06)
−0.62 *
(−1.2, −0.10)
Maternal iron indices
 Dietary iron (mg/day)−0.17
(−0.52, 0.17)
−0.23
(−0.57, 0.12)
−0.11
(−0.48, 0.26)
−0.11
(−0.49, 0.27)
0.00
(−0.02, 0.03)
0.00
(−0.02, 0.03)
0.02
(0.00, 0.05)
0.02
(0.00, 0.05)
−0.12
(−0.32, 0.05)
−0.09
(−0.26, 0.09)
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−1.10
(−2.54, 0.34)
−0.66
(−2.20, 0.89)
0.00
(−1.61, 1.61)
−0.05
(−1.79, 1.68)
−0.05
(−0.15, 0.05)
−0.05
(−0.15, 0.05)
0.08
(−0.02, 0.19)
0.09
(−0.01, 0.20)
−0.15
(−0.96, 0.67)
0.00
(−0.81, 0.80)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.36
(−1.44, 0.73)
−0.16
(−1.27, 0.95)
0.27
(−0.84, 1.38)
0.22
(−0.95, 1.40)
−0.07 *
(−0.15, 0.00)
−0.08 *
(−0.16, 0.01)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.09)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.10)
0.22
(−0.35, 0.78)
0.22
(−0.32, 0.75)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)−0.50
(−0.62, 1.62)
−0.12
(−1.06, 1.30)
0.08
(−1.13, 1.28)
0.09
(−1.18, 1.36)
−0.01
(−0.09, 0.07)
−0.02
(−0.10, 0.06)
−0.06
(−0.14, 0.02)
−0.06
(−0.14, 0.02)
−0.20
(−0.41, 0.82)
0.04
(−0.56, 0.64)
 Anemia of inflammation c−1.28
(−5.77, 3.21)
−0.23
(−4.82, 4.37)
−1.40
(−6.24, 3.43)
−1.46
(−6.47, 3.55)
0.02
(−0.30. 0.34)
0.05
(−0.27, 0.36)
0.24
(−0.07, 0.56)
0.26
(−0.05, 0.57)
−2.32 *
(−4.51, −0.11)
−1.63
(−3.80, 0.54)
Neonatal iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.35
(−2.23, 1.54)
−0.27
(−2.13, 1.60)
−2.13 *
(−4.16, −0.10)
−2.27 *
(−4.35, −0.18)
0.01
(−0.10, 0.11)
0.01
(−0.10, 0.11)
−0.05
(−0.18, 0.08)
−0.01
(−0.18, 0.08)
0.36
(−0.64, 1.36)
0.10
(−0.87, 1.06)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)0.25
(−0.27, 0.76)
0.26
(−0.26, 0.78)
−0.41
(−0.96, 0.14)
−0.45
(−1.02, 0.13)
0.00
(−0.03, 0.03)
0.00
(−0.03, 0.03)
−0.01
(−0.05, 0.02)
−0.04
(−0.17, 0.08)
0.36 **
(0.09, 0.63)
0.25
(−0.02, 0.52)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)1.60
(−0.83, 4.04)
1.39
(−0.99, 3.76)
0.27
(−2.32, 2.86)
0.30
(−2.35, 2.95)
−0.03
(−0.16, 0.10)
−0.02
(−0.15, 0.11)
0.01
(−0.15, 0.17)
0.01
(−0.15, 0.17)
1.10
(−0.18, 2.37)
0.95
(−0.25, 2.15)
6.5-month infant iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/g values)−0.25
(−1.74, 1.24)
−0.56
(−2.06, 0.94)
−0.71
(−2.23, 0.82)
−0.82
(−2.41, 0.77)
0.09
(−0.02, 0.19)
0.09
(−0.02, 0.19)
−0.11 *
(−0.21, −0.01)
−0.11 *
(−0.20, −0.01)
1.00 **
(0.25, 1.75)
0.39
(−0.40, 1.18)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.51
(−1.54, 0.52)
−0.52
(−1.55, 0.52)
0.03
(−1.07, 1.13)
−0.05
(−1.19, 1.09)
−0.05
(−0.13, 0.02)
