Next Article in Journal
Eye Tracking Applied to Tobacco Smoking: Current Directions and Future Perspectives
Previous Article in Journal
Visual Scanpath Training to Emotional Faces Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: A Single Case Design
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Word Length and Word Frequency Among Dyslexic, Adhd-I and Typical Readers

by
Norberto Pereira
1,2,3,
Maria Armanda Costa
2 and
Manuela Guerreiro
4
1
NeuroCog—Centro de Reabilitação da Lesão Cerebral, 2625-387 Forte da Casa, Portugal
2
Center of Linguistics, School of Arts and Humanities, University of Lisbon, 1600-214 Lisboa, Portugal
3
Escola Superior de Saúde Dr. Lopes Dias (ESALD), 6000-767 Castelo Branco, Portugal
4
Instituto Medicina Molecular, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, 1649-028 Lisboa, Portugal
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2022, 15(1), 1-27; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.1.1
Submission received: 28 August 2021 / Published: 14 June 2022

Abstract

:
This study aimed to investigate the neuropsycholinguistic functioning of children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder – inattentive subtype (ADHD-I) in a reading task. The psycholinguistic profile of both groups was assessed using a battery of neuropsychological and linguistic tests and compared to typical readers. Participants were submitted to a silent reading task with lexical manipulation of the text. Eye movements were recorded and compared aiming to find cognitive processes involved in reading that could help differentiate groups. The study examined whether word-frequency and word-length effects distinguish between groups. Participants included 19 typical readers, 21 children diagnosed with ADHD-I and 19 children with DD. All participants were attending 4th grade and had a mean age of 9.08 years. Children with DD and ADHDI exhibited significant different cognitive and linguistic profiles on almost all measures evaluated when compared to typical readers. The effects of word length and word frequency interaction also differed significantly in the 3 experimental groups. The results support the multiple cognitive deficits theory. While the shared deficits support the evidence of a phonological disorder present in both conditions, the specific ones corroborate the hypothesis of an oculomotor dysfunction in DD and a visuo-spatial attention dysfunction in ADHD.

Introduction

Dyslexia

Developmental Dyslexia (DD) is a neurodevelopmental reading disability that adversely affects the speed and accuracy of word recognition, phonological processing, and as a consequence, impairs reading fluency and text comprehension, despite adequate instruction, and in the absence of general cognitive or sensory deficits (Benfatto et al., 2016; Berninger, 2001; Lyon et al., 2003; Peterson & Pennington, 2012, 2015). Dyslexics also exhibit distortions, substitutions, and omissions when reading aloud and silently (American Psychiatric Association, 2002). It is commonly estimated to affect between 5 to 10% of the population. Since reading ability is a skill that falls along a continuum, dyslexia is considered a difficulty along this continuum with no clear-cut or absolute limit. Thus, it is not possible to specify exactly how common dyslexia is, other than in relation to an approximate reader profile of what can be considered typical reading ability (Benfatto et al., 2016). Despite this uncertainty, there is good evidence for its neurobiological basis (Rimrodt et al., 2009; B. A. Shaywitz et al., 2006), which reflects the fact that dyslexia occurs in varying degrees of severity (Peterson & Pennington, 2012; S. E. Shaywitz et al., 1992).
Although the causes of dyslexia are still not fully understood, and definitions and terminology vary, it is generally agreed that children who fail to acquire reading skill at a typical rate need careful monitoring and support during the early years of school (Benfatto et al., 2016).
Early identification and professional support is the most effective form of intervention for children with pronounced reading difficulties (Peterson & Pennington, 2012; Vaughn et al., 2010). Linguistic parameters such as text complexity (Rayner & Duffy, 1986; Trauzettel-Klosinski et al., 2010), its syntax (von der Malsburg et al., 2015; von der Malsburg & Vasishth, 2013), word length and word frequency (Reichle et al., 1998; Tiffin-Richards & Schroeder, 2015) can influence eye movements. Consequently, intervention strategies should account for the importance of perceptual parameters such as the properties of fonts (e.g., spacing), which are another aspect that has been shown to affect reading performance (Dotan & Katzir, 2018; Hakvoort et al., 2017; Hermena, 2021; Rayner et al., 2010; Sjoblom et al., 2016; Zorzi et al., 2012).
To lessen the reading burden of dyslexic readers, designers have created dyslexia-friendly fonts such as OpenDyslexic69 and Dyslexie70. Nevertheless, these manipulations – whose fonts omit serifs, increase inter and intraword spacing, and have unique letter strokes (Franzen et al., 2021) – have not been found to increase reading speed (Kuster et al., 2018; Marinus et al., 2016; Wery & Diliberto, 2017).
Although most individuals are diagnosed during early school grades, the diagnosis can be made at any age (Bazen et al., 2020). Fast, systematic and automated screening methods based on objective measurements of reading may help identify individuals at risk of dyslexia during the early school years (Benfatto et al., 2016; Pereira et al., 2016). Current methods, however, are limited in that they only measure individual cognitive skills that natural reading depends upon but say little about their interplay and function in actual reading (Benfatto et al., 2016). Invariably, these tests require the subject to produce some explicit response, typically under time pressure, such as marking the word boundaries in sequences of words without inter-word spaces, matching target words to corresponding pictures, or reading aloud pronounceable pseudowords of increasing difficulty. The proportion of correct responses gives an estimate of performance on a task related to reading but does not reflect the actual process of reading as it naturally occurs (Benfatto et al., 2016).
To overcome this limitation, we investigated the use of eye tracking during reading as a method for identifying and comparing different reading patterns among children with Dyslexia, ADHD–I and typical readers. By tracking eye movements during reading, it is possible to follow the reading process as it occurs in real-time and obtain objective measurements of this process. The data collected with this technique can provide a continuous record of reading that reflects both the speed and accuracy of the processes involved on text and word-based reading measures (Hyönä & Olson, 1995; Just & Carpenter, 1980; Pollatsek et al., 2003; Rayner, 1998; Rayner et al., 1981), in which reading accuracy can be determined from specific eye tracking data, such as regressions and go-past time.
Importantly, this type of measurement requires no overt response extraneous to the reading process itself and thus makes it possible to assess reading performance without placing additional task demands on the subject. As such, this approach differs in important ways from the screening methods currently in use.
Although it has long been known that the eye movements of children with dyslexia are different from those of typical readers, previous research has focused almost exclusively on identifying group-level differences (Olson et al., 1991; G. Pavlidis, 1980; Rubino & Minden, 1973).

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

ADHD is one of the most commonly diagnosed disorders in children with prevalence rates in the general population ranging from 3-7% (American Psychiatric Association, 2002). Assessing a child for ADHD can be difficult given the subjectivity of currently utilized assessment measures and the high degree of comorbidity between ADHD and other disorders (Deans et al., 2010). According to Willcutt et al. (2005), ADHD and DD are two of the most common disorders of childhood, each occurring in approximately 5% of the population. Furthermore, ADHD and DD frequently co-occur, with a comorbidity typically ranging from 25 to 40% (Faraone et al., 1998; Rommelse et al., 2009; Tamm et al., 2017; Willcutt et al., 2005, 2011); boys with dyslexia have higher rates of comorbid externalizing disorders, including ADHD (Willcutt & Pennington, 2000).
Students with ADHD often fall behind academically because of their attention problems. As a result of their poor academics, children with ADHD may appear to have a learning disability, such as a Reading Disability (RD). Additional deficits that children with ADHD may display in the school setting include poor rote memory, excessive vocalizations, difficulty delaying gratification, distractibility by extraneous stimuli, and difficulty listening and maintaining a conversation (Barkley, 1999). These deficits, both individually and in combination, can make learning in the school setting very difficult (Deans et al., 2010).
The term ADHD-I will be used henceforth to refer only to that subgroup of this population whose topmost problem is inattention alone (predominantly inattentive type), which is the one that comprises our study. This subtype does not reflect a developmental deficiency in behavioural inhibition but probably one of focused/selective attention and speed of information processing (Barkley, 1997; Barkley et al., 1992).

