Introduction
Eye blinking is an essential physiological function for protecting the eyes and preserving ocular health. Eye blinks maintain a vital tear film on the ocular surface. In between blinks, the tear film becomes thinner and non-uniform across the corneal surface (
Tutt et al., 2000;
Wong et al., 2002). Long durations between blinks may lead to the disruption of the tear film and introduce deviations in the eye’s optical system (
Himebaugh et al., 2009). The tear film is immediately restored after a blink. Therefore, the ability to maintain a healthy tear film depends heavily on the eye blinking rate (
Wong et al., 2002).
The eye blinking rate is influenced by many factors, such as ambient lighting, gaze direction, mental activity, attention, and task difficulty (
Andrzejewska & Stolińska, 2016;
Cho et al., 2000;
Hammerschmidt & Wollner, 2018;
Holland & Tarlow, 1972;
Holm et al., 2021;
Kaakinen & Simola, 2020;
Smith & Henderson, 2008). Multiple studies have investigated eye blinking rates at different conditions. Eye blinking rates were reported higher when subjects were engaged in conversations, compared to blinking rates while performing other tasks, such as reading, watching or waiting (
Doughty, 2001). Conversational eye blinking rates were reported at 10.5 - 32.5 blinks/min, which is much higher than blinking rates when reading in a reading posture (1.4 - 14.4 blinks/min) or in a primary gaze position (8.0 - 21.0 blinks/min) (
Abusharha, 2017;
Cho et al., 2000;
Doughty, 2001;
Portello et al., 2013).
Working on computers for long hours has become a regular daily routine for millions of people around the world (
Rosenfield et al., 2012;
Sen & Richardson, 2007). The drastically high number of computer users has led to the increase of health issues related to prolonged computer use, such as eye strain, eye fatigue, eye dryness, eye irritation, headaches and blurred vision (
Akinbinu & Mashalla, 2014;
Blehm et al., 2005;
Portello et al., 2013;
Sen & Richardson, 2007). The group of eye and vision complications associated with prolonged computer use is defined as computer vision syndrome (
Blehm et al., 2005;
Portello et al., 2013). The primary causes of computer vision syndrome are ocular motor and ocular surface factors (
Blehm et al., 2005;
Rosenfield, 2011). Ocular motor responses to computer screens, such as accommodation and vergence, appear to be similar to viewing printed materials (
Rosenfield, 2011). However, multiple studies have reported that changes in ocular surface, specifically dry eyes, is strongly associated with prolonged computer use (
Uchino et al., 2008,
2013). Furthermore, reading on computer screens produced the highest disturbance to the tear film of ocular surfaces compared to other reading devices and control (
Talens-Estarelles et al., 2020).
A main contributor to computer vision syndrome caused by dry eyes is the reduction of eye blinking rates (
Blehm et al., 2005). Previous studies have shown that eye blinking rates while working on computers were reduced to 30 - 40% of the spontaneous eye blinking rate (
Freudenthaler et al., 2003;
Schlote et al., 2004;
Tsubota & Nakamori, 1993). Patients suffering from computer vision syndrome caused by dry eyes due to decreased eye blinking rates are typically prescribed with lubricating eye drops to alleviate the symptoms of this syndrome (
Blehm et al., 2005). However, these eye drops can cause a decrease in overall visual acuity and many users were dissatisfied with the therapeutic effects of eye drops (
Blehm et al., 2005;
Shimmura et al., 1999). Another common treatment solution to computer vision syndrome is taking frequent breaks from working on a computer (
Blehm et al., 2005). However, this solution may not be practical for many workers due to the demanding nature of their work, which requires them to continue working without interruption.
Recent technological advancements provide a valuable opportunity to explore impactful solutions to prevent computer vision syndrome. A research group developed a wearable device that uses infrared (IR) sensing to continuously track eye blinking rates and triggers blinks by light flashes, physical taps or air puffs near the eye (
Dementyev & Holz, 2017). The study concluded that intense air puffs near the eye were the most effective in triggering blinks with low distraction ratings. However, the continuous emission of IR light directly to the eye and frequent intense air puffs near the eye raise safety concerns for the long-term use of this device (
Kourkoumelis & Tzaphlidou, 2011). Another research group designed wearable blink-sensing glasses using a pressure sensor (
Chen et al., 2021). The pressure sensor was developed by the research group using novel flexible iontronic sensing (FITS) technology that is not currently available in the market. Blinking monitors using a web camera have been developed with screen notifications to trigger blinks (
Crnovrsanin et al., 2014;
Divjak & Bischof, 2009;
Lapa et al., 2023). These blinking monitors utilize the processing power of the computer being occupied by the user and screen notifications that may interfere with the user’s work on the screen. Therefore, there is a need for a blinking monitoring system composing of easily available components that is not obtrusive to the user and does not interfere with the processing unit or screen display of the user’s computer.
In this pilot study, a standalone eye blinking monitoring system using a camera and multiple independent alarm modes (audible, visual and tactile) was developed to induce eye blinks for computer users. The system does not visually interfere with the user’s work or impede the user’s computer performance. Furthermore, the components of the monitoring system are affordable and easily available in the market. The hypothesis in this study is that the developed eye blinking monitoring system would increase eye blinks for a computer user. To test this hypothesis, the eye blinking monitoring system was evaluated on a group of subjects to determine the impact of the blinking monitoring system on blinking rates while the subjects performed different tasks on a computer.
Results
The evaluation studies were performed on 20 subjects (13 male, 7 female) with a mean age of 26.7 years (age range: 18 – 60 years). None of the subjects had any known eye conditions during the study. Most of the subjects (12) did not normally were glasses or contact lenses, and 8 subjects normally wore glasses but they did not wear the glasses during the study tasks. The subjects were requested to remove their eye glasses for the consistency of the evaluation study. Removing eye glasses may have caused irritation for a few subjects but a subject remained in the same situation in all the tasks.
From the data obtained in the first task, the blinking counts of the eye blinking monitoring system were compared to the true blinking counts determined manually by watching the full-length of the recorded videos to calculate the error rate and accuracy. The overall mean error rate for the eye blinking monitoring system was 4.1 ± 4.1%. The accuracy of the eye blinking monitoring system was 95.9%. The mean difference between the blink counts of the blinking monitoring system and the actual blink counts (bias) was −1.7 blinks. The negative sign indicates that on average, the blink counts of the blinking monitoring system was lower than the actual blink counts. The 95% limits of agreement (mean ± 1.96 standard deviation) were −5.3 and 1.9 blinks. The Bland-Altman plot for the blinking monitoring system and actual blink counts is shown in
Fig. 3. As expected, the data points in
Fig. 3 are clustered around 36 and 42 blinks/min since the subjects were instructed to blink at the sound of a metronome at 36 and 42 BPM in the first task.
The overall mean spontaneous (conversational) eye blinking rate for all the subjects was 43.1 ± 14.7 blinks/min. The eye blinking rate significantly decreased to 25.2 ± 11.9 blinks/min when the subjects were watching movie trailers (without the alarm) (
p < 0.001, estimated median difference: −17.6 blinks/min, 95% CI: −22.9 to −14.0 blinks/min). A significant decrease to 24.2 ± 12.1 blinks/min was also observed when the subjects were reading articles (without the alarm), compared to the spontaneous eye blinking rate (
p < 0.001, estimated median difference: −18.8 blinks/min, 95% CI: −24.3 to −13.2 blinks/min). There was no significant difference between the blinking rates of the subjects when watching movie trailers and reading articles without the alarm (
p = 0.71, estimated median difference: −0.8 blinks/min, 95% CI: −5.2 to 4.0 blinks/min). The significance level was adjusted according to a Bonferroni correction in these tests (α = 0.05/3 = 0.017). The mean spontaneous eye blinking rate and blinking rates during movie trailers and reading articles for all the subjects are shown in
Fig. 4.
The spontaneous eye blinking rate was calculated over a time period of 6.1 ± 1.4 min. Subjects completed the task of watching movie trailers in 4.4 ± 0.6 min without the alarm and 4.6 ± 1.9 min with the alarm (no significant difference, p = 0.49). The task of reading articles took 3.6 ± 1.8 min for the subjects to complete without the alarm and 2.9 ± 0.7 min with the alarm, with no significant difference (p = 0.33).
The threshold for the alarm of the eye blinking monitoring system was set between 1 and 6 blinks/10 sec (6 - 36 blinks/min), which was 50% of the spontaneous eye blinking rate for each subject. The number of alarm messages that occurred in the task of watching movie trailers with the alarm of the eye blinking monitor turned on was 9.0 ± 7.7 (range: 0 - 27) alarm messages, while 6.8 ± 5.7 (range: 0 – 20) alarm messages occurred in the task of reading articles. The overall mean blinking rate during the movie trailers without the alarm and with the alarm was 25.2 ± 11.9 blinks/min and 28.2 ± 12.1 blinks/min, respectively. The difference was not statistically significant (
p = 0.09, estimated median difference: 2.8 blinks/min, 95% CI: −0.7 to 6.5 blinks/min) and the Cohen’s effect size (
d) was 0.25. The mean blinking rate when the subjects were reading articles without the alarm was 24.2 ± 12.1 blinks/min, which was not statistically different than the mean blinking rate of 24.6 ± 11.3 blinks/min with the alarm (
p = 0.68, estimated median difference: 0.5 blinks/min, 95% CI: −2.1 to 3.8 blinks/min,
d = 0.03). The mean of all the blinking rates for all the subjects without the alarm during both tasks (watching movie trailer and reading articles) was 24.7 ± 11.9 blinks/min, while the mean of all the blinking rates with the alarm was 26.4 ± 11.7 blinks/min, with no statistically significant difference (
p = 0.13, estimated median difference: 1.6 blinks/min, 95% CI: −0.4 to 4.0 blinks/min) and Cohen’s effect size of
d = 0.14. The mean blinking rate for each subject while watching movie trailers and reading articles without and with the alarm is shown in
Fig. 5. A summary for all the blinking rates observed in this study is shown in
Table 1.
An online System Usability Scale (SUS) questionnaire was sent out to the participating subjects and 17 subjects (85%) responded. The SUS score for the usability of the developed eye blinking monitoring system was 74.6 out of 100, which is a grade of B according to the Sauro-Lewis curved grading scale for general interpretation of SUS scores (
Lewis, 2018;
Sauro & Lewis, 2016).
Discussion
An intelligent, standalone eye blinking monitoring system for computer users was developed in this study. The developed system used a camera to track blinking rates and operated audible, visual and tactile alarm modes to induce eye blinks (
Fig. 1 and
Fig. 2). A personalized alarm threshold was set at 50% of the spontaneous (conversational) eye blinking rate for each user. The hypothesis in this study is that the developed eye blinking monitoring system would increase eye blinks for a computer user. The eye blinking monitoring system detected blinks with high accuracy (95.9%). A significant decrease was observed in blinking rates when subjects were watching movie trailers or reading articles on a computer, compared to spontaneous eye blinking rates (
Fig. 4). The blinking monitoring system with the alarm function turned on showed an increase in blinking rates compared to blinking rates without the alarm function when watching movie trailers (
Fig. 5), which supports the hypothesis. This work is an advancement towards the development of an effective technological solution for preventing computer vision syndrome to the highly prevalent and drastically increasing number of computer users around the world.
The eye blinking monitoring system successfully detected eye blinks with an accuracy of 95.9%. This accuracy level is similar to the accuracy levels of previous studies using infrared (IR) sensors (85.2%) (
Dementyev & Holz, 2017), pressures sensors (96.3%) (
Chen et al., 2021), cameras (85 - 97%) (
Crnovrsanin et al., 2014;
Divjak & Bischof, 2009;
Ferhat et al., 2014;
Tansakul & Tangamchit, 2015) and microphones (95%) (
Liu et al., 2021). The Bland-Altman plot in
Fig. 3 showed that the eye blinking monitoring system on average underestimated the actual blink counts by 1.7 ± 1.8 blinks. The undetected blinks by the monitoring system were mostly involuntarily rapid consecutive blinks, which were the cause for the outlier data point observed in
Fig. 3. The programming code of the monitoring system enters a standby period of 0.5 sec in the event of a blink before registering any other blinks. This standby period was introduced to avoid multiple counts for a single blink. However, this standby period needs to be investigated and adjusted in future studies to increase the detection accuracy of the eye blinking monitoring system. Another possible factor that may have caused the misdetection of blinks is the manual determination of the suitable Eye Ratio threshold for each subject. Eye Ratio is the ratio of distances between eyelid landmarks to assess whether a blink has occurred. The Eye Ratio was selected manually in this study but the development of an automated calibration process, including the selection of a suitable Eye Ratio for each user, is expected to considerably reduce the blink misdetections, and in return, increase the accuracy of the system. Furthermore, calculating the spontaneous eye blinking rate to determine the personalized alarm threshold for each individual could be integrated in the calibration process in future eye blinking monitoring systems to facilitate the setup process for a user.
The eye blinking rates in this study were noticeably higher than reported values in previous studies. The observed eye blinking rates of this study in a spontaneous (conversational) setting was 43.1 ± 14.7 blinks/min, which decreased to 25.2 ± 11.9 blink/min while watching movie trailers and 24.2 ± 12.1 blinks/min when reading articles at the primary gaze level. These values are marginally higher than the reported eye blinking rates in previous studies, which were 10.5 – 32.5 blinks/min during conversations, 12.5 – 20.0 blinks/min while watching movie trailers and 8.0 – 21.0 blinks/min in primary gaze positions (
Cho et al., 2000;
Dementyev & Holz, 2017;
Doughty, 2001). There are multiple possible reasons for the increased level of blinking rates in this study. It was noticed that the blinking monitor would count blinks continuously when the subject’s gaze position is much below the primary gaze level. Thus, a stand for the computer screen was added to the setup (
Fig. 2b) that allowed the subjects to comfortably view the screen at their primary gaze level. It was also noticed in some occasions that the blinking monitor would count false blinks when the subject is laughing especially during the conversation task of the study. Differentiation between actual blinks and false blink counts as a result of facial expressions needs to be considered when developing an accurate blinking monitoring system. Another possible reason is the nervousness of the subjects during the study experiments. Studies have shown that nervous tension may lead to increased rates of blinking (
Ponder & Kennedy, 1927). Inserting an adaptation period in future blinking monitor evaluation studies may help in capturing blinking rates within the expected ranges reported in previous studies (
Cho et al., 2000;
Doughty, 2001;
Freudenthaler et al., 2003).
The study participants that completed the System Usability Scale (SUS) questionnaire graded the usability of the developed blinking monitor system at a score of 74.6 out of 100, which is considered generally good (B grade) according to the Sauro-Lewis curved grading scale for general interpretation of SUS scores (
Lewis, 2018;
Sauro & Lewis, 2016). The SUS score is more meaningful when compared to other eye blinking monitors but none have been reported so far. A major aspect of the usability and acceptance of a blinking monitor is the alarm system. The developed eye blinking monitoring system in this study used three different alarm modes to induce blinks: audible sound, visual light and tactile vibration. Most of the subjects found the system alerts not very disturbing. The item “I thought the sound, light and vibration alerts of the blinking monitor were very disturbing” was rated 1.7 out of 5 in the SUS questionnaire. Although all of these alarm modes were used simultaneously in this study to increase the impact an alert, the overall increase in blinking rate with the alarm system compared to without the alarm was only 6.9% with no statistical significance and an effect size (
d) of 0.14, which is considered a small effect size (
Sullivan & Feinn, 2012). A more tailored approach based on the user’s preference is likely to be more effective and sustainable for inducing blinks. In previous studies, physical tapping near the eye was found to be an effective tactile alarm that resulted in a 35.8% increase in the participants’ mean blinking rate (
Dementyev & Holz, 2017). Double blink animations on computer screens increased blinking rates by 91.8% (
Nosch et al., 2015). Slowly blurring a screen until the user blinked increased blinking rates by over 100% (
Crnovrsanin et al., 2014). The low increase in blinking rate observed in this study is likely due to the misdetection of blinks. In addition to the misdetection of rapid consecutive blinks due to the 0.5 sec standby period after each blink, it was noticed in later stages of this study that blinks were not being detected during the momentary alarm trigger period (~1-2 sec). In previous studies, participants blinked in response to 56.2% of the physical taps within the first 2 sec (
Dementyev & Holz, 2017) and the average blinking response to a blurring screen was 1.7 sec (
Crnovrsanin et al., 2014). Therefore, this issue in the programming code has likely severely diminished the effect of the alarms in this study and must be addressed with high priority in future studies. Furthermore, no instructions were provided to the participants in this study on the appropriate response to an alarm. Instructing users to blink twice upon receiving an alarm may be an effective method to increase blinking rates with a lower number of alarm messages (
Nosch et al., 2015) and hence, increase the acceptance of a blinking monitor designed for daily use. More studies are needed to investigate tailored alarms and instructions to determine the most effective method for inducing blinks with minimal distraction.
There are some limitations to this study. The misdetection of rapid consecutive blinks and blinks during the alarm trigger period that were explained earlier are considerable limitations to this study and must be addressed. Although the subjects were requested to remove their eye glasses for the consistency of the evaluation study, the eye blinking monitoring system can detect eye blinks for subjects wearing eye glasses with reasonable accuracy (77.8%, n=1). More trials and modifications on the programming code are required to improve the accuracy of blink detection using cameras for users with eye glasses. Along with decreased eye blinking rates, higher percentages of incomplete eye blinks during computer use is considered another major possible contributor to the prevalence of computer vision syndrome (
Himebaugh et al., 2009;
Portello et al., 2013;
Talens-Estarelles et al., 2022). An exploration study showed that symptoms of computer vision syndrome for computer users was directly associated with blinking rate (
Lapa et al., 2023). The results of this study reported that for each added blink to the average blinking rate, the computer vision syndrome score decreased by 1.3. This suggests that even a small increase in blinking rate can have a positive impact on alleviating symptoms of computer vision syndrome. However, another study showed that the score of computer vision syndrome symptoms was associated with incomplete blinks, while an increase in blinking rate did not produce a significant change in the symptoms score (
Portello et al., 2013). This conflict of results requests the detection of blinking rate and incomplete blinks in blinking monitor studies. Unfortunately, incomplete blinks were not assessed in this study due to limitations in developing the programming code but should be considered in future blinking monitor studies. Furthermore, obtaining clinical measurements, such as tear film parameters (e.g. tear meniscus height, tear break-up time, tear osmolarity and tear volume), pupil size to quantify visual fatigue and questionnaires to evaluate dry eye symptoms and computer vision syndrome symptoms are necessary to clinically evaluate blinking monitors and must be considered in future studies (
Kim & Lee, 2020;
Talens-Estarelles et al., 2020). In the design of this study, training and fatigue are factors that may have had an effect on blinking rates. However, the training and calibration process (tasks 1 and 2) were reduced to the minimum possible time of around 10 min to mitigate potential fatigue at the end of the experiment. Also, after completing the tasks of watching movie trailers (tasks 3 and 4), the subjects were requested to take a break for at least 5 min away from the computer to reenergize themselves before proceeding to the tasks of reading articles (tasks 5 and 6) and completing the experiment. Although the movie trailers and reading articles selected in this study were randomized between the subjects to minimize the effect of confounding variables, the fixed order of the tasks (watching movie trailers and reading articles with and without the alarm of the eye blinking monitoring system) is a possible confounding variable. The order of these tasks should be randomized to reduce the effects of any potential confounding variables and must be considered in future blinking monitor studies. Also, some users may find difficulties with the current setup process for the developed eye blinking monitoring system, which was performed by the experimenters in this study. The setup process includes appropriate positioning of the blinking monitoring system, specifying the optimal Eye Ratio for each user, and setting the personalized alarm threshold for each individual based on their spontaneous eye blinking rate. More investigations are needed to determine the optimal setup process that can be performed by a user to operate an eye blinking monitoring system on a daily basis.
Overall, an intelligent, standalone eye blinking monitoring system for computer users was developed in this study. The experimental procedures showed that the developed blinking monitoring system was able to detect blinking with high accuracy and induce blinking with a personalized alarm function. The developed system is completely non-obtrusive for the user and independent of the user’s computer. The components of the developed system are affordable and easily available in the market. Further work is needed to avoid blink misdetections in specific conditions, assess incomplete blinks, refine the study design, evaluate clinical parameters and investigate effective alarm triggers for inducing blinks. This work provides a technological advancement for preventing eye and vision complications to the highly prevalent and drastically increasing number of computer users around the world.