Introduction
Environmental protection has gained increasing attention worldwide, particularly in the context of sustainable development. As a crucial means of addressing global issues such as climate change and resource depletion, environmental protection not only relies on professionals in relevant fields but also requires enhancing public awareness of sustainability (
Venghaus et al., 2022). In this process, visual communication, especially visual elements in eco-friendly products, has gradually become a key method for conveying sustainability information (
Majer et al., 2022). In recent years, with the rapid development of eco-friendly product design, researchers have begun to explore how visual elements affect people's understanding and memory of sustainability concepts (
Ji & Lin, 2022). Eye-tracking technology, as an effective tool for studying visual attention, can reveal how individuals distribute their attention to sustainability graphics when viewing eco-friendly products, providing important insights into the effectiveness of visual communication (
Spakov et al., 2017;
Tsotsos et al., 2016;
Van Loo et al., 2021).
Studies have shown that the distribution of visual attention and the recall of information are closely related to an individual's background knowledge and cognitive level. An individual's professional background and cognitive preferences play a critical role in visual information processing (
Carrasco, 2011). In the field of sustainability, this background difference may be even more pronounced, as education in environmental studies can influence an individual's sensitivity to sustainability information and cognitive levels (
Sheppard, 2015). However, there is still a limited amount of research on how individuals with different professional backgrounds and levels of sustainability awareness distribute their visual attention and recall sustainability information when viewing ecofriendly products. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the distribution of visual attention and the recall of sustainability information among individuals with varying professional backgrounds and sustainability levels through eye-tracking experiments.
Water waste disposal
Water is an essential resource for human survival, playing a crucial role in human health and sustainable development. It is imperative to implement necessary measures to protect water resources and raise awareness about the sustainable development of water resources (
Kılıç, 2020). Waterborne waste has become a significant sustainability issue. Over 2 million tons of plastic are dumped into water bodies each year, ultimately ending up in the oceans (
Othman et al., 2020). Additionally, water contains various types of natural or man-made waste such as food scraps, metals, clothing, and glass. With technological advancements, the methods for managing waterborne waste have evolved beyond manual labor to include specialized products. Low-cost, highly corrosion-resistant, and durable automated waterborne waste management products are now available and effectively perform waste collection and processing tasks (
Mohd Shamsuddin et al., 2020). The design of these waterborne waste management products can reflect sustainable principles. For example, the design of a waterborne waste management robot uses renewable energy to reduce energy consumption and incorporates sensors and computer vision technology to enhance cleaning efficiency and enable autonomous operation (
Phirke et al., 2021). Although these designs are effective, the waste management process and the underlying sustainability aspects are not easily understood by nonspecialists. The appearance and design specifications of waterborne waste management products, such as trash boats, often do not intuitively convey sustainability. This lack of intuitive understanding can hinder citizens' comprehension of waterborne waste management practices and the formation of concepts related to the sustainable development of water resources.
Sustainability graphics in eco-friendly products
With the worsening of global environmental issues and the promotion of sustainable development concepts, the design of eco-friendly products has become a significant trend in contemporary design (
Cenci et al., 2022). In eco-friendly products, the design of visual elements not only concerns aesthetics but also carries the mission of conveying sustainability concepts (
Ji & Lin, 2022). Graphics are an effective way to communicate ideas, methods, and styles (
Bowers, 2012). Incorporating graphic design into industrial product design can more effectively express the designer's intentions (
He, 2017). Integrating sustainability elements into graphics and product design is an important approach to promote sustainable development (
Alahira et al., 2024). Industrial designers have both the ability and responsibility to raise public awareness of sustainability by not only enhancing the functional sustainability of products but also highlighting sustainability visually to enhance public sustainability education. By using specific visual symbols, colors, and patterns, designers can communicate environmental messages and influence the public's cognition and attitudes. These visual elements help users cultivate environmental awareness in their daily lives, thereby promoting sustainable behaviors (
Chiu et al., 2023).
In various fields, sustainability graphics have been proven to play a role in promoting sustainable development. Eco-graphic labels have played an important role in consumers’ food choices (
Grebmer & Diefenbach, 2022). The use of sustainability graphics in product packaging not only influences consumers' perceptions and inferences about the product but also affects their sustainability attitudes (
Steenis et al., 2017). One study further demonstrated that not only the material type of the packaging affects consumers but also the graphic materials, especially for consumers with lower environmental awareness (
Grebmer & Diefenbach, 2022). Additionally, incorporating sustainability elements into electronic graphic interfaces can guide users toward environmentally friendly actions (
Müller, 2022). Symbolic visual elements, such as green and plant images, are often associated by consumers with environmental protection and sustainability (
Pancer et al., 2017). For example, green labels and eco-symbols not only significantly increase consumers’ awareness of the environmental attributes of products but also enhance consumers' trust and loyalty toward the brand (
Taufique et al., 2014).
However, the public's response to these visual elements is not uniform, but rather influenced by their professional background, environmental awareness, and cognitive level. Individuals with an environmental studies background are more likely to identify sustainability visual elements in products and are more receptive to this information compared to the general public (
Sheppard, 2015). Furthermore, individuals' sustainability levels can also influence their consumption behaviors and environmental actions (
Silva & Semprebon, 2021). Therefore, studying the differences in how individuals with different professional backgrounds and sustainability levels respond to visual elements in eco-friendly products can help reveal the effectiveness of these visual elements in conveying sustainability concepts.
Eye-tracking study
Eye-tracking technology is an effective method for exploring graphics and is frequently used in visual and product design (
Li et al., 2023;
Mao, 2024). Eye-tracking technology can record participants' eye movements, allowing researchers to infer visual attention and attention transaction patterns (
Mathot et al., 2012;
Takemoto et al., 2022;
Walker & McSorley, 2008). For example, a study analyzing eye movements on warning labels on alcoholic beverage packaging found that the position, size, and color of the labels significantly influenced consumer visual attention (
Cholewa-Wójcik et al., 2024)). Fixation is a common eye movement in eye-tracking research, referring to the behavior of the foveal vision staying in a specific area for more than a certain amount of time (
Krauzlis et al., 2017). Although the human field of view is broad, only the positions of fixations are the actual targets of individual attention(
Jainta & Jaschinski, 2010). Eye-tracking metrics related to fixation include fixation duration, fixation count, and time to first fixation. Fixation duration and fixation count are generally interpreted as measures of attention, while time to first fixation reflects the ability of a target to quickly attract attention (
Jian & Ko, 2017). In eye-tracking research, an area that contains key experimental objects can be defined on the material, and this area is referred to as the Area of Interest (AOI). Its primary function is to mark the areas that need to be analyzed in the study, allowing for the analysis of eye-tracking metrics within single AOI and across multiple AOIs (
Atik & Arslan, 2019;
K. Krejtz et al., 2016;
Mao, Li, et al., 2024).
In sustainability research, eye-tracking technology has also been extensively used. One study focused on consumers' eye movements regarding in-store recycling signage, collecting data on fixation duration, fixation count, and time to first fixation(
Zhao et al., 2024). The results indicate that the total fixation duration (1.72 s) and fixation count (5.91 counts) for shelf strips performed better than those for aisle invaders and store drop-off bin panels. However, for time to first fixation (0.12 s), store drop-off bin panels showed the best performance. This indicates that there are differences in the appeal of sustainability visual elements to users. In a study on meat sustainability, researchers analyzed consumers' eye movements and found that the Free-Range logo received the highest attention, as reflected in the average fixation count (4.03 counts), average fixation duration (0.89 s), and average visit duration (0.95 s) (
Garcez de Oliveira Padilha et al., 2021). Through further interviews, researchers found that consumers often make incorrect inferences about sustainability labels, frequently associating higher prices with greater sustainability. This highlights the crucial role of graphics in conveying sustainability concepts, as well as the shortcomings in the design of graphics within related products. In the field of sustainable heritage, eye-tracking demonstrated that the diverse cultural backgrounds of monuments did not lead to significant differences in visual responses among observers from different ethnicities (
Rusnak et al., 2024). A study utilizing eye-tracking technology in the sustainability education of industrial design students revealed that students employed different strategies and performances when reading sustainability articles (
Mao, Han, et al., 2024). Specifically, students with a higher level of sustainability scored higher on perspective scores, but there was no difference in recall scores. They also had a longer fixation duration on images compared to the lower group. Furthermore, the fixation transition patterns differed between theoretical articles and case articles; in case articles, students showed more transitions between fixations on images. Eye-tracking technology has been validated for its reliability in sustainability research and has shown great potential in understanding visual cognitive behaviors and patterns, addressing the limitations of subjective data in this field (
Majer et al., 2022;
Steenis et al., 2017;
Zhao et al., 2024). These studies provide new insights into sustainability research.
The present study
Although many studies have explored the importance of visual elements in product design and their influence on user behavior (
Ji & Lin, 2022;
Wang & Chen, 2022), research on the impact of sustainability visual elements in eco-friendly products on individuals with different professional backgrounds and levels of sustainability awareness remains limited. Most eye-tracking studies in the sustainability domain have focused on areas such as food packaging and electronic graphic interfaces, with relatively little attention given to how sustainability visual elements in eco-friendly products affect consumers' visual attention distribution and information recall. Moreover, while some studies have examined the relationship between consumers' environmental awareness and their attitudes toward eco-friendly products (
Chen & Chang, 2013;
Silva & Semprebon, 2021), there is still a lack of systematic research into how individuals from environmental and non-environmental backgrounds differ in their visual responses to sustainability visual elements in eco-friendly products. These differences not only affect the distribution of visual attention but may also influence consumers' memory and information processing.
This study aims to fill the aforementioned research gap by exploring the differences in visual attention distribution and information recall among individuals with different professional backgrounds and sustainability levels when confronted with sustainability visual elements in eco-friendly products. Given the lack of research on the application of sustainability graphics in waterborne waste management products, we designed an eco-trash boat and applied sustainability graphics to both the boat design and the accompanying design description. Through the use of eye-tracking technology, this study will provide empirical data to reveal the differences in attention focus and information processing between environmental and non-environmental students regarding sustainability visual elements.
This study will help address the current research gap on the role of visual elements in ecofriendly product design, offering empirical evidence for future research. The results of this study will provide crucial design recommendations for eco-friendly product designers, helping them understand how to optimize visual elements based on the professional backgrounds and sustainability awareness levels of different user groups. By creating more appealing and memorable sustainability graphics, designers can more effectively communicate environmental messages. In the context of global sustainable development, the promotion of eco-friendly products has become an important task. This study, by revealing the differences in users' cognition and recall of sustainability visual elements, will provide valuable insights for raising public environmental awareness and promoting green consumer behavior. This, in turn, will help foster broader environmental consciousness and contribute to the achievement of sustainable development goals.
This study proposes three research questions (RQs):
RQ1: Do major and sustainability level have main effects and interaction effects on eye movement (fixation count and time to first fixation)? We expect differences in eye movement characteristics depending on professional background and sustainability level. Specifically, we hypothesized that environmental students and students with higher sustainability levels will have a higher fixation count on sustainability graphics and will detect sustainability graphics more quickly.
RQ2: Do major and sustainability level have main effects and interaction effects on the recall of sustainability elements (sustainability graphic and sustainability feature)? We expect differences in the recall of sustainability elements depending on professional background and sustainability level. Specifically, we hypothesized that students with higher sustainability levels will have an advantage in recalling sustainability graphics, while environmental students will have an advantage in recalling sustainability features.
RQ3: What aspects did participants mention when recalling the sustainability graphics? We hypothesized that participants will more frequently mention nature-related and design-related attributes of the sustainability graphics.
Results
The mean sustainability score for participants was 69.0 (SD = 14.62). Based on the sustainability scores, the top 25 participants, classified as the higher group, had an average score of 81.2 (SD = 5.21), with 16 environmental majors and 9 non-environmental majors. The bottom 25 participants, classified as the lower group, had an average score of 56.8 (SD = 9.93), with 9 environmental majors and 16 non-environmental majors. There was a significant difference in sustainability scores between the higher and lower groups, t (48) = -10.895, p < .001.
To address RQ1, eye movement metrics, including fixation count and time to first fixation, were analyzed for participants with different majors and sustainability levels (
Table 2).
In the fixation count results, the main effect of major was not significant,
F (1, 46) = 0.371,
p = .546,
η² = .008. The main effect of sustainability level was significant, with the fixation count in the higher group being significantly greater than that in the lower group,
F (1, 46) = 26.220,
p < .001,
η² = .363. The interaction effect between major and sustainability level was not significant,
F (1, 46) = .251,
p = .619,
η² = .005 (
Figure 4).
In the results for time to first fixation, the main effect of major was significant, with non-environmental participants having a significantly shorter time to first fixation than environmental participants,
F (1, 46) = 76.051,
p < .001,
η² = .623. The main effect of sustainability level was also significant, with the lower group having a significantly shorter time to first fixation than the higher group,
F (1, 46) = 12.210,
p < .001,
η² = .210. The interaction effect between major and sustainability level was significant,
F (1, 46) = 252.624,
p < .001,
η² = 0.846 (
Figure 5).
Simple effects results show that in the lower group, non-environmental participants had a significantly longer time to first fixation than environmental participants, F (1, 46) = 25.729, p < .001, η² = 0.359. In the higher group, non-environmental participants had a significantly shorter time to first fixation compared to environmental participants, F (1, 46) = 302.945, p < .001, η² = .2868. Among non-environmental students, the lower group had a significantly longer time to first fixation than the higher group, F (1, 46) = 76.879, p < .001, η² = .626. Conversely, among environmental students, the lower group had a significantly shorter time to first fixation than the higher group, F (1, 46) = 187.9549, p < .001, η² = .803.
To address RQ2, the scores for recall of sustainability elements, including sustainability graphic and sustainability feature, were analyzed for participants with different majors and sustainability levels (
Table 3).
In the results for sustainability graphic, the main effect of major was not significant,
F (1,46) = 2.706,
p = .107,
η² = .056. The main effect of sustainability level was significant, with the higher group recalling significantly more sustainability graphics than the lower group,
F (1,46) = 55.863,
p < .001,
η² = .548. The interaction effect between major and sustainability level was not significant,
F (1,46) = .584,
p = .449,
η² = .013 (
Figure 6).
In the results for sustainability feature, the main effect of major was significant, with non-environmental participants recalling significantly fewer sustainability features compared to environmental participants,
F (1,46) = 61.410,
p < .001,
η² = .572. The main effect of sustainability level was not significant,
F (1,46) = 1.480,
p = .230,
η² = .031. The interaction effect between major and sustainability level was not significant,
F (1,46) = .003,
p = .955,
η² < 0.001 (
Figure 7).
To address RQ3, the coding of sustainability graphics was organized and categorized into 3 main categories: nature, design, and concept, and further divided into 9 sub-categories (
Figure 8). Nature was mentioned by all participants. The most frequently mentioned sub-category was green, followed by plant, vivid, and eco. For example, one participant mentioned, “I saw a green symbol with two arrows indicating recycling, and a green leaf in the center.” This statement referenced green, plant**, and cycling. Another participant said, “The green image on the boat was particularly striking. It was very vividly designed, allowing people to intuitively feel that the boat is something related to environmental protection.” Here, the coding includes green, vivid, and eco. Some participants specifically commented on the design of the graphic, such as, “There’s an icon on the lower side of the boat showing a green leaf with recycling arrows. However, the graphic’s size is a bit small, which might make it hard for people to see from a distance.” This statement corresponds to green, plant, cycling, and also location and size in terms of design. Style and process were also mentioned, for instance, “The icon is very simple, like elements seen on the packaging of some eco-friendly products,” and “These two arrows make me understand that the waste on the water can be processed into something useful.”
Discussion
This study aims to explore the eye movements of students with different majors and sustainability levels when viewing sustainability graphics in eco-friendly products, as well as their recall of sustainability elements. This study begins with the product design of water-based sustainability products, focusing on functional and visual optimization of the eco-trash boat, and further discusses the design’s impact on public awareness of sustainability. The study employed a mixed-method approach, collecting both quantitative eye-movement data and qualitative analysis of participants' verbal reports. In the sustainability grouping of participants, the higher group had a larger proportion of students majoring in environmental fields, which was expected. Environmental students receive more education related to sustainability and tend to have a higher awareness of it (
Chuvieco et al., 2018;
Goodale, 2021). However, being an environmental major cannot be the sole criterion for determining sustainability levels, and the study results indicate that major and sustainability level have different effects on some measures and may interact. Overall, sustainability graphics have an impact on both visual attention and recall. The specific discussions are provided below.
The eye-movement data was used to address RQ1. We hypothesized that environmental students and higher sustainability students would have a higher fixation count and a shorter time to first fixation on sustainability graphics. The degree of attention to the sustainability graphic in the design's four views can be reflected by fixation count ((
Mao, Ban, et al., 2024;
Tang et al., 2016;
Zhao et al., 2024). Students with higher sustainability levels showed more visual attention to the sustainability graphic. However, there was no significant difference between environmental and non-environmental majors. This result indicates that visual elements related to sustainability are more likely to attract the attention of the higher group. This is because the higher group has a greater sensitivity to sustainability, which is reflected in their increased fixation count (
Ma & Zhuang, 2021). Sustainability graphics effectively convey awareness of environmental protection, energy conservation, and emission reduction (
Grebmer & Diefenbach, 2022;
Robertson, 2018). For individuals with already high sustainability levels, this communication is even more effective as they pay more attention to the sustainability graphics. Consumers who spend more time focusing on sustainability elements are more likely to value sustainability and engage in sustainable practices (
Van Loo et al., 2015). This result further demonstrates that the appeal of sustainability graphics is independent of whether a person is an environmental major. Therefore, the application of sustainability graphics on water-based sustainability products should be designed for the public, not just for those in environmental fields.
An important role of sustainability graphics should be their ability to help people quickly notice sustainability information. Generally, a shorter time to first fixation indicates that participants detected relevant information more quickly (
Fokin et al., 2020;
I. Krejtz et al., 2013;
Zhao et al., 2024). The results for time to first fixation are more complex than for fixation count, with both major and sustainability level influencing time to first fixation, and an interaction effect observed. Non-environmental students detected the sustainability graphic earlier than environmental students, and students with lower sustainability levels detected the sustainability graphic earlier than the higher group. This result is somewhat surprising and contradicts our pre-experiment hypothesis. Further analysis of the interaction effect revealed that, in the lower group and among non-environmental students, the results align with our hypothesis, which is higher sustainability levels and being an environmental major facilitate earlier detection of the sustainability graphic. However, in the higher group and among environmental students, the results were contrary. We speculate that this interaction effect is related to participants' knowledge base and the specialized information presented in the eco-trash boat design. The eco-trash boat’s design includes not only sustainability graphics but also aspects like materials, working principles, and operational methods, which may be unfamiliar to non-environmental students or those with lower sustainability levels. Environmental students and higher group participants, due to their higher sustainability literacy (
Goodale, 2021), might focus on more specialized sustainability information, such as materials and functionality (
Grebmer & Diefenbach, 2022), and therefore might not detect the sustainability graphic as quickly. Combining the results for fixation count, despite lower group students not having a higher number of observations, the sustainability graphic was able to capture their attention more quickly. Effective communication of sustainability is crucial, particularly in contexts with a large amount of visual information or insufficient observation time.
To address RQ 2, participants' recall of sustainability graphic and sustainability feature information was encoded and analyzed. Before the experiment, we hypothesized that higher sustainability students would achieve higher recall scores for sustainability graphics, while environmental students would score higher in recalling sustainability features. The results indicate that the higher group reported more information related to the sustainability graphic compared to students with lower sustainability levels, with no significant differences between majors. Environmental majors, however, demonstrated an advantage in the sustainability feature scores. For environmental students, their education provides them with a better knowledge base related to sustainability (
Chuvieco et al., 2018;
Goodale, 2021). Thus, when reading the design description, they can easily understand sustainability principles, functions, and implementation methods, leading to higher scores in sustainability features. This professional advantage is not influenced by the level of sustainability awareness. In contrast, the results reaffirm that being an environmental major is not a key factor for sustainability graphic recall. Sustainability awareness not only affects attention to sustainability elements but also impacts understanding and recall of these elements (
Majer et al., 2022). Individuals with higher sustainability awareness are more proactive in seeking sustainability-related information (
Mao, Han, et al., 2024), allowing them to recall more content related to sustainability graphics. This result aligns with the fixation count findings, further supporting that the higher group allocated more attention to sustainability graphic-related information and demonstrated better understanding and recall of it.
The categorization results of the coding for sustainability graphic-related information in all participants' oral reports address RQ 3. We aim to interpret which aspects participants focused on by coding their recall of sustainability graphics. The use of green plant imagery in sustainability graphics made a strong impression on participants and was the most frequently mentioned. Visual elements related to nature are helpful for expressing and communicating sustainability (
El-Shafey et al., 2024), as demonstrated again in this study. In the design of sustainability graphics, participants more frequently mentioned vividness rather than specific design specifications. This reflects the role of visualization in enhancing impressions in visual communication design (
Fish, 2021). Vivid visual elements are crucial in sustainability visual expression (
Birt, 2015;
Pichierri & Pino, 2023). They enable viewers to make more associations with environmental friendliness, aiding in internalizing sustainability concepts. The purpose of using sustainability graphics in products is to convey sustainability ideas or knowledge, making the reception of concepts an important effect. In this study, concepts like cycling and eco were mentioned by nearly half of the participants in relation to sustainability graphics, supporting the effectiveness of graphics in information transmission (
Wang & Chen, 2022). The concepts conveyed through sustainability graphics contribute to sustainable development (
Van Loo et al., 2015). For individuals, especially those without an environmental background, there are few opportunities for formal sustainability education. Therefore, incorporating sustainability graphics into products is a significant educational approach. This type of education has low learning costs and can be seamlessly integrated into daily life.
In summary, sustainability graphics on the eco-friendly product can capture people's attention and influence their reception of sustainability information. This impact is significant for both higher and lower sustainability individuals. People with higher sustainability levels tend to engage with sustainability graphics more frequently, while those with lower sustainability levels are attracted to these graphics more quickly. The effectiveness of sustainability graphics is independent of the participant's major, indicating that these graphics are intended for the general public, not just those in environmental fields. Although non-environmental majors may lack specialized knowledge in sustainability, the concepts embedded in sustainability graphics can be effectively conveyed through visual observation. Furthermore, cultivating students' sustainability awareness is crucial (
Mao, Han, et al., 2024), and educators should pay attention to this aspect. In designing sustainability graphics, it is recommended to use natural elements and adopt more concrete design approaches. Sustainability graphics are essential for conveying concepts and serve as a crucial method for designers to communicate sustainability information, making it an important focus in design.
This study has certain limitations. Firstly, the stimuli used in the research, the eco-trash boat designed by the researchers, may not represent all types of water-based sustainable products, and the sample size is limited (although the set sample size was achieved, the values for effect size, error probability, and power were relatively lenient.), which restricts the generalizability of the findings. Secondly, while sustainability graphics were used in this study, there was no control group for comparative analysis. Third, regarding eye-tracking data, we only collected data on participants' eye movements while viewing the diagrams, but not while reading the design description. This additional, complex data could have been helpful in interpreting the participants' recall results. Lastly, the qualitative data was solely derived from participants' verbal reports, lacking deeper exploration. In future research, we plan to validate our findings across a broader range of eco-friendly products and apply sustainability graphics in various ways, including adding control groups to further explore the effects of sustainability graphics. In the collection and analysis of eye-tracking data, adopt more comprehensive and appropriate metrics, and include eye-tracking data from participants' reading processes. Additionally, we will conduct targeted interviews with participants to uncover more effective information, investigate the reasons behind eye-movement behavior and sustainability awareness, and gather related suggestions and expectations. We will also place greater emphasis on the correlation between eye-tracking data and qualitative data, which will help in better understanding user behavior.