Dyslexia Vs. ADHD

The vast majority of the research clearly demonstrates that dyslexia and ADHD–I represent two distinct clinical syndromes with separate cognitive profiles (Willcutt et al., 2001, 2005, 2010). Children with dyslexia exhibit deficits in phonological processing and other reading related skills while children with ADHD exhibit difficulties in executive functioning (Pennington et al., 1993). Moreover, although not considered a primary deficit, difficulties in reading and listening comprehension have been associated with ADHD and likely contribute to their academic struggles (Flake et al., 2007; Flory et al., 2006). Miller et al. (2013) suggested that even when word reading ability was controlled, children with ADHD had difficulty building a coherent mental representation, and that difficulty was likely related to deficits in working memory.
These unique and distinctive deficits provide support for the validity of each diagnosis (Deans et al., 2010) and highlight the importance of assessing reading performance in children with ADHD.

Eye Movements in Children with Dyslexia

Previous research has demonstrated that children with dyslexia exhibit different patterns of eye movements on reading tasks as compared to typical readers (Deans et al., 2010). While typical readers can read about 250 words per minute, the reading speed of children with dyslexia tends to be much slower because they make longer fixations, more frequent fixations, shorter saccades, and more regressions than typical readers (Deans et al., 2010). Longer fixations often occur because it takes them more time to comprehend information from the text. Children with dyslexia also have shorter saccades because they cannot cover as much information in their perceptual span (Adler-Grinberg & Stark, 1978; Rayner, 2009). Additionally, children with dyslexia tend to have unstable fixations and make more shorter saccades than typical readers (Deans et al., 2010). Dyslexics also process less parafoveal information on each fixation leading to more frequent and shorter saccades (Rayner, 1998). Overall, these eye movement patterns are correlated with slower reading speed and poorer comprehension (Garzia et al., 1990). Shorter saccades are common in letter-by-letter reading and contribute to a slow and laborious reading style, being the source of greater fixations among children with dyslexia as compared to typical readers (Hawelka & Wimmer, 2005). Hawelka and Wimmer (2005) examined fixations, saccades, reading speed, and errors in reading in atypical and typical readers and found that the first group made fewer errors than normal readers, however, their reading speed was significantly slower than typical readers. These authors found that differences in reading rate were associated with the number of eye movements – fixations and saccades – made during reading. That is, participants with more eye movements had slower reading speeds (Hawelka & Wimmer, 2005).

Eye Movements in Children with ADHD

Children with ADHD, analogous to children with dyslexia, may also have unique eye movement patterns, particularly regarding visual tracking tasks that require response inhibition of automatic saccadic eye movements (Munoz et al., 2003). Munoz et al. (2003), developed a prosaccade task in which ADHD and control participants ranging in age from 6 to 59 years old were asked to look at a target stimulus when it appeared on the screen and an antisaccade task where participants were asked to inhibit looking at the target stimulus. Results indicated that participants with ADHD displayed longer reaction times, more variability, and slower saccades in the prosaccade task compared to participants in the control group. In the antisaccade task, participants with ADHD had more difficulty inhibiting automatic saccades, displayed longer reaction times, and greater variability (Munoz et al., 2003). In another study, children with ADHD – Combined subtype – and control children were compared to determine if eye movement data could be used to provide objective criteria for diagnosing ADHD (Gould et al., 2001). The eye movement task required children to remain focused on a fixation point that was stable for a period of 30 seconds and then moved back and forth on a computer screen. Results indicated that children with ADHD had greater difficulty maintaining fixations and made larger and more saccades than normal readers. There were no gender or age differences. It should also be noted that this task required visual tracking ability only and not reading skills specifically. Several studies which examined eye movements among children with ADHD, used eye movement paradigms that required tracking a visual stimulus rather than tasks that needed reading skills (Klein et al., 2003; Munoz et al., 2003).
In this study, we examined whether word-frequency and word-length effects would generalize equally to children with dyslexia, ADHD-I and typical readers. Word length and word frequency are two text characteristics which have a direct influence on eye movements of beginning readers during reading of connected text (Blythe & Joseph, 2011; Reichle et al., 2003, 2013). Long length words usually receive longer and more fixations than short words (Hyönä & Olson, 1995; Just & Carpenter, 1980; Kliegl et al., 2004) and infrequent words are fixated longer than frequent words (Inhoff & Rayner, 1986; Rayner & Duffy, 1986). Younger children show stronger length effects than older children (Huestegge et al., 2009), and dyslexic reading deficits in children also lead to stronger word length effects (Hutzler & Wimmer, 2004). Similarly, word frequency effects appear larger for children than for adults (Blythe et al., 2009; Joseph et al., 2013). There is also some evidence for stronger word length effects for infrequent than frequent words in children’s eye movements (Hyönä & Olson, 1995; Rau et al., 2014, 2015) while the evidence for adults is less consistent (Tiffin-Richards & Schroeder, 2015).
However, according to Tiffin-Richards and Schroeder (2015), only a few studies have used eye tracking methods with children in experimental designs to investigate the joint effects of word length and frequency on eye movements during reading. These authors sustain that the findings of those studies are mixed and may reflect differences in participant ages and reading ability as well as the nature of reading materials used in these studies. In addition, it is unclear whether findings can be generalized to children’s silent reading (Ashby et al., 2012; Smyrnakis et al., 2021; Spichtig et al., 2017; Vorstius et al., 2014) since, in a vast majority of studies done (Huestegge et al., 2009; Hyönä & Olson, 1995; Rau et al., 2014, 2015), participants read aloud.
To investigate this question, we present empirical evidence from a silent reading experiment which focused specifically on the interaction of word length and word frequency effects in a sample of young readers. Hence, the present study sought to examine the eye movement patterns of children diagnosed with ADHD-I and DD during a reading task. It was hypothesised that: 1) children with dyslexia would exhibit longer fixation durations, more frequent fixations and higher number of regressive saccades than children with ADHD-I and normal readers, due to different neurocognitive and linguistic profiles; 2) children in the control group would exhibit a smaller number of regressive saccades, fewer fixations, and shorter duration fixations than children in the ADHD-I and DD groups; 3) children with DD and ADHD-I would be differently affected by the lexical properties of words presented in the text as compared to typical readers and, 4) there would be statistically significant differences in ocular movement patterns between DD and ADHD-I children as compared to typical readers.

Methods

Participants

The sample consisted of 59 Portuguese children, all were 9 years old (9.08±0.68), 61% female, native speakers of European Portuguese (L1) attending 4th grade, distributed in three distinctive neuropsycholinguistic profiles, namely: 1) Control group (19 participants of whom 78.9% were female); 2) Children with dyslexia (19 participants of whom 57.9% were female) and 3) ADHD-I children (21 participants of whom 47.6% were female).
Control group inclusion criteria included: 1) Portuguese as first language; 2) a WISC-III full-scale IQ > 85; 3) absence of known neurological diseases; 4) absence of sensory (auditory or visual) or motor deficits; 5) exposure to adequate schooling; 6) medium-low minimum socioeconomical level, and 7) average or above average word reading skills assessed on a standardized test of reading fluency and accuracy. Dyslexia inclusion criteria included 1-6 criteria mentioned above plus a) experienced persistent problems in learning to read according to an independent assessment completed by the classroom teacher and, b) reading performance in the lower 5th percentile of the full cohort on a standardized test of reading fluency and accuracy. ADHD-Inattentive subtype (ADHD-I) inclusion criteria included: 1) no comorbid pervasive developmental disorder, traumatic brain injury, or other neurological conditions; and 2) a WISC-III full-scale IQ > 75. ADHD-I children medicated with methylphenidate were excluded from the study. The diagnosis of ADHD-I and dyslexia was performed according to DSM-IV-R (American Psychiatric Association, 2002) diagnostic criteria. All children were administered the WISC-III as part of a neuropsychological evaluation (Wechsler, 1991, 2003).
Neuropsychological and linguistic evaluations were carried out in Lisbon, Portugal. Eye movement recordings were collected at the Psycholinguistics Laboratory, School of Arts and Humanities, University of Lisbon.
Written informed consent was obtained from next of kin, caretakers, or guardians on behalf of the children enrolled in the study. The study protocol was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of the Faculty of Medicine in 2016, University of Lisbon.
Table 1 presents group means for age and IQ, while Table 2 shows demographic characteristic according to gender.

Materials

Eye movements were recorded with SMI IVIEW XTM HI-SPEED eye tracking system (SensoMotoric Instruments) (Test & Bubble, 2012). This video-based eye tracking compares the relative position of the pupil with the reflex coming from the cornea to calculate the ocular position at a sampling rate of 1250 Hz. This equipment was used to track eye position over time, sampling the horizontal and vertical position of the dominant eye (monocular). Under well controlled experimental conditions, the system afforded a tracking resolution of 0.01º with a gaze position accuracy of 0.25-0.5º, as per the manufacturer’s specification. Fixations were calibrated using 9-13 dots that randomly appeared in a 17-inch screen. The spatial accuracy of the equipment is 0.5º and to limit participant’s head movement a chin and forehead rest was deployed to minimize head movements and stabilize the viewing distance at 550 mm.
Word frequency in Portuguese language was determined using “Multifunctional Lexicon Computing of Contemporary Portuguese”(Bacelar do Nascimento et al., n.d.) and ESCOLEX (Soares et al., 2014) databases. For frequency, words were divided in two intervals: 1) low-frequency words (LF) - [0-1000] Token and 2) medium-frequency words (MF) - [1001-10000] Token.
Regarding word-length, the criteria related to the size of the perceptual window and word size were the following (we have adjusted the criteria used by Hyönä & Olson, 1995) to Portuguese: 1) short words (S) - [4-6] letters; 2) medium words (M) - [7-10] letters and 3) long words (L) - [11-14] letters (Table 3).

Procedure

We first determined the neuropsycholinguistic profile of each group. The neuropsychological and linguistic evaluations included instruments to assess intellectual performance (Wechsler, 2003), verbal working memory (digit span backward), short-term verbal memory (digit span forward), visual attention, phonological awareness (Sucena & Castro, 2011), non-verbal fluid intelligence (Burke, 1985; Measso et al., 1993; Raven et al., 1984, 1990; Simões, 2000, 2008), visuospatial ability and visuospatial memory (Rey, 1959), text comprehension (Cadime et al., 2012) and, reading fluency and accuracy (Carvalho, 2010).
After this phase, each group was submitted to a reading task with control of text lexical properties and eye movements were recorded. Target words were distributed throughout the text to prevent them from being placed at the end of the paragraph and close to punctuation marks, which are positions favourable to wrap-up effects and, therefore, can be confused with words themselves. Also, contiguities between target words were avoided to mitigate spill over and agglomeration effects, that could hinder eye movement analysis. To improve readability and the posterior analysis of eye movement data, we selected Courier New, a non-proportional font, size 22; double line spacing was used in the final version of the text displayed on screen.
The reading task consisted of a text taken from the 2021 National Portuguese Language Final Examination, given to 4th graders at the end of their school year. This text, entitled “120 new species discovered in Berlengas islands”, was subject to multiple changes at the level of its lexical, syntactic, and discursive properties. The objective was to reduce the level of complexity of the original text, so that it did not interfere with the lexical processing of the text. We gave preference to simple sentences and explicit correlational chains.
Our final goal was to devise a comprehensive eye movement account of reading profiles by investigating how eye movement patterns of children with dyslexia differ from ADHD-I children and, typical readers on global (text-based) and local (word-based) reading measures during an ecologically valid silent text reading task in Portuguese.
The text was divided in 3 pieces for presentation on a 17-inch screen (Figure 1: A’, B’ and C’). At the end of each slide, transition to the next slide was performed trough ocular fixation of the top right corner of the screen. The main experiment was preceded by a set of instructions and a pre-test. Monocular record of the dominant eye was recorded; ocular dominance was determined prior to the beginning of the experiment.
The pre-test training consisted of a silent reading task followed by three multiple-choice questions to determine the degree of text comprehension. The inclusion of a comprehension questionnaire at the end of each reading task served to ensure that participants identified the words, accessed their meaning, and integrated them into broader, syntactic, and discursive structures. The questions mainly served to encourage young readers to read for comprehension and to eliminate those who failed to answer 2 out of 3 questions. The comprehension outcomes were not used in any step of our analysis.
After this step, the equipment was once again calibrated according to the previously described specifications. The main reading experiment started afterwards.
Eye tracking data was collected at 1250Hz and stored offline for posterior analysis. To examine each target word as an Area of Interest (AOI), the following dependent variables were selected: Fixation Count (FC: number of fixations of all selected trials); Single Fixation Duration (SFD: the fixation duration of the fixation on a word, for AOIs in which only one fixation has been made); First Pass Reading Time (FPRT: sum of fixation durations from the first entry into an AOI until the eye leaves it in any direction), Second Pass Reading Time (SPRT: sum of fixation durations from the second entry into an AOI until the eye leaves it in any direction) and Total Fixation Time (TFT). The latter measure corresponds to the sum of FPRT and SPRT. AOIs for each target word were selected as represented in Figure 1 (A, B and C).
In data analysis, to answer the hypotheses formulated, Frequency2 x Length3 interaction effects on eye tracking variables were measured through duration and frequency of fixations that landed on the target words, as also with FPRT and SPRT.

Results

Participants eye movement behavior was determined using parametric and non-parametric statistics, by confirming normality assumption through Shapiro-Wilk normality test. Multivariate analysis was performed with Anova F statistic for equality of variances. In case of equality of variances, multiple comparisons were performed with Tukey HSD test. In the absence of equality of variances, Brown-Forsythe statistic was used as an alternative to the Anova F statistic using the post-hoc Games-Howell test. In case of normality violation assumption, Kruskal-Wallis test was used for independent samples. The assumptions for using the different statistical methods described above were as described in Marôco (2014) and Pestana & Gageiro (2014). Statistical analysis was performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics Version 25.
The results are presented in the following order: 1) Verbal Short-Term Memory; 2) Fixation Count (FC); 3) Single Fixation Duration (SFD); 4) Skipped Words; 5) First Pass Reading Time (FPRT); 6) Second Pass Reading Time (SPRT) and 7) Total Fixation Time (TFT).
No statistically significant differences between genders were found.

Neuropsychological Profile

WISC-III digit span subtest was subdivided in two additional variables to improve the search for group differences, namely digit span forward and digit span backwards; digit span subtest is included in WISC-III Verbal Working Memory Index (VWMI). Table 4 and Table 11 (see Appendix) show that typical readers differed from children with dyslexia in digit span (F (2, 49) = 8.235, p = 0.001) and in digit span forward (F (2, 49) = 5.195, p = 0.009), both measures are linked to phonetic recoding.
Regarding the overall result achieved in digit span, we found statistically significant differences among children with dyslexia (M = 8.42, SD = 2.06; n = 19) and typical readers (M = 10.62, SD = 2.43; n = 13) (p = 0.023), the first group achieving worse results. Significant differences were also identified between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (M = 7.40, SD = 2.26; n = 20) (p = 0.027), the last group displaying an identical pattern as atypical readers. As far as digit span forward, statistically significant differences were found among children with dyslexia (M = 6.47, SD = 1.31; n = 19) and typical readers (M = 7.62, SD = 1.12; n = 13) (p = 0.041), and between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (M = 6.20, SD = 1.32; n = 20) (p = 0.008). Higher scores were obtained by typical readers. Lastly, digit span backwards allowed to find statistically significant differences within group distributions of this variable ( X K W 2 (2) = 6.237; N = 52; p = 0.044). Multiple comparison analysis showed that significant differences exist among typical readers and ADHD-I children (p = 0.042), with the first group achieving better results (see Table 4).
These results confirm that children with dyslexia have deficits in short-term verbal memory (articulatory loop), specifically in verbal working memory and attention, while children with ADHD-I additionally exhibited cognitive control and executive function deficits.
As for the remaining WISC-III subtests and composite results, it was observed that compared to typical readers, children with ADHD-I showed significant weaker cognitive performances in picture completion (p = 0.012), block design (p = 0.000), coding (p = 0.019), symbol search (p = 0.003), information (p = 0.036), vocabulary (p = 0.000), picture arrangement (p = 0.041), verbal scale IQ (p = 0.000), performance scale IQ (p = 0.000), full scale IQ (p = 0.000), Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) (p = 0.000) and Perceptual Organization Index (POI) (p = 0.001) (see Appendix Table 10 and Table 11).
Finally, the items that uniquely distinguished between children with dyslexia and children with ADHD-I were WISC-III similarities (p = 0.036), symbol-search (p = 0.003), vocabulary (p = 0.000), block design (p = 0.000), picture arrangement (p = 0.006), and coding (p = 0.015) subtests. Children with ADHD–I had overall worst results in all mentioned variables and composite results (see Appendix Table 10 and Table 11).
As for Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPMP), the mean total score achieved by children on this test differed significantly between children (F = (2, 49.778) = 4.631, p = 0.014), with differences among children with ADHD-I and typical readers (p = 0.009). There were also statistically significant differences between the distribution of set B across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 10.238; N = 57; p = 0.006) especially between normative readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.005). In both cases, lower performances were observed in children with ADHD-I, which suggests that this group had several difficulties, namely in visuospatial reasoning, non-verbal abstraction capacity, visual attention, and language processing (see Appendix Table 12).
Finally, no significant differences were found between groups in Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT), which means that visual-perceptual function did not interfere with reading skills (see Appendix Table 13).
In summary, the data suggests that there were more cognitive similarities between normative readers and children with dyslexia than between typical readers and children with ADHD-I.

Linguistic Profile

Data from the reading fluency and accuracy test, which assesses decoding, identification, integration, and production skills, made the detection of deficits common to dyslexia more evident. The results show that this test was highly discriminative, enabling to distinguish typical readers from children with dyslexia (p ≤ 0.001) and ADHD-I (p ≤ 0.001). No statistically significant differences were found between atypical readers and children with ADHDI (see Appendix Table 14).
At last, reading comprehension test shows that children with ADHD-I, regardless of the type of comprehension assessed, were globally distinguished from typical readers by a worse performance in reading comprehension (F (2, 43) = 4.044, p = 0.025). This finding suggests that children with ADHD-I were more likely to make errors and respond impulsively given the nature of their attentional deficits. This overall effect at task level was not found between children with dyslexia and typical readers. Literal comprehension was the only level of comprehension that enabled to statistically distinguish between typical readers and children with dyslexia (F (2, 42) = 3.760, p = 0.031). This result suggests that children with dyslexia have low level of decoding and information integration because of difficulties in recognising the word, its phonological form and access to its meaning (see Appendix Table 15).

Fixation Count (FC)

The eye tracking independent variables that showed statistically significant differences between one or more groups were: 1) Low-Frequency Short words (S + LF); 2) Medium-Frequency Short words (S + MF); 3) Low-Frequency Medium words (M + LF); 4) Medium-Frequency Medium words (M + MF); 5) Low-Frequency Long words (L + LF) and 6) Medium-Frequency Long words (L + MF).

Low-Frequency Short words (S + LF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable S + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 1.934; N = 409; p = 0.000). Multiple comparison analysis shows significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000). The latter displaying the highest number of fixations counts in lowfrequency short words (Table 5). This data suggests that typical readers were not significantly different from ADHD-I children.

Medium-Frequency Short words (S + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable S + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 20.443; N = 420; p = 0.000). Multiple comparison analysis shows significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) and, between typical readers and ADHD-I children (p = 0.005). In both cases, typical readers had the lowest number of fixations counts in medium-frequency short words (Table 5).

Low-Frequency Medium words (M + LF)

Statistically significant differences were found between the distribution of the variable M + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 23.275; N = 430; p = 0.000). Multiple comparison analysis shows significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) and, between children with dyslexia and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.012). Dyslexics had the highest number of fixations counts in low-frequency medium words (Table 5).

Medium-Frequency Medium words (M + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable M + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 23.943; N = 430; p = 0.000). Multiple comparison analysis shows significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) and, between children with ADHD-I and children with dyslexia (p = 0.025). The latter group had the highest number of fixations counts in medium-frequency medium length words (Table 5).

Low-Frequency Long words (L + LF)

Statistically significant differences were found between the distribution of the variable L + LF across groups ( X K W 2  (2) = 18.768; N = 448; p = 0.000). Multiple comparison analysis reveals significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0,000) and, between children with ADHD-I and children with dyslexia (p = 0.024) (Table 5). In both cases, children with dyslexia had the highest number of fixations counts in low-frequency long words and, simultaneously, higher number of skipped words (Figure 2). This finding suggests that typical readers do not significantly differ from children with ADHD-I.

Medium-Frequency Long words (L + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable L + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 14.682; N = 443; p = 0.001). Multiple comparison analysis shows significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) (Table 5). Once again, children with dyslexia had the highest number of fixations counts in medium-frequency long words. This finding also suggests that typical readers were not significantly different form ADHD-I children.
Finally, Figure 2 allows to conclude that children with ADHD-I had the highest percentage of skipped words as far as this measure is concerned, making it a strong discriminant variable.
Synthesizing, FC has a strong discriminative power to statistically distinguish between typical and dyslexic readers since there were significant differences between groups in all six-word conditions, with typical readers making fewer fixations than atypical readers. It also distinguishes between dyslexia and ADHD-I in two conditions of low frequency (medium and long length words) and one of medium frequency (medium length word).

Single Fixation Duration (SFD)

The eye-tracking measure that produced the most significant group difference for SFD was S + MF (Mediumfrequency Short Words). According to Kruskal-Wallis, statistically significant differences were found between group distributions of the variable S + MF ( X K W 2 (2) = 8.329; N = 158; p = 0.016). Multiple comparison analysis shows that significant differences exist between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.012). The latter group had more single fixation durations in medium-frequency short words (Table 6). This finding suggests that children with dyslexia had higher lexical activation times in medium-frequency short words, a phenomenon that occurs at the early stages of word processing.

Skipped words

Regarding the number of skipped words (see Figure 2), we can observe that children with dyslexia ignored the highest percentage of low-frequency short words. Regarding low-frequency medium words, this variable recruits more attention mechanisms to aid grapheme-phoneme decoding in children with dyslexia. This phenomenon was also observed in low-frequency long words, whose characteristics attract visual attention resources in children with dyslexia due to their uncommon lexical properties.
Furthermore, as word length increases, the number of target words skipped by typical readers decreases; the effect of word-frequency was emphasized in medium size words. Compared to typical readers, children with dyslexia and ADHD-I had globally higher numbers of skipped words. The latter group had the lowest sensitivity to linguistic variables, while children with dyslexia were more sensible to the combined properties of size and frequency interaction effects. Finally, Figure 2 allows to conclude that medium-frequency short words were the most skipped by children with ADHD-I.
In summary, Figure 2 shows that: 1) compared to typical and dyslexic readers, children with ADHD-I had consistently more skipped words in all conditions, which was expected given their visual attention deficits; 2) short and low-frequency words were the most skipped by children with dyslexia and, finally, 3) typical readers, as expected, exhibited a constant relationship between fixated words and their length. The shorter and familiar the words were, the more they were skipped, probably because typical readers have the capacity to perceive and recognize them in parafoveal vision.

First Pass Reading Time

The eye tracking independent variables that produced statistically significant differences between groups were: 1) medium-frequency short words (S + MF); 2) low-frequency medium words (M + LF); 3) medium-frequency medium words (M + MF) and, 4) medium-frequency long words (L + MF).

Medium-frequency short words (S + MF)

Kruskal-Wallis analysis shows statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable S + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 13.104; N = 420; p = 0.001). Multiple comparisons analysis indicates that there were significant differences between normative children and children with dyslexia (p = 0.001), with the latter group having the highest FPRT observed in medium-frequency short words (Table 7).

Low-frequency medium words (M + LF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable M + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 8.666; N = 430; p = 0.013). Multiple comparisons analysis indicates that there were significant differences between normative children and children with dyslexia (p = 0.015). The latter group had higher FPRT for low-frequency medium words (See Table 7).

Medium-frequency medium words (M + MF)

Kruskal-Wallis points out significant differences between the distribution of the variable M + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 11.577; N = 430; p = 0.003). Multiple comparisons test indicates that there were statistical differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.002), with the latter group showing the highest FPRT for medium-frequency short words (Table 7).

Medium-frequency long words (L + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable L + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 9.833; N = 443; p = 0.007). Multiple comparisons analysis indicates that there were significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.033), and between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.015). Moreover, children with ADHD-I had the highest first reading times for medium-frequency long words (Table 7).
In summary, the average duration of FPRT, considered a measure of slower linguistic processes when compared to lexical activation, was correlated both with word-frequency and comprehension processes that integrate several words, the latter related to the integration of grapho-phonological information and access to meaning. This finding suggests that children with dyslexia had longer reaction times, especially in low-frequency words.

Second Pass Reading Time

The eye tracking measures that exhibited statistically significant differences between one or more groups were: 1) low-frequency short words (S + LF); 2) medium-frequency short words (S + MF); 3) low-frequency medium words (M + LF); 4) medium-frequency medium words (M + MF); 5) low-frequency long words (L + LF) and 6) medium-frequency long words (L + MF). In a first observation, we would say that SPRT – as a late measure that captures lexical access processes and word integration into syntactic structures – had a strong discriminative value because it distinguished, in all condition’s, normal readers from children with dyslexia and, in the 3 conditions of word size medium frequency, ADHD-I from controls.

Low-frequency short words (S + LF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable S + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 28.815; N = 183; p = 0.000). Multiple comparisons test shows that there were significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) and, between children with ADHD-I and children with dyslexia (p = 0.007). In both cases, children with dyslexia had larger SPRT in low-frequency short words (Table 8).

Medium-frequency short words (S + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable S + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 11.278; N = 140; p = 0.004). Multiple comparisons analysis shows that there were significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.007) and, between normative readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.012). This measure shows that children with dyslexia produced higher SPRT in medium-frequency short words when compared to the remaining groups (Table 8).

Low-frequency medium words (M + LF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable M + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 10.106; N = 205; p = 0.006). Multiple comparisons analysis indicates that there were significant differences between normative children and children with dyslexia (p = 0.006). The latter group had higher SPRT for low-frequency medium size words (Table 8).

Medium-frequency medium size words (M + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable M + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) =16.616; N = 204; p = 0.000). Multiple comparisons test shows that there were significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.000) and, between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.019). When compared to the remaining groups, children with dyslexia had higher SPRT in medium-frequency medium length words (See Table 8).

Low-frequency long length words (L + LF)

Statistically significant differences were found between the distribution of the variable L + LF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 14.021; N = 221; p = 0.001). Multiple comparisons analysis shows that there were significant differences between normative children and dyslexic readers (p = 0.001) and, between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.032). Moreover, children with dyslexia exhibited higher SPRT in low-frequency long length words (See Table 8).

Medium-frequency long length words (L + MF)

We found statistically significant differences between the distribution of the variable L + MF across groups ( X K W 2 (2) = 13.115; N = 239; p = 0.001). Multiple comparisons analysis shows that there were significant differences between typical readers and children with dyslexia (p = 0.007) and, also between typical readers and children with ADHD-I (p = 0.005). When compared with the remaining groups, children with dyslexia had higher SPRT in medium-frequency long length words (Table 8).
The results listed above clearly demonstrate that, compared to normal readers and children with ADHD-I, children with dyslexia had longer reading times in practically all measures that involved word integration processes, which reflect slow processing effects. This aspect is more noticeable at the integration level of low-frequency words, with a simultaneous word length interaction effect, since low-frequency medium and long-length words require integration times greater than those for low-frequency shortlength words (L + LF_SPRT > M + LF_SPRT > S + LF_SPRT).

Total Fixation Time (TFT)

Total Fixation Time (TFT) had a similar behaviour as SPRT, corroborating statistically significant differences between groups (See Table 9). Clearly, TFT is the variable that best distinguished all groups in all conditions: controls vs. children with dyslexia and controls vs. ADHD-I.
This finding is due to what this measure can reveal: visual word recognition cumulative processes, access to meaning, word integration in syntactic structures and in the mental representation of the text that is being constructed while reading. Subsequently, there is an interaction effect between word-frequency and word length (L + LF > M + LF > S + LF), with children with dyslexia having greater TFT’s, followed by children with ADHD-I and, finally, by the control group.

Discussion

Firstly, it should be noted that all target words were expected to be fixated and that size and frequency should have an impact by increasing or decreasing the number of fixations, as well as increasing or decreasing the duration of fixations and regressions in each word. This performance will depend on the participants neuropsycholinguistic profile. The magnitude of this effect should vary according to the two lexical properties (less frequent / longer words, more fixations) studied, and be influenced by idiosyncratic group characteristics.
The eye movement dependent variables that best distinguished typical readers from children with dyslexia were: 1) Fixation Counts (FC); 2) First Pass Reading Times (FPRT) and 3) Second Pass Reading Times (SPRT).
Regarding the first variable, a global effect was found for all lexical properties studied, namely short and medium-frequency short words, low and medium-frequency medium words, and long-length low and medium-frequency words. Low-frequency short words activate more attentional mechanisms in children with dyslexia, since they are unknown or unfamiliar and, therefore require phonological decoding representations/mechanisms that may be of poor quality in this population.
In turn, the second variable, FPRT, highlights differences between medium-frequency short words, low and medium-frequency medium words and medium-frequency long words. SPRT had also a similar behaviour as FC and TFT, making it possible to find effects in all conditions evaluated.
In all the above-mentioned variables, children with dyslexia showed a poorer performance that was independent of their cognitive profile. Reading difficulties shown by dyslexic readers are better explained by deficits in phonological processing and by diminished activation speeds when accessing lexical pathway.
Regarding children with ADHD-I, the eye tracking variables that best distinguished them from typical readers were FC, FPRT, SPRT and, TFT. Regarding the first measure, compared to children with dyslexia, we found lexical properties effects only on medium-frequency short length words. As for the second measure, it was only possible to identify effects in one condition, namely long length medium-frequency words. For the third variable, effects were identified on four conditions, namely mediumfrequency short and medium words and, low and mediumfrequency long words. On the other hand, TFT emphasised effects in all lexical conditions studied, like what happens in children with dyslexia.
Regarding children with ADHD-I, only two ocular variables distinguished them from dyslexic readers, with the last group showing higher FC and longer SPRT. In the first measure, effects were found in three conditions, namely in medium and long low-frequency words and, in medium length medium-frequency words. As for the last, effects were identified only when the condition was short low-frequency words.
These findings allow us to conclude that eye movement differences between normative readers and children with ADHD-I are fewer than the ones found for typical readers compared to children with dyslexia, and that low-frequency long words were the ones that most affected both groups.
Furthermore, this investigation found very significant frequency-word and word-length effects between groups. Similar to the study done by Hyönä and Olson (1995) and Tiffin-Richards and Schroeder (2015), who found that the most difficult words to recognize, namely long length words and low-frequency words, received more fixations than relatively easier words to process, namely short length words and high frequency-words. Our research has also found that short and medium low-frequency words have an identical profile as long low-frequency words. The number of fixations they received were directly proportional to the word size, our data supports this finding unequivocally.
The effects of length and frequency were equally found both on the initial encounter with the word and on the frequency of regressions back to the target word. These effects were due to the lexical properties of more complex words which attracted multiple fixations on themselves. This effect, at the word-length level, was also observed in the studies mentioned earlier, as well as the frequency effect that also influences the duration of the initial fixation on the target word, which is higher for low-frequency words and lower for high-frequency words (Hyönä & Olson, 1995; Tiffin-Richards & Schroeder, 2015).
The study by Hyönä and Olson (1995) also found a very similar pattern of outcomes between dyslexic and typical readers. These authors concluded that, in both groups, fixation patterns during reading reflect momentary variations in relative ease of processing in a similar fashion to that observed in adults’ typical readers. These findings do not match our results which revealed statistically significant differences between groups according to the lexical properties of words, supporting the conclusions of other authors who found that children with dyslexia, compared to other readers, have qualitatively and quantitatively different eye movement patterns and ocular characteristics (Deans et al., 2010; G. Pavlidis, 1981b; Zangwill & Blakemore, 1972).
According to some authors (Caldani et al., 2020; Deans et al., 2010; Eden et al., 1994, 1996; G. Pavlidis, 1981a), children with dyslexia exhibit erratic eye movements distributed almost randomly across the line of text, suggesting a deficiency in visual attentional processing and an immaturity of brain structures responsible for pursuit triggering. According to these authors, the largest quantitative and qualitative differences between children with dyslexia and typical readers lies in the size and number of regressions.
Our study confirms the findings of several authors, who concluded that children with dyslexia have higher number of regressions/revisits and, in turn, longer SPRT compared to typical readers, as well as higher number of fixations on target words (Deans et al., 2010; Hawelka & Wimmer, 2005; G. Pavlidis, 1981a). Our data also confirms Pavlidis’s theory of oculomotor dysfunction (G. Pavlidis, 1981b), which predicts that children with dyslexia often make more fixations and more regressions/revisits in particular, along with shorter fixation durations. In other studies by the same author (G. Pavlidis, 1978, 1981a), it has been shown that children with dyslexia make almost twice as many regressions as 6-year-old typical readers attending first grade. While 6-year-old typical readers performed regressive eye movements invariably smaller in size than the previous progressive saccade, the regressions of children with dyslexia usually appeared in groups of two or more and were often larger than the previous progressive saccade.
The finding in both Pavlidis’s studies (G. Pavlidis, 1978, 1981a) that high frequency of regressions, substantiated by their large size and erratic behaviour, makes them a decisive element in distinguishing children with dyslexia from other readers. Given the absence of differences between children with dyslexia and typical readers in their study, Hyönä and Olson (1995) do not support the hypothesis of an oculomotor dysfunction. According to these authors, this data is consistent with the developmental delay theory.
Two decisions need to be made while reading with regard to readers’ eye movements: how long to stay fixated at the present location and where to go next (Hyönä & Olson, 1995). Rayner and McConkie (1976) found reasons to believe that these decisions are governed by independent mechanisms, namely automatic perceptual behaviours derived from experience, cognitive abilities, and lexical knowledge. Consequently, according to Hyönä and Olson (1995), it could be argued that only one mechanism operates erroneously in children with dyslexia.
If the chunk involved in the duration works inappropriately, it means that the duration of the first fixation or the length of the gaze (i.e., the initial encounter with the word) will not reflect difficulties in word recognition. However, similarly to Hyönä and Olson (1995) work, this notion was not supported by our data. Alternatively, it could be argued that the mechanism of reading redirection is not working properly. This change is involved in the visual deficit theory, which postulates a visual transient system defect sensitive to stimuli presented outside the foveal region (Lovegrove, 1992).
Previous research has confirmed that extrafoveal information is used to determine where to go next in the text (Chace et al., 2005; Jones et al., 2013; Rayner & Pollatsek, 1981; Schotter et al., 2014). This type of deficit would be implicated in many regressive fixations and rereading’s in the absence of any processing difficulties. Contrary to Hyönä and Olson (1995) study, our data on SPRT for correctly read words were not identical between children with dyslexia and typical readers. Moreover, the frequency of performing a regression immediately after an initial fixation on the target word was higher for children with dyslexia. Another finding corroborated by the same study was the presence of significant differences resulting from frequency-word effect on first and second pass reading times. In previous studies with adult readers, the effect appeared consistently at the level of first fixation durations.
The finding that word frequency affects the duration of the first fixation is consistent with the view that word frequency influences a relatively early phase of word processing (Hyönä & Olson, 1995). This effect was particularly observed in low-frequency words, which is consistent with the idea that only robust effects are reflected in first fixation durations. In previous studies, reinspection’s were not often analysed as a function of word frequency (Hyönä & Olson, 1995).
The few studies to investigate reinspection data (Henderson & Ferreira, 1993; Subbaram, 2005), have found significantly longer regressive fixation durations for low-frequency words than for high-frequency words, as in the present study. Furthermore, they found a similar but not significant trend in the number of fixations, unlike the present study which found a significant but equally identical trend.
The word-length effect was shown in the study by Hyönä and Olson (1995) both by a larger number of fixations and, by longer FPRT and SPRT in long words, a finding also corroborated by our study. Like the study mentioned previously, first fixation duration was not influenced by word-length, which also happens in proficient reading (Kliegl et al., 1983; Rayner & McConkie, 1976).
In the same study mentioned previously, long words attracted more fixations than medium and short words, a finding that was also supported by our study. At the time of the publication of the study by Hyönä and Olson (1995), no word length influences on reinspection among typical readers had been observed. Carpenter and Daneman (1981) also observed that word length did not correlate with the duration of regressive fixations. Our results point in the same direction as both studies mentioned above, as it can be attributed to the ability of typical readers to process words effortlessly, regardless of their size.
In the present study, we found an interaction between word length and frequency in all eye movement measurements used, namely FC, SFD, FPRT, SPRT and, TFT, which also corroborates the study of Hyönä and Olson (1995). Hyönä and Olson (1995) found a clearly significant word-frequency effect on FFD for short and long words, whereas in our study the effect was significant for medium and long words. According to these authors, there is no clear explanation for the absence of a frequency-word effect on medium-length words. The finding that mediumlength low-frequency words tend to be slightly more frequent than other low-frequency words may, according to Hyönä and Olson (1995), be a possible reason.
Our data identified an effect of frequency on mediumlength words, supporting the explanation given by Hyönä and Olson (1995). The finding that children with dyslexia have higher first fixation durations in medium-frequency long length words compared to low-frequency medium length words suggests that the latter were more familiar. However, the interaction was more easily interpretable considering first pass reading time. This variable reflects, in the study conducted by Hyönä and Olson (1995), the finding that low-frequency long words capture a greater number of fixations on them.
Nevertheless, in the present study, the interaction between long and low frequency words relative to FPRT was not significant enough to discriminate the groups. In contrast, it was significant for medium-frequency short words, low and medium-frequency medium words, and mediumfrequency long words. Among these, the highest FPRT were made by children with dyslexia at the low-frequency medium word level, followed by medium-frequency medium words.
Relatively to SPRT, low and medium-frequency words attracted considerably more reinspection’s than other words, corroborating once again Hyönä and Olson (1995) work. These were among the least frequent words in the range of target words selected for this study.
Hyönä and Olson (1995) stated that the presence of statistically significant differences at the FFD level makes this measure the one that presents the clearest and most general effects in terms of frequency and word length, but it was little discriminative in our study.
Our results show that FC, SPRT and TFT are the most comprehensive measures to study word frequency and word length effects; the last measure was not included in the study by Hyönä and Olson (1995). According to Hyönä and Olson (1995), SPRT was restricted to a subset of words. For them, the probability of going back to a word seems to be determined more by its frequency than by its length. However, in our study we found that children with dyslexia when dealing with low-frequency words have SPRT directly proportional to word size.
It is important to mention that the results obtained by Hyönä and Olson (1995) were obtained through a reading aloud task, so they cannot be generalized to silent reading, as happens in our study. Unlike reading aloud, which implies a stronger link between eye movements occurring during reading and word recognition processes, silent reading is where reading differences between groups in oculomotor functioning is most likely to be observed. This data was later corroborated by Rayner (1998) who found that the average fixation duration is shorter in silent reading, approximately 225 milliseconds compared to 275 milliseconds in reading aloud. Our study allows us to accept this possibility.
Regarding the effects of word length and word frequency in children with ADHD-I during silent reading, we did not find any study that addressed this theme. Children with ADHD-I are characterized by being unable to keep their attention on a continuous performance test, such as a task involving reading for understanding. This data is expressed by their high false alarm rates and increased reaction times (Fried et al., 2014). However, this inability to maintain attention is also shared with children with dyslexia (Deans et al., 2010). This striking feature was discovered by Pavlidis (1981b) in a group of children with dyslexia characterized by their inability to sustain fixation for more than a second on a task that involved following precisely and as fast as possible five light-emitting diodes (LEDs). It is important to note that the inability to accurately maintain fixation at a given point for one second or more has also been observed in other dyslexic studies (Lloyd & Pavlidis, 1978; Vurpillot, 1976).
Finally, it can be concluded that reading performance in children with dyslexia is affected proportionally by the number of low-frequency words in the text, and the interaction between frequency and word size further contributes to a decreased reading fluency. This finding ultimately alters the degree of understanding given the allocation of a greater number of cognitive resources for processing the lexical complexity of words.

Conclusion

The present study allowed us to conclude that children with dyslexia have cognitive profiles and eye movement patterns during reading that are qualitatively and quantitatively different from normative children and their peers with ADHD-I. The observation that children with ADHDI also differ from dyslexic and typical readers in linguistic performance measures, whether in formal reading assessment or in eye tracking, points to the existence of different cognitive resources at the base of their reading problems.
This study supports the theory that in the genesis of developmental dyslexia there is a predominance of phonological impairments in comorbidity with other cognitive deficits, predominantly at the level of short-term verbal memory and verbal working memory. The use of eye tracking in this study allowed us the identification of interaction effects between word-length and word-frequency, characteristics that require identical cognitive resources, such as working memory, as well as, in some cases, oculomotor coordination, processing speed, short-term verbal memory, visual attention and ability to access lexical knowledge. The finding that children with dyslexia make, on average, more fixations, and regressions than typical readers support, in our opinion, the hypothesis that, in addition to being a phonological disorder, an oculomotor dysfunction and/or sequential incapacity coexist in dyslexia. This dysfunction also produces erratic eye movements and changes in visual perception of orderly and sequential text processing. However, while phonological processing is a necessary and ever-present condition for the decoding of any written word, regardless of its size or frequency, the oculomotor function is activated only during reinspection’s of words with certain lexical properties.
As for children with ADHD-I, we found that deficits in several cognitive functions, namely visual attention, lexical knowledge access, short-term verbal memory, shortterm visual memory, visuospatial working memory, and processing speed were responsible for the neuropsycholinguistic difficulties manifested by these children. Unlike children with dyslexia, children with ADHD-I have a greater and more generalized number of cognitive shortfalls, which affect visual perception patterns and measures of linguistic performance. Our study supports the evidence that children with ADHD-I also have difficulties in phonological awareness, however, to a lesser degree than their dyslexic peers due to the absence of deficits at the level of verbal working memory. Another feature that allowed us to distinguish the reading profile of children with ADHDI from those with dyslexia and to safely affirm that the reading problems of the former are triggered by different brain (dis)functions, was the complete absence of oculomotor dysfunctions in this group, regardless of the lexical property of the word. While oculomotor dysfunction seems to be a characteristic unique to dyslexia and observed only in words with certain lexical characteristics, the disturbance of visuospatial attention appears to be a specific property of ADHD-I, present in the decoding of any type of word.
The data gathered in this study confirms that in the origin of the different reading profiles observed in dyslexia and ADHD-I are multiple cognitive deficits, which supports the multiple cognitive deficits theory, which states that the reading difficulties encountered both in dyslexia and ADHD-I are explained by different cognitive underlying mechanisms. Within these deficits, there are those that are shared by both conditions, but of special importance for this study are the ones that would allow dyslexia to be distinguished from ADHD-I. To discover the specific neuropsycholinguistic limitations of each of these neurodevelopmental disorders, it was essential to record eye movements using eye tracking, which could be a tool with high diagnostic capacity in the future.
With the data gathered and presented in this work, in a future development of this study, we will show, using predictive modelling, that it is possible to move from grouplevel descriptions to individual-level predictions with high sensitivity and specificity, which is a first step towards making eye tracking a viable screening method.
Finally, it is important to highlight some limitations of the present study, namely the sample size and the problems related to the differential diagnosis and give indications of future directions for this research. In relation to the first, in a future edition of this work, we intend to increase sample size to reinforce the generalization of our conclusions to the studied target populations. As for the second limitation, we were not immune to the limitations encountered in other studies when selecting participants and assign them by clinical groups, since there is a high comorbidity between both disorders. We believe that many of the doubts and wrong conclusions that have arisen in other investigations regarding the sharing of the same cognitive deficits by both clinical conditions are due to misdiagnosis at the stage of selecting participants. In our study, we believe that the identification of neuropsycholinguistic traits distinct from children with dyslexia and children with ADHD-I helped to mitigate the effects of comorbidity.

Acknowledgments

This work has been funded by the Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT, Foundation for Science and Technology), grant UIDB/00214/2020.

Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Appendix A

Table 10. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: WISC-III composite results.
Table 10. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: WISC-III composite results.
Jemr 15 00001 i001
Note: 1ANOVA F Test; 2Tukey HSD; 3Brown-Forsythe statistic; 4Games-Howell;5Kruskal-Wallis independent samples; SD – Standard deviation; X ˜ – Median, Q3 – 3rd percentile, Q1 – 1st percentile.
Table 11. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: WISC-III subtest results.
Table 11. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: WISC-III subtest results.
Jemr 15 00001 i002
Note. n.s. – not statistically significant. 1ANOVA F test; 2Tukey HSD; 3Brown-Forsythe Statistic; 4Games-Howell;5Kruskal-Wallis independent samples. SD – Standard deviation; X ˜ – Median, Q3 – 3rd percentile, Q1 – 1st percentile.
Table 12. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM).
Table 12. Means, standard deviations, median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM).
Jemr 15 00001 i003
Note: n.s. – not statistically significant. 3Brown-Forsythe statistic; 5Kruskal-Wallis independent samples; SD – Standard deviation; 7 X ˜ – Median, Q3 – 3rd percentile, Q1 – 1st percentile.
Table 13. Means and standard deviations: Rey Complex Figure Test.
Table 13. Means and standard deviations: Rey Complex Figure Test.
ItemsGroupsMean (SD)Multiple Comparisons
Copy score Control 25.50 (5.68) n.s.
Dyslexia 26.08 (6.52)
ADHD-I 21.50 (7.11)
Copy (time of execution in seconds) Control 293.33 (78.75)
Dyslexia 318.28 (111.72)
ADHD-I 341.44 (119.45)
Memory score Control 10.13 (6.14)
Dyslexia 11.97 (4.24)
ADHD-I 8.55 (5.46)
Memory (time of execution in seconds) Control 158.45 (87.48)
Dyslexia 210.00 (109.79)
ADHD-I 150.22 (55.80)
Note: n.s. – not statistically significant.
Table 14. 1st and 3rd percentiles and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: O Rei – Reading Fluency and Accuracy Test.
Table 14. 1st and 3rd percentiles and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples: O Rei – Reading Fluency and Accuracy Test.
Jemr 15 00001 i004
Note. 5Kruskal-Wallis independent samples; X ˜ – Median, Q3 – 3rd percentile, Q1 – 1st percentile.
Table 15. Means, standard deviations and ANOVA independent samples test: TCL – Reading Comprehension Test.
Table 15. Means, standard deviations and ANOVA independent samples test: TCL – Reading Comprehension Test.
Jemr 15 00001 i005
Note. n.s. – not statistically significant. 1 ANOVA F test; 2Tukey HSD; SD – Standard Deviation.

References

  1. Adler-Grinberg, D., and L. Stark. 1978. Eye movements, scanpaths, and dyslexia. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 55, 8: 557–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. 2002. DSM-IV-TR Manual de Diagnóstico e Estatística das Perturbações Mentais, Climepsi (ed.); 4a Edição. American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ashby, J., J. Yang, K. H. C. Evans, and K. Rayner. 2012. Eye movements and the perceptual span in silent and oral reading. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 74, 4: 634–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bacelar do Nascimento, M., J. Casteleiro, M. Marques, F. Barreto, R. Amaro, and R. Veloso. n.d.Léxico Multifuncional Computorizado do Português Contemporâneo. http://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/en/recurso/multifunctional-computational-lexicon-contemporary (accessed on 1 January 2014).
  5. Barkley, R. A. 1997. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin 121, 1: 65–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Barkley, R. A. 1999. Response inhibition in attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 5, 3: 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Barkley, R. A., G. Grodzinsky, and G. J. DuPaul. 1992. Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review and research report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 20, 2: 163–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Bazen, L., M. van den Boer, P. F. de Jong, and E. H. de Bree. 2020. Early and late diagnosed dyslexia in secondary school: Performance on literacy skills and cognitive correlates. Dyslexia 26, 4: 359–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Benfatto, M. N., G. Ö. Seimyr, J. Ygge, T. Pansell, A. Rydberg, and C. Jacobson. 2016. Screening for dyslexia using eye tracking during reading. PLoS ONE 11, 12: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Berninger, V. W. 2001. Understanding the ‘Lexia’ in dyslexia: A multidisciplinary team approach to learning disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia 51, 1: 21–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Blythe, H. I., and H. S. S. L. Joseph. 2011. Children’s eye movements during reading. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Blythe, H. I., S. P. Liversedge, H. S. S. L. Joseph, S. J. White, and K. Rayner. 2009. Visual information capture during fixations in reading for children and adults. Vision Research 49, 12: 1583–1591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Burke, H. R. 1985. Raven’s Progressive Matrices (1938): More on norms, reliability, and validity. Journal of Clinical Psychology 41, 2: 231–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cadime, I., I. S. Ribeiro, and F. L. Viana. 2012. TCL-Teste de Compreensão da Leitura. CEGOC-TEA Edições. [Google Scholar]
  15. Caldani, S., C.-L. Gerard, H. Peyre, and M. P. Bucci. 2020. Pursuit eye movements in dyslexic children: evidence for an immaturity of brain oculomotor structures? Journal of Eye Movement Research 13, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carpenter, P. A., and M. Daneman. 1981. Lexical retrieval and error recovery in reading: A model based on eye fixations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 2: 137–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Carvalho, A. 2010. O REI - Teste de Avaliação da Fluência e Precisão da Leitura. EDIPSICO - edições e investigação em psicologia, Lda. [Google Scholar]
  18. Chace, K. H., K. Rayner, and A. D. Well. 2005. Eye Movements and Phonological Parafoveal Preview: Effects of Reading Skill. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Exp?Rimentale 59, 3: 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Deans, P., L. O’Laughlin, B. Brubaker, N. Gay, and D. Krug. 2010. Use of Eye Movement Tracking in the Differential Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Reading Disability. Psychology 01, 04: 238–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dotan, S., and T. Katzir. 2018. Mind the gap: Increased inter-letter spacing as a means of improving reading performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 174: 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Eden, G. F., J. F. Stein, H. M. Wood, and F. B. Wood. 1994. Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal childre. Vision Res 34: 1345–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Eden, G. F., J. W. VanMeter, J. M. Rumsey, J. M. Maisog, R. P. Woods, and T. A. Zeffiro. 1996. Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal children. Nature 382: 66–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Faraone, S. V., J. Biederman, W. Weber, and R. L. Russel. 1998. Psychiatric, Neuropsychological, and Psychosocial Features of DSM-IV Subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results From a Clinically Referred Sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 37, 2: 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Flake, R. A., E. P. Lorch, and R. Milich. 2007. The Effects of Thematic Importance on Story Recall among Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Comparison Children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 35, 1: 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Flory, K., R. Milich, E. P. Lorch, A. N. Hayden, C. Strange, and R. Welsh. 2006. Online Story Comprehension among Children with ADHD: Which Core Deficits are Involved? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34, 6: 850–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Franzen, L., Z. Stark, and A. P. Johnson. 2021. Individuals with dyslexia use a different visual sampling strategy to read text. Scientific Reports 11, 1: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Fried, M., E. Tsitsiashvili, Y. S. Bonneh, A. Sterkin, T. Wygnanski-jaffe, T. Epstein, and U. Polat. 2014. ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication. In Vision Research. Elsevier Ltd: Vol. 101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Garzia, R. P., J. E. Richman, S. B. Nicholson, and C. S. Gaines. 1990. A new visual-verbal saccade test: the development eye movement test (DEM). Journal of the American Optometric Association 61, 2: 124–135. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2313029.
  29. Gould, T. D., T. M. Bastain, M. E. Israel, D. W. Hommer, and F. X. Castellanos. 2001. Altered performance on an ocular fixation task in attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 50, 8: 633–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hakvoort, B., M. van den Boer, T. Leenaars, P. Bos, and J. Tijms. 2017. Improvements in reading accuracy as a result of increased interletter spacing are not specific to children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 164: 101–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Hawelka, S., and H. Wimmer. 2005. Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research 45, 7: 855–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Henderson, J. M., and F. Ferreira. 1993. Eye movement control during reading: fixation measures reflect foveal but not parafoveal processing difficulty. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Experimentale 47, 2: 201–221. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8364530. [PubMed]
  33. Hermena, E. W. 2021. Manipulating interword and interletter spacing in cursive script: An eye movements investigation of reading Persian. Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Huestegge, L., R. Radach, D. Corbic, and S. M. Huestegge. 2009. Oculomotor and linguistic determinants of reading development: A longitudinal study. Vision Research 49, 24: 2948–2959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Hutzler, F., and H. Wimmer. 2004. Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language 89, 1: 235–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hyönä, J., and R. K. Olson. 1995. Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21, 6: 1430–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jones, M. W., J. Ashby, and H. P. Branigan. 2013. Dyslexia and fluency: parafoveal and foveal influences on rapid automatized naming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Joseph, H. S. S. L., K. Nation, and S. P. Liversedge. 2013. Using Eye Movements to Investigate Word Frequency Effects in Children’s Sentence Reading. School Psychology Review 42, 2: 207–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Just, M. A., and P. A. Carpenter. 1980. A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review 87, 4: 329–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Klein, C., A. Raschke, and A. Brandenbusch. 2003. Development of pro- and antisaccades in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. Psychophysiology 40, 1: 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Kliegl, R., E. Grabner, M. Rolfs, and R. Engbert. 2004. Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 16, 1–2: 262–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kliegl, R., R. K. Olson, and B. J. Davidson. 1983. Edited by K. Rayner. On problems of unconfounding perceptual and language processes. In Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes. Academic Press: pp. 333–343. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kuster, S. M., M. van Weerdenburg, M. Gompel, and A. M. T. Bosman. 2018. Dyslexie font does not benefit reading in children with or without dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 68, 1: 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lloyd, P., and G. Pavlidis. 1978. Child language and eye movements: The relative effects of sentence and situation on the comprehension in young children. Bull. Briet. Psychol. Soc. 31: 70–71. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lovegrove, W. 1992. Edited by N. Singh and I. Beale. The visual deficit hