The Influence of Caffeine Expectancies on Sport, Exercise, and Cognitive Performance
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. CAF Expectancies and Sport and Exercise Performance
3. CAF Expectancies and Cognitive Performance
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Beedie, C.J.; Foad, A.J. The placebo effect in sports performance: A brief review. Sports Med. 2009, 39, 313–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Del Coso, J.; Munoz-Fernandez, V.E.; Munoz, G.; Fernandez-Elias, V.E.; Ortega, J.F.; Hamouti, N.; Barbero, C.J.; Munoz-Guerra, J. Effects of a caffeine-containing energy drink on simulated soccer performance. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Elliman, N.A.; Ash, J.; Green, M.W. Pre-existent expectancy effects in the relationship between caffeine and performance. Appetite 2010, 55, 355–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harrell, P.T.; Juliano, L.M. Caffeine expectancies influence the subjective and behavioral effects of caffeine. Psychopharmacology 2009, 207, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDaniel, L.W.; McIntire, K.; Streitz, C.; Jackson, A.; Guadet, L. The Effects of caffeine On Athletic Performance. Coll. Teach. Methods Styles J. 2010, 6, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saunders, B.; de Oliveira, L.F.; da Silva, R.P.; de Salles Painelli, V.; Goncalves, L.S.; Yamaguchi, G.; Mutti, T.; Maciel, E.; Roschel, H.; Artioli, G.G.; et al. Placebo in sports nutrition: A proof-of-principle study involving caffeine supplementation. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2017, 27, 1240–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burke, L.M. Caffeine and sports performance. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2008, 33, 1319–1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huntley, J.; Juliano, L.M. Caffeine Expectancy Questionnaire (CaffEQ): Construction, psychometric properties, and associations with caffeine use, caffeine dependence, and other related variables. Psychol. Assess. 2012, 24, 592–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lieberman, H.R.; Tharion, W.J.; Shukitt-Hale, B.; Speckman, K.L.; Tulley, R. Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Sea-Air-Land. Psychopharmacology 2002, 164, 250–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Foskett, A.; Ajmol, A.; Gant, N. Caffeine enhances cognitive function and skill performance during simulated soccer activity. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab. 2009, 19, 410–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pickering, C.; Kiely, J. Are the Current Guidelines on caffeine Use in Sport Optimal for Everyone? Inter-individual Variation in caffeine Ergogenicity, and a Move Towards Personalised Sports Nutrition. Sports Med. 2018, 48, 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gilbert, R.M. Caffeine consumption. Prog. Clin. Biol. Res. 1984, 158, 185–213. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Bchir, F.; Dogui, M.; Ben Fradj, R.; Arnaud, M.J.; Saquern, S. Differences in pharmacokinetic and electroencephalographic responses to caffeine in sleep-sensitive and non-sensitive subjects. C. R. Biol. 2006, 329, 512–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silverman, K.; Evans, S.M.; Strain, E.C.; Griffiths, R.R. Withdrawal syndrome after the double-blind cessation of caffeine consumption. N. Engl. J. Med. 1992, 327, 1109–1114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartley, T.R.; Sung, B.H.; Pincomb, G.A.; Whitsett, T.L.; Wilson, M.F.; Lovallo, W.R. Hypertension risk status and effect of caffeine on blood pressure. Hypertension 2000, 36, 137–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Green, P.J.; Kirby, R.; Suls, J. The effects of caffeine on blood pressure and heart rate: A review. Ann. Behav. Med. 1996, 18, 201–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abraham, J.; Mudd, J.O.; Kapur, N.K.; Klein, K.; Champion, H.C.; Wittstein, I.S. Stress cardiomyopathy after intravenous administration of catecholamines and beta-receptor agonists. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2009, 53, 1320–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chait, L.D. Factors influencing the subjective response to caffeine. Behav. Pharmacol. 1992, 3, 219–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kendler, K.S.; Schmitt, E.; Aggen, S.H.; Prescott, C.A. Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 678–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sachse, C.; Bhambra, U.; Smith, G.; Lightfoot, T.J.; Barett, J.H.; Scollay, J.; Garner, R.C.; Boobis, A.R.; Wolf, C.R.; Gooderham, N.J.; Colorectal Cancer Study Group. Polymorphisms in the cytochrome P450 CYP1A2 gene (CYP1A2) in colorectal cancer patients and controls: Allele frequencies, linkage disequilibrium and influence on caffeine metabolism. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2003, 55, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tracy, T.S.; Venkataramanan, R.; Glover, D.D.; Caritis, S.N.; National Institute for Child Health and Human Development Network of Maternal-Fetal-Medicine Units. Temporal changes in drug metabolism (CYP1A2, CYP2D6 and CYP3A Activity) during pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Glynecol. 2005, 192, 633–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Djordjevic, N.; Ghotbi, R.; Jankovic, S.; Aklillu, E. Induction of CYP1A2 by heavy coffee consumption is associated with the CYP1A2 -163C > A polymorphism. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2010, 66, 697–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cornelis, M.C.; El-Sohemy, A.; Kabagambe, E.K.; Campos, H. Coffee, CYP1A2 genotype, and risk of myocardial infarction. JAMA 2006, 295, 1135–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- El-Sohemy, A.; Cornelis, M.C.; Kabagambe, E.K.; Campos, H. Coffe, CYP1A2 genotype and risk of myocardial infarction. Genes Nutr. 2007, 2, 155–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carrillo, J.A.; Benitez, J. CYP1A2 activity, gender and smoking, as variables influencing the toxicity of caffeine. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1996, 41, 605–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ou-Yang, D.-S.; Huang, S.-L.; Wang, W.; Xie, H.-G.; Xu, Z.-H.; Shu, Y.; Zhou, H.-H. Phenotypic polymorphism and gender-related differences of CYP1A2 activity in a Chinese population. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2000, 49, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, A.; Palmer, A.A.; de Wit, H. Genetics of caffeine consumption and responses to caffeine. Psychopharmacology 2010, 211, 245–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Soyama, A.; Saito, Y.; Hanioka, N.; Maekawa, K.; Komamura, K.; Kamakura, S.; Kitakaze, M.; Tomoike, H.; Ueno, K.; Goto, Y.; et al. Single nucleotide polymorphisms and haplotypes of CYP1A2 in a Japanese population. Drug Metab. Pharmacokinet. 2005, 20, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Higgins, M.F.; Shabir, A. Expectancy of ergogenicity from sodium bicarbonate ingestion increases high-intensity cycling capacity. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2016, 41, 405–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McClung, M.; Collins, D. “Because I know it will!”: Placebo effects of an ergogenic aid on athletic performance. J. Sport Exerc. Physiol. 2007, 29, 382–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirsch, I. Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. Am. Psychol. 1985, 40, 1189–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mark, A. Placebo: The Belief Effect. J. R. Soc. Med. 2003, 96, 199–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montgomery, G.H.; Kirsch, I. Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain 1997, 72, 107–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedetti, F.; Arduino, C.; Costa, S.; Vighetti, S.; Tarenzi, L.; Rainero, I.; Asteggiano, G. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain 2006, 121, 133–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ross, M.; Olson, J.M. An expectancy-attribution model of the effects of placebos. Psychol. Rev. 1981, 88, 408–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Foad, A.J.; Beedie, C.J.; Coleman, D.A. Pharmacological and psychological effects of caffeine ingestion in 40-km cycling performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2008, 40, 158–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tallis, J.; Muhammad, B.; Islam, M.; Duncan, M.J. Placebo effects of caffeine on maximal voluntary concentric force of the knee flexors and extensors. Muscle Nerve 2016, 54, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ariel, G.; Saville, W. The effect of anabolic steroids on reflex components. Med. Sci. Sports 1972, 4, 124–126. [Google Scholar]
- Maganaris, C.N.; Collins, D.; Sharp, M. Expectancy Effects and Strength Training: Do Steroids Make a Difference? Sport Psychol. 2000, 14, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, V.R.; Hopkins, W.G.; Hawley, J.A.; Burke, L.M. Placebo effect of carbohydrate feedings during a 40-km cycling time trial. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2000, 32, 1642–1647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hulston, C.J.; Jeukendrup, A.E. No placebo effect from carbohydrate intake during prolonged exercise. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Metab. 2009, 19, 275–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalasountas, V.; Reed, J.; Fitzpatrick, J. The Effect of Placebo-Induced Changes in Expectancies on Maximal Force Production in College Students. J. Appl. Sport Psych. 2004, 19, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porcari, J.; Jennifer, O.; Heidi, F.; Richard, M.; Foster, C. The placebo effect on exercise performance. J. Cardiopulm. Rehabil. Rev. 2006, 25, 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, G.; Porcari, J.P.; Foster, C.; Felker, H.; Kosholek, J.; Otto, E.M.; Sorenson Udermann, B. Placebo effects on Exercise Performance. Med. J. 2009, 6, 3–7. [Google Scholar]
- Beedie, C.J.; Stuart, E.M.; Coleman, D.A.; Foad, A.J. Placebo effects of caffeine on cycling performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2006, 38, 2159–2164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duncan, M.J.; Lyons, M.; Hankey, J. Placebo effects of caffeine on short-term resistance exercise to failure. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 2009, 4, 244–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scarpina, F.; Tangini, S. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vakil, E.; Weisz, H.; Jedwab, L.; Groswasser, Z.; Aberbuch, S. Stroop color-word task as a measure of selective attention: Efficiency in closed-head-injured patients. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1995, 17, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anuar, N.; Williams, S.E.; Cumming, J. Do the physical and environment PETTLEP elements predict sport imagery ability? Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2017, 17, 1319–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verburgh, L.; Scherder, E.J.; Van Lange, P.A.; Oosterlaan, J. Do Elite and Amateur Soccer Players Outperform Non-Athletes on Neurocognitive Functioning? A Study Among 8-12 Year Old Children. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wulf, G.; McConnel, N.; Gartner, M.; Schwarz, A. Enhancing the learning of sport skills through external-focus feedback. J. Motor Behav. 2002, 34, 171–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jackson, S.A. The growth of qualitative research in sport psychology. In Sport Psychology: Theory, Applications and Issues; Morris, T., Summers, J.J., Eds.; John Wiley: Milton, UK, 1995; pp. 575–591. [Google Scholar]
- Pollo, A.; Carlino, E.; Benedetti, F. The top-down influence of ergogenic placebos on muscle work and fatigue. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Walach, H.; Schmidt, S.; Dirhold, T.; Nosch, S. The effects of a caffeine placebo and suggestion on blood pressure, heart rate, well-being and cognitive performance. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2002, 43, 247–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oei, A.; Hartley, L.R. The effects of caffeine and expectancy on attention and memory. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2005, 20, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fillmore, M.; Vogel-Sprott, M. Expected effect of caffeine on motor performance predicts the type of response to placebo. Psychopharmacology 1992, 106, 209–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tunnicliffe, J.M.; Erdman, K.A.; Reimer, R.A.; Lun, V.; Shearer, J. Consumption of dietary caffeine and coffee in physically active populations: Physiological interactions. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2008, 33, 1301–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borg, G.A. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1982, 14, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chavarria, V.; Vian, J.; Pereira, C.; Data-Franco, J.; Fernandes, B.S.; Berk, M.; Dodd, S. The Placebo and Nocebo Phenomena: Their Clinical Management and Impact on Treatment Outcomes. Clin. Ther. 2017, 39, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, C.M.; House, T.J.; Hill, E.C.; Schmidt, R.J.; Johnson, G.O. Time Course of Changes in Neuromuscular Responses at 30% versus 70% 1 Repetition Maximum during Dynamic Constant External Resistance Leg Extensions to Failure. Int. J. Exerc. Sci. 2017, 10, 365–375. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Bishop, P.A.; Jones, E.; Woods, A.K. Recovery from training: A brief review: Brief review. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2008, 22, 1015–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duncan, M.J. Placebo effects of caffeine on anaerobic performance in moderately trained adults. Serb. J. Sports Sci. 2010, 4, 99–106. [Google Scholar]
- Pollo, A.; Amanzio, M.; Arslanian, A.; Casadio, C.; Maggi, G.; Benedetti, F. Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain 2001, 93, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vase, L.; Riley, J.L., 3rd; Price, D.D. A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. Pain 2002, 99, 443–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geers, A.L.; Kosbab, K.; Helfer, S.G.; Weiland, P.E.; Wellman, J.A. Further evidence for individual differences in placebo responding: An interactionist perspective. J. Psychosom. Res. 2007, 62, 563–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yerkes, R.M.; Dodson, J.D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. 1908, 18, 459–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beedie, C.J. Placebo Effects in Competitive Sport: Qualitative Data. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2007, 6, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Vogel-Sprott, M.; Fillmore, M. The placebo response to alcohol: A three-expectancy problem? University of Waterloo: Waterloo, Canada, (found in [56]). 1990; unpublished work. [Google Scholar]
- Walach, H.; Schmidt, S.; Bihr, Y.-M.; Wiesch, S. The effectsof a caffeine placebo and experimenter expectation onblood pressure, heart rate, well-being and cognitive performance:a failure to reproduce. J. Eur. Psychiatry 2001, 6, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinclair, C.J.; Geiger, J.D. Caffeine use in sports. A pharmacological review. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 2000, 40, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Somani, S.M.; Gupta, P. Caffeine: A new look at an age-old drug. Int. J. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. Toxicol. 1988, 26, 521–533. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Dawkins, L.; Shahzad, F.Z.; Ahmed, S.S.; Edmonds, C.J. Expectation of having consumed caffeine can improve performance and mood. Appetite 2011, 57, 597–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moerman, D.E.; Jonas, W.B. Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response. Ann. Intern. Med. 2002, 136, 471–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zimmerman, B.J. Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner: An Overview. Theory Pract. 2002, 41, 67–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, R.; Gruner, M.; Heiland, A.; Keller, M.; Kujanova, Z.; Peper, M.; Reigl, M.; Schmidt, S.; Volz, P.; Walach, H. Effects of expectation and caffeine on arousal, well-being, and reaction time. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2006, 13, 330–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, S.M.; Griffiths, R.R. Caffeine tolerance and choice in humans. Psychopharmacology 1992, 108, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denson, T.F.; Jacobsen, M.; von Hippel, W.; Kemp, R.I.; Mak, T. CAF expectancies but not CAF reduce depletion-induced aggression. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2012, 26, 140–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stuart, G.R.; Hopkins, W.G.; Cook, C.; Cairns, S.P. Multiple effects of caffeine on simulated high-intensity team-sport performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2005, 37, 1998–2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Golby, J.; Sheard, M. Mental toughness and hardiness at different levels of rugby league. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2004, 37, 933–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guidetti, L.; Musulin, A.; Baldari, C. Physiological factors in middleweight boxing performance. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 2002, 42, 309–314. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Rogers, J.P.; Dernoncourt, C. Regular caffeine consumption: A balance of adverse and beneficial effects for mood and psychomotor performance. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1998, 59, 1039–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeomans, M.R.; Ripley, T.; Davies, L.H.; Rusted, J.M.; Rogers, P.J. Effects of caffeine on performance and mood depend on the level of caffeine abstinence. Psychopharmacology 2002, 164, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- James, J.E.; Rogers, P.J. Effects of caffeine on performance and mood: Withdrawal reversal is the most plausible explanation. Psychopharmacology 2005, 182, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Vohs, K.D.; Tice, D.M. The Strength Model of Self-Control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 351–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- DeWall, C.N.; Baumeister, R.F.; Stillman, T.F.; Gailliot, M.T. Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 43, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Domotor, Z.; Szemersky, R.; Koteles, F. Subjective and objective effects of coffee consumption—Caffeine or expectations? Acta Physiol. Hung. 2015, 102, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cha, S.D.; Patel, H.P.; Hains, D.S.; Mahan, J.D. The Effects of Hypertension on Cognitive Function in Children and Adolescents. Int. J. Pediatr. 2012, 2012, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peters, A.; McEwen, B.S.; Friston, K. Uncertainty and stress: Why it causes diseases and how it is mastered by the brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 2017, 156, 164–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Author(s) | Sample Characteristics | Experimental Design & Main Outcome Measure(s) | Intervention/Informed | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|
Beedie et al. [45] | 7 well trained male cyclists (30 ± 11 years). Habitual caffeine consumption not reported | Design Deceptive administration, randomised, within-subjects and double-blind Main outcome measure 10 km cycle ergometer time trial | Received PLA during all trials. No treatment Control (CON) Informed Placebo (PLA) 4.5 mg/kg/BM caffeine (CAF-LOW) 9 mg/kg/BM (CAF-HIGH) CON Expectancy manipulation Literature detailing caffeine ergogenicity amongst elite cyclists. | Perceived placebo reduced mean power output by −2.3 W vs. baseline. Perception of 4.5 mg/kg/BM and 9 mg/kg/BM caffeine increased mean power output by 4 and 9.3 W vs. baseline, respectively. |
Foad et al. [36] | 14 male (43 ± 7 years), moderate caffeine consuming (310 ± 75 mg) recreational cyclists | Design Double-dissociation, within-subjects, non-randomised and single-blind Main outcome measure 40 km cycle ergometer time trial | Received Saline solutions (told for hydration purposes only) containing PLA or CAF (5 mg/kg/BM) Informed Given CAF told CAF (GC/TC) Given CAF/told PLA (GC/TP) Given PLA/told CAF (GP/TC) Given PLA/told PLA (GP/TP) Expectancy manipulation Placebo capsule perceived to contain 5 mg/kg/BM CAF and a 90-min presentation displaying CAF benefits on cycling performance | Consumption (3.5 ± 2.0%) and belief of CAF (0.7–1.4%), respectively resulted in very likely and possibly beneficial increases in MPO. Following CAF consumption, individuals were 100%, 99% and 98% likely to display improvements in MPO equivalent to 0.5%, 1.0% and 1.5%, respectively. The chances of improved MPO following belief of CAF only, was 62%, 33% and 12%, respectively. Synergism of caffeine belief and pharmacology (2.6 ± 3.3%) indicated improvements following lower expectations. A possibly harmful nocebo effect (−1.9% ± 2.2%) was observed for given PLA/told PLA. |
Pollo et al. [53] | 44 male undergraduate students (22 ± 2 years). Habitual caffeine consumption N/A | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measure Knee extension exercise at 60% 1 repetition maximum (1 RPM) | Received PLA No treatment CON Informed 20 mL caffeinated coffee (CAF) CON Expectancy manipulation Literature displaying CAF benefits on resistance exercise. During study 2, two acute conditioning sessions were included, whereby exercise intensity was reduced to 45% 1 RPM but perceived as 60% 1 RPM | CAF increased PPO (11.8 ± 16.1%) and repetitions performed (2.53) versus baseline, however no effect was observed for a control. A greater placebo effect was observed during study 2 with more repetitions (4.82) performed and a greater improvement in PPO (22.1 ± 23.5%) for CAF versus baseline. CAF also reduced perceptual exertion (RPE) (~1) and this was for repetitions 3, 6, 9, 12 and 15 during study 2. |
Duncan et al. [46] | 12 resistance trained male participants (23 ± 6 years). Habitual caffeine consumption not reported | Design Deceptive administration, within-subjects, randomised and double-blind Main outcome measure Single leg knee extension at 60% 1 RPM | Received 250 mL artificially sweetened water No treatment CON Informed CAF (3 mg/kg/BM) PLA CON Expectancy manipulation Literature displaying the benefits of CAF on resistance-based exercise performance | CAF increased the number of repetitions performed (20 ± 5) and weight lifted (weight x repetitions) (713 ± 121 kg) versus CON (16 ± 4; 577 ± 101 kg) and PLA (18 ± 4; 656 ± 155 kg), respectively. RPE was ~1 unit lower for CAF versus PLA, but similar for CAF and CON. |
Duncan et al. [62] | 12 male (24 ± 4 years) moderate caffeine consuming (250 mg per day) trained participants | Design Double-dissociation, randomised, within-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measure 30 s Wingate test at a resistance equivalent to 7.5% BM | Received 250 mL artificially sweetened water combined with 5 mg/kg/BM or PLA Informed GC/TC GC/TP GP/TC GP/TP Expectancy manipulation Literature reviewing the benefits of caffeine on high intensity exercise performance | GC/TC significantly increased PPO, MPO and lowered RPE, in comparison to all other conditions. No significant differences were observed for GP/TC versus GC/TP. However, both groups improved PPO (59.5 and 48.9 W) and RPE (−1 and −1), versus GP/TP, respectively. |
Tallis et al. [37] | 14 male (21 ± 1 years) low caffeine consuming (92 ± 17 mg per day) participants | Design Double-dissociation, randomised, counterbalanced and single-blind Main outcome measure Maximal voluntary concentric force and fatigue resistance of the knee flexors and extensors at velocities equivalent to 30° per second and 120° per second | Received Orange squash solutions (4 mL/kg/BM water and 1 mL/kg/BM sugar free orange squash) with or without 5 mg/kg/BM caffeine Informed GC/TC GC/TP GP/TC GP/TP Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed TP orange squash solutions contained no caffeine. | Peak force produced for GC/TP and GC/TC was comparable, but significantly greater versus GP/TP at both 30° per second (12.8% and 15.8%) and 120° per second (6.8% and 11.2%, respectively). Only GC/TC produced significantly greater average force production versus GP/TP, at both 30° per second (18%) and 120° per second (14.4%), respectively. |
Saunders et al. [6] | 42 male (37 ± years) moderate habitual caffeine consuming (195 ± 56 mg per day) trained cyclists | Design Randomised, counterbalanced, double-blind and within-subjects Main outcome measures Cycle ergometer time trial at 85% peak power output Questionnaire exploring which supplement participants believed they had ingested pre and post exercise | Received Capsules containing CAF (6 mg/kg/BM) or PLA. No treatment CON Informed N/A CON Expectancy manipulation N/A | Correct identification of CAF (n = 17) increased MPO by 4.5% (+10 W) versus CON. Three more participants correctly identified CAF post-exercise, this increased MPO by a further 1.3% (+3 W). MBI indicated 100% chance of beneficial effects after administration and correct identification of caffeine. Correct identification of PLA (n = 17) decreased MPO by −0.8% for PLA (−2 W) versus CON. One more participant identified PLA post-exercise, this decreased MPO by -a further 0.6% (−1 W) versus CON. The chance of harmful effects at pre-exercise and post-exercise was 31% and 47%, respectively. Expectation for CAF following PLA ingestion (n = 8) increased MPO by 2.5% (+5 W) versus CON. Three more participants incorrectly perceived PLA as CAF post-exercise, this increased MPO by a further 0.9% (+3 W) versus CON. The chance of beneficial effects at pre-exercise and post-exercise, was 66% and 87%, respectively. |
Author(s) | Sample Characteristics | Experimental Design & Main Performance Measure(s) | Intervention/Informed | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|
Fillmore & Vogel-Sprott [56] | 56 male (19–29 years) low caffeine consuming (2 ± 2 cups of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Deceptive administration, single-blind and between-subjects Main outcome measure Computerised pursuit rotor task adjudged by % time correctly following moving object | Received Decaffeinated coffee No treatment CON Informed Caffeinated coffee CON Expectancy manipulation ‘Fairly strong dose of coffee’ was prepared in front of participants. Groups were subsequently informed caffeine would positively (E+), negatively (E−) or not effect performance (E?) | Baseline psychomotor performance was similar between all groups. Additionally, all participants expected caffeine to have negligible influence. The expected effect of caffeine predicted the placebo response observed with E+ displaying the greatest performance benefits (67.5 ± 10.27%) vs. E− (49.17 ± 14.20%), E? (57.40 ± 11.78%) and CON (57.62 ± 9.98%). |
Walach et al. [54] | 53 male and 104 female (28 ± 8 years) regular caffeine consuming (≥1 cup of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and double-blind Main outcome measure Self-devised test (finding misprints in a text), and Wally the worm video game | Received Decaffeinated coffee No treatment CON Informed Caffeinated coffee Decaffeinated coffee Double-blind administration CON Expectancy manipulation Flyer describing caffeine’s effects on concentration levels | No expectancy effect observed. |
Walach et al. [69] | 44 male undergraduate students (22 ± 2 years). Habitual caffeine consumption not reported | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and double-blind Main outcome measure Self-devised test finding misprints in a text and clicking X on a computer when a previously denoted sequence of numbers appeared once more | Received Decaffeinated coffee No treatment CON Informed Caffeinated coffee Decaffeinated coffee. Double-blind administration CON Expectancy manipulation Flyer describing caffeine’s effects on concentration levels | No expectancy effect observed. |
Oei & Hartley [55] | 11 male and 21 female (25 ± 8 years) low caffeine consuming (≤120 mg per day or 2 cups of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Deceptive administration, mixed-factorial, between-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measure Sustained attention, memory, and delayed recall task | Received 250 mL caffeinated (~143 mg) or decaffeinated coffee Informed GC/TC GC/TP GP/TC GP/TP Expectancy manipulation Caffeinated coffee prepared in front of participants Participants were also allowed to inspect the jar that was perceived to contain caffeine | For sustained attention, more correct detections were observed for told caffeine (69.05 ± 0.97) and given caffeine (69.00 ± 1.23) versus placebo (66.48 ± 1.51 and 66.53 ± 1.21, respectively) for individuals displaying positive habituated expectancies only. Participants committed fewer false alarms for told caffeine (5.42 ± 0.78) and given caffeine (5.42 ± 0.68) versus placebo (7.11 ± 1.01 and 7.11 ± 1.08, respectively). |
Schneider et al. [75] | 20 males and 25 female German adults (27 ± 8 years) Habitual caffeine consumption not reported | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and double-blind Main outcome measure The interactive test battery for attentional performance [75] | Received 250 mL caffeinated (2 mg/kg/BM) orange juice solution in all trials Informed Caffeinated orange juice solution Non-caffeinated orange juice solution Expectancy manipulation Flyer describing caffeine’s effects on the central nervous, cognitive and cardiovascular systems | No expectancy effect observed. |
Harrell & Juliano [4] | 19 male and 41 female (23 years) regular caffeine consuming (463 ± 208 mg per day) adults | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measures Rapid visual information processing (RVIP), and finger tapping tasks Perceived motivation was explored prior to cognitive performance using a 4-point Likert scale (0—not at all, to 4—extremely) | Received 500 mL caffeinated (280 mg) coffee 500 mL decaffeinated coffee Informed Caffeinated coffee Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed caffeine would either enhance or impair performance | CAF consumption resulted in improvements across all performance measures versus PLA, however no significant differences were observed between told impair/enhance conditions. Told enhance increased motivation for the RVIP (+0.58) and finger tapping task (+0.87) versus told impair. However, given placebo/told impair resulted in greater improvements in reaction time (−10.08 ± 10.67 milliseconds (ms)) and RVIP hits (+2.67 ± 2.33) versus given placebo/told enhance. |
Elliman et al. [3] | 6 male and 21 female (21 years) habitual caffeine consuming (≥1 cup of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Double-dissociation, within-subjects, counter-balanced and single blind Main outcome measure Bakan vigilance task | Received 200 mL caffeinated (200 mg) or decaffeinated coffee Informed GC/TC GC/TP GP/TC GP/TP Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed decaffeinated coffee was administered in TP conditions | No effect was observed for mean correct and false hits for GC/TP (3.88 and 0.31 hits) versus GP/TC (3.72 and 0.32 hits), respectively. Neither group presented a meaningful improvement versus GP/TP. Significant differences for correct hits were observed for GC/TC versus GC/TP (+0.24) and GP/TC (+0.40), respectively. |
Dawkins et al. [72] | 44 male and 44 female habitual caffeine consuming 75 mg per day) undergraduate students | Design Double-dissociation, between-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measures A card sorting task, 40 congruent (printed words and colours the same) and 40 incongruent stimulus tasks | Received 250 mL caffeinated (75 mg) or decaffeinated coffee Informed GC/TC GC/TP GP/TC GP/TP Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed decaffeinated coffee was administered in TP conditions | GC/TC performed the best on all 3 performance measures, whilst GP/TP performed the worst. GP/TC performed better on the congruent (39 versus 36 correct responses), incongruent (37 versus 35 correct responses) and card sorting task (10% faster) versus GC/TP. |
Denson et al. [77] | 63 male and 61 female (27 ± 8 years) light caffeine consuming (≤1 cup of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and single-blind Main outcome measures The Taylor aggression paradigm following cognitive depletion (e.g., exhausting reading task and aggression provocation procedure) | Received CAF tablets (200 mg) PLA tablets No tablet CON Informed CAF tablets CON Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed CAF tablets were equivalent to 2 cups of coffee | Following cognitive depletion, PLA resulted in greater executive control capacity versus CON and CAF. No difference was observed for CAF vs CON. |
Domotor et al. [86] | 42 male and 65 female (22 ± 4 years) habitual caffeine consuming (3 ± 1 cups of coffee per day) undergraduate students | Design Deceptive administration, between-subjects and double-blind. Main outcome measure Simple reaction time using the PsychLabWin v.1.1 software (Informer technologies Inc., Washington, DC, USA). | Received Caffeinated coffee (5 mg/kg/BM) Decaffeinated coffee No treatment CON Informed CON Conditional placebo (Group 2) Conditional caffeine (Group 3) Deceived placebo (Group 4) Caffeine (group 5) Expectancy manipulation Verbally informed CAF tablets were equivalent to 2 cups of coffee | No expectancy effect observed. |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Shabir, A.; Hooton, A.; Tallis, J.; F. Higgins, M. The Influence of Caffeine Expectancies on Sport, Exercise, and Cognitive Performance. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1528. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10101528
Shabir A, Hooton A, Tallis J, F. Higgins M. The Influence of Caffeine Expectancies on Sport, Exercise, and Cognitive Performance. Nutrients. 2018; 10(10):1528. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10101528
Chicago/Turabian StyleShabir, Akbar, Andy Hooton, Jason Tallis, and Matthew F. Higgins. 2018. "The Influence of Caffeine Expectancies on Sport, Exercise, and Cognitive Performance" Nutrients 10, no. 10: 1528. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10101528
APA StyleShabir, A., Hooton, A., Tallis, J., & F. Higgins, M. (2018). The Influence of Caffeine Expectancies on Sport, Exercise, and Cognitive Performance. Nutrients, 10(10), 1528. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10101528