−0.05
(−0.12, 0.03)
0.01
(−0.06, 0.08)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.09)
0.19
(−0.37, 0.74)
0.08
(−0.45, 0.61)
 Iron deficiency c−0.29
(−2.72, 2.13)
0.22
(−2.21, 2.65)
1.00
(−1.61, 3.61)
1.13
(−1.55, 3.80)
−0.03
(−0.20, 0.14)
−0.03
(−0.20, 0.14)
0.21 **
(0.04, 0.37)
0.21 **
(0.04, 0.37)
−1.42 *
(−2.73, −0.12)
−0.69
(−1.99, 0.61)
 Iron deficiency anemia c−0.82
(−3.83, 2.18)
−0.53
(−3.51, 2.45)
0.11
(−3.13, 3.35)
0.23
(−3.05, 3.52)
0.19
(−0.02, 0.4)
0.18
(−0.03, 0.39)
0.13
(−0.08, 0.33)
0.13
(−0.08, 0.33)
−1.74 *
(−3.73, −0.10)
−1.18
(−2.77, 0.40)
B. Temperament (n = 70 Controls, 81 Heavily Exposed)
EAS Temperament Scale d (12 months)
EmotionalityActivitySociabilityShyness
B1B2B1B2B1B2B1B2
Maternal alcohol consumption
 Drinking days/week0.10 **
(0.04, 0.17)
0.09 **
(0.03, 0.15)
−0.02
(−0.10, 0.05)
−0.02
(−0.09, 0.05)
−0.05
(−0.13, 0.02)
−0.04
(−0.11, 0.04)
0.09 *
(0.01, 0.17)
0.08 *
(0.00, 0.16)
Maternal iron indices
 Dietary iron (mg/day)0.01
(−0.01, 0.03)
0.01
(−0.01, 0.04)
0.00
(−0.02, 0.02)
−0.00
(−0.02, 0.02)
−0.01
(−0.04, 0.01)
−0.02
(−0.04, 0.01)
0.00
(−0.02, 0.03)
0.00
(−0.02, 0.03)
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)0.09
(−0.01, 0.18)
0.07
(−0.03, 0.16)
0.02
(0.08, −0.12)
0.0
(−.10, 0.10)
−0.09
(−0.19, 0.02)
−0.05
(−0.15, 0.06)
0.04
(−0.08, 0.15)
0.03
(−0.09, 0.14)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)0.04
(−0.03, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.03, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.04, 0.11)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.10)
−0.10 **
(−0.17, −0.02)
−0.09 *
(−0.16, −0.01)
0.08 *
(0.00, 0.16)
0.08 *
(0.00, 0.16)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)−0.04
(−0.11, 0.04)
−0.02
(−0.09, 0.05)
0.00
(−0.08, 0.08)
0.02
(−0.06, 0.09)
0.04
(−0.04, 0.12)
0.01
(−0.07, 0.09)
0.00
(−0.08, 0.09)
0.01
(−0.07, 0.10)
 Anemia of inflammation c0.29 *
(0.00, 0.57)
0.27
(−0.02, 0.55)
−0.03
(−0.34, 0.29)
−0.03
(−0.33, 0.28)
−0.03
(−0.35, 0.30)
0.01
(−0.31, 0.33)
0.04
(−0.32, 0.40)
0.01
(−0.35, 0.37)
Neonatal iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)0.11 *
(0.00, 0.22)
0.11
(−0.00, 0.22)
0.03
(−0.10, 0.16)
0.05
(−0.08, 0.18)
−0.06
(−0.20, 0.08)
−0.07
(−0.21, 0.07)
0.00
(−0.15, 0.14)
0.00
(−0.14, 0.15)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)0.01
(−0.04, 0.03)
−0.01
(−0.04, 0.02)
0.01
(−0.03, 0.05)
0.01
(−0.02, 0.05)
0.00
(−0.04, 0.04)
0.00
(−0.04, 0.04)
−0.02
(−0.06, 0.02)
−0.02
(−0.06, 0.02)
 Hemoglobin:log(ferritin)−0.12
(−0.26, 0.03)
−0.12
(−0.26, 0.03)
−0.01
(−0.18, −0.16)
−0.03
(−0.19, 0.14)
0.05
(−0.12, 0.23)
0.07
(−0.11, 0.24)
0.66
(−0.89, 2.21)
0.56
(−0.98, 2.09)
6.5-month infant iron indices
 Ferritin (logged ug/L values)−0.04
(−0.13, 0.05)
−0.04
(−0.13, 0.05)
0.09
(−0.01, 0.19)
0.12 *
(0.02, 0.22)
0.08
(−0.02, 0.18)
0.07
(−0.03, 0.17)
−0.06
(−0.17, 0.05)
−0.06
(−0.17, 0.05)
 Hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.04
(−0.10, 0.03)
−0.03
(−0.10, 0.03)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.09)
0.03
(−0.04, 0.10)
0.03
(−0.04, 0.10)
0.02
(−0.05, 0.10)
−0.01
(−0.09, 0.07)
−0.02
(−0.10, 0.06)
 Iron deficiency c0.17 *
(0.02, 0.33)
0.16 *
(0.01, 0.31)
−0.19 *
(−0.35, 0.02)
−0.22 **
(−0.39, −0.06)
−0.15
(−0.32, 0.02)
−0.11
(−0.29, 0.06)
0.19 *
(0.01, 0.38)
0.18 *
(0.00, 0.37)
 Iron deficiency anemia c0.11
(−0.09, 0.30)
0.09
(−0.10, 0.28)
−0.22 *
(−0.42, −0.01)
−0.25 *
(−0.45, −0.04)
−0.07
(−0.28, 0.14)
−0.04
(−0.25, 0.17)
−0.09
(−0.28, 0.10)
−0.07
(−0.26, 0.11)
B1 = regression coefficient (95% CI) from univariate linear regression models. B2 = regression coefficient (95% CI) from multivariable linear regression models including: maternal age, gravidity, and weeks gestation at delivery for visual recognition memory; prenatal marijuana use (days/month) for information processing time; maternal education, weeks gestation at delivery, infant sex, and age at time of assessment for symbolic play; gravidity for emotionality; socioeconomic status, prenatal marijuana use (days/month), infant sex for activity; gravidity and prenatal methamphetamine use (days/month) for sociability; prenatal cigarette smoking (cigarettes/day) and marijuana use (days/month) for shyness. a From the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence [44,56]; missing for 2 controls and 8 heavily exposed infants at 6.5 months, 5 control and 2 heavily exposed infants at 12 months. b Highest level of imitated play complexity [48]; missing for 6 control and 3 heavily exposed infants. c Yes = 1; no = 0. d Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability temperament scale [50]. p ≤ 0.10; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01.
Table 4. Stratified models demonstrating interaction effects between prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices on growth and neurobehavior.
Table 4. Stratified models demonstrating interaction effects between prenatal alcohol exposure and maternal and infant iron indices on growth and neurobehavior.
Interaction Term p aExposure Stratification GroupsIron Measure Stratification Groups
Neonatal length-for-age z-score b (age 2 weeks) ControlsHeavy exposureLower thirdMiddle thirdUpper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal hb:log(ferritin) c 0.105 0.09
(−0.25, 0.43)
0.45 **
(0.10, 0.80)
−0.33
(−1.52, 0.85)
−0.35
(−0.96, 0.26)
−0.04
(−0.50, 0.41)
 Neonatal hb:log(ferritin) c0.021−0.09
(−0.53, 0.36)
0.91 **
(0.23, 1.58)
−0.65
(−1.44, 0.15)
−0.17
(−0.94, 0.60)
0.45
(−0.34, 1.25)
Neonatal weight-for-age z-score a (age 2 weeks) Controls Heavy exposure Lower third Middle third Upper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Neonatal hemoglobin (g/dL)0.0200.00
(−0.1, 0.1)
0.24 **
(0.08, 0.39)
−0.79
(−1.63, 0.06)
−0.08
(−0.72, 0.58)
0.09
(−0.51, 0.69)
 Neonatal hb:log(ferritin) c0.011−0.11
(−0.53, 0.31)
0.93 **
(0.26, 1.60)
−0.59
(−1.54, 0.37)
−0.25
(−0.95, 0.45)
0.47
(−0.06, 1.00)
Neonatal head circumference-for-age z-score b (age 2 weeks) Controls Heavy exposure Lower third Middle third Upper
third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal ferritin (logged ug/L values) d0.0280.14
(−0.31, 0.60)
−0.53 *
(−0.96, −0.11)
−0.06
(−0.60, 0.49)
−0.37
(−1.14, 0.39)
−1.07 *
(−1.99, −0.15)
 Maternal hb:log(ferritin) c0.0740.04
(−0.24, 0.32)
0.43 *
(0.06, 0.79)
−0.57
(1.51, 0.37)
−0.28
(−1.01, 0.44)
−0.21
(−0.68, 0.26)
Postnatal height-for-age z-score b (age 5 years) Controls Heavy exposure Lower third Middle third Upper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL)0.081−0.23 *
(−0.44, −0.02)
0.01
(−0.18, 0.21)
−0.62 **
(−1.07, −0.17)
−0.28
(−0.89, 0.33)
−0.17
(−0.73, 0.40)
 Neonatal hb:log(ferritin) c0.0590.01
(−0.40, 0.42)
0.64 *
(0.14, 1.1)
−0.71 *
(−1.42, −0.01)
0.16
(−0.49 0.81)
−0.21
(−0.59, 0.17)
Visual recognition memory (6.5 months) e ControlsHeavy exposureLower thirdMiddle thirdUpper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal ferritin (logged ug/L values) d 0.021 2.54
(−0.35, 5.42)
−1.51
(−3.47, 0.45)
0.27
(−4.24, 4.78)
0.43
(−3.13, 3.98)
−7.15 ***
(−11.35, −2.95)
Visual recognition memory (12 months) e Controls Heavy exposure Lower third Middle third Upper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL) 0.038 −0.74
(−2.09, 0.62)
1.58
(−0.13, 3.29)
−4.60 *
(−8.55, −0.67)
−0.21
(−3.78, 3.36)
−1.59
(−6.26, 3.09)
Information processing time (6.5 months) e Controls Heavy exposure Lower third Middle third Upper third
BindBindBalcBalcBalc
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL) 0.091 −0.03
(−0.14, 0.09)
−0.16 **
(−0.26, −0.06)
0.20
(−0.03, 0.44)
−0.05
(−0.25, 0.16)
−0.06
(−0.40, 0.29)
Controls Heavy exposure No iron deficiency anemia Iron deficiency anemia
BindBindBalcBalc
 Infant iron deficiency anemia at 6.5 months
 (Yes = 1, No = 0) d
0.088 0.00
(−0.45, 0.45)
0.41 **
(0.16, 0.66)
−0.18
(−0.43, 0.07)
0.20
(−0.19, 0.59)
Bind = regression coefficient (95% CI) for the given iron measure regressed on the given outcome and potential confounders (maternal age, prenatal cigarettes/day, maternal education, and weeks gestation at delivery for anthropometry measures, with the addition of age at time of measurement for 5-year head circumference; maternal age, gravidity, and weeks gestation at delivery for visual recognition memory), stratified by heavy exposure vs. controls. Balc = regression coefficient (95% CI) for prenatal alcohol exposure (heavy exposure = 1, control = 0) regressed on the given outcome and potential confounders (as listed above), stratified by the given iron measure. a p-value for the interaction term in a model regressing a given outcome on PAE, the given iron measure, a PAE x iron measure outcome, and potential confounders (please see Supplementary Table S2 for full results from these models). b Age/sex-specific z-scores from World Health Organization norms [43]; n = 61 controls, 69 heavily exposed at 2 weeks; 67 controls, 80 heavily exposed at 5 years. c Negatively related to prenatal alcohol exposure in previously published analyses [12]. d Positively related to prenatal alcohol exposure in previously published analyses [12]. e From the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence [44,56]; n = 69 controls, 75 heavily exposed at 6.5 months; 65 controls, 79 heavily exposed at 12 months. p ≤ 0.10; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Table 5. Alterations in Maternal and Infant Iron Homeostasis as Potential Mediators in Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits a.
Table 5. Alterations in Maternal and Infant Iron Homeostasis as Potential Mediators in Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits a.
Natural
Direct Effect
Natural
Indirect Effect
Total EffectsProportion Mediation (%)
Neonatal length-for-age z-scores b (age 2 weeks)
 Maternal ferritin (logged ug/L values) c−0.52
(−1.11, 0.08)
−0.26 *
(−0.50, −0.03)
−0.78 **
(−1.35, −0.22)
33.3
 Maternal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.11
(−0.98, 0.76)
−0.51 *
(−1.04, 0.01)
−0.62
(−1.26, 0.01)
82.3
 Neonatal hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.34
(−0.97, 0.29)
−0.09
(−0.23, 0.06)
−0.43
(−1.06, 0.20)
−−
 Neonatal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d0.25
(−0.59, 1.10)
−0.57
(−1.20, 0.07)
−0.31
(−1.07, 0.45)
−−
Neonatal weight-for-age z-scores b (age 2 weeks)
 Maternal ferritin (logged ug/L values) c−0.38
(−0.96, 0.18)
−0.19
(−0.40, 0.02)
−0.58 **
(−1.11, −0.04)
32.8
 Maternal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.43
(−1.00, 0.14)
−0.15
(−0.35, 0.04)
−0.59 *
(−1.12, 0.05)
−−
 Neonatal hemoglobin (g/dL)0.11
(−0.54, 0.75)
−0.31
(−0.75, 0.11)
−0.21
(−0.88, 0.46)
−−
 Neonatal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d0.69
(−0.12, 1.50)
−0.68
(−1.37, 0.00)
0.01
(−0.77, 0.79)
−−
Neonatal head circumference-for-age z-scores b (age 2 weeks)
 Maternal ferritin c−0.49
(−1.23, 0.26)
−0.37
(−0.75, 0.01)
−0.85 **
(−1.44, −0.27)
43.5
 Maternal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.55
(−1.38, 0.27)
−0.35
(−0.82, 0.12)
−0.90 **
(−1.49, −0.31)
−−
Postnatal length-for-age z-scores b (age 5 years)
 Maternal ferritin (logged ug/L values) c−0.43 *
(−0.86, 0.00)
−0.16 *
(−0.32, 0.00)
−0.59 **
(−1.01, −0.18)
27.1
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL)−0.62 **
(−1.04, −0.20)
0.00
(−0.12, 0.11)
−0.62 **
(−1.04, −0.20)
−−
 Maternal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.48 *
(−0.91, −0.05)
−0.11
(−0.25, 0.03)
−0.59 **
(−1.01, −0.17)
−−
 Neonatal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.32
(−1.01, 0.38)
−0.39
(−0.87, 0.10)
−0.70 *
(−1.32, −0.08)
−−
Postnatal weight-for-age z-scores b (age 5 years)
 Maternal ferritin c−0.48 *
(−0.89, −0.01)
−0.20 *
(−0.37, −0.04)
−0.68 ***
(−1.09, −0.27)
29.4
 Maternal hemoglobin:log(ferritin) d−0.52 **
(−0.94, −0.10)
−0.15 *
(−0.30, −0.00)
−0.68 ***
(−1.09, −0.27)
22.6
Visual recognition memory (6.5 months) e
 Maternal ferritin (logged values) c−3.12
(−7.31, 1.06)
−0.39
(−2.37, 1.57)
−3.52 *
(−6.85, −0.19)
−−
Visual recognition memory (12 months) e
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL)−1.41
(−4.95, 2.14)
−0.03
(−0.54, 0.48)
−1.44
(−5.01, 2.13)
−−
 Neonatal ferritin
Information processing time (6.5 months) e
 Maternal hemoglobin (g/dL)0.12
(−0.11, 0.21)
−0.04
(−0.16, 0.09)
0.09
(−0.16, 0.33)
−−
 Infant iron deficiency anemia at 6.5 months c,g 0.13
(−0.10, 0.35)
0.08
(−0.06, 0.21)
0.20
(−0.04, 0.45)
−−
Symbolic play (12 months) f
 Infant iron deficiency anemia at 6.5 months c,g−0.97
(−2.82, 0.88)
−0.30
(−0.99, 0.38)
−1.27
(−2.14, 0.59)
−−
EASS emotionality (12 months) h
 Infant iron deficiency c,g0.42 ***
(0.20, 0.65)
0.03
(−0.02, 0.09)
0.46 ***
(0.23, 0.68)
−−
EASS shyness (12 months) h
 Infant iron deficiency c,g0.29 *
(0.00, 0.59)
0.03
(−0.03, 0.08)
0.32 *
(0.03, 0.61)
−−
a Values are regression coefficients (95% confidence intervals) for prenatal alcohol exposure from marginal structural models and the product method for variable triads where PAE (exposure) and a previously identified [12] PAE-related iron measure (potential mediator) were both related to an infant outcome [52,53]. This method allows for estimation of natural direct effects (exposure→outcome, independent of the potential mediator), natural indirect effects (exposure→outcome, through the potential mediator), and total effects with exposure-mediator interactions, with control for potential confounders (covariates from the relevant regression models in Table 2 and Table 3). PAE-mediator interaction was modeled with drinking frequency increasing from 0 to 3 days/week to mimic the weekend binge-drinking pattern seen in this cohort. Proportion mediation (%) = natural indirect effect/total effects, calculated only where p ≤ 0.10 natural indirect effect or total effect. b Age/sex-specific z-scores from World Health Organization norms [43]; n = 61 controls, 69 heavily exposed at 2 week; 67 controls, 80 heavily exposed at 5 years. c Positively related to prenatal alcohol exposure in previously published analyses [12]. d Negatively related to prenatal alcohol exposure in previously published analyses [12]. e From the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence [44,56]; n = 69 controls, 75 heavily exposed at 6.5 months; 65 controls, 79 heavily exposed at 12 months. f Highest level of imitated play complexity [48]; n = 64 controls, 78 heavily exposed. g Yes = 1; no = 0. h Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability temperament scale [50]; n = 70 controls, 81 heavily exposed.  p ≤ 0.10; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Carter, R.C.; Dodge, N.C.; Molteno, C.D.; Meintjes, E.M.; Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W. Mediating and Moderating Effects of Iron Homeostasis Alterations on Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4432. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204432

AMA Style

Carter RC, Dodge NC, Molteno CD, Meintjes EM, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Mediating and Moderating Effects of Iron Homeostasis Alterations on Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits. Nutrients. 2022; 14(20):4432. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204432

Chicago/Turabian Style

Carter, R. Colin, Neil C. Dodge, Christopher D. Molteno, Ernesta M. Meintjes, Joseph L. Jacobson, and Sandra W. Jacobson. 2022. "Mediating and Moderating Effects of Iron Homeostasis Alterations on Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits" Nutrients 14, no. 20: 4432. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204432

APA Style

Carter, R. C., Dodge, N. C., Molteno, C. D., Meintjes, E. M., Jacobson, J. L., & Jacobson, S. W. (2022). Mediating and Moderating Effects of Iron Homeostasis Alterations on Fetal Alcohol-Related Growth and Neurobehavioral Deficits. Nutrients, 14(20), 4432. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204432

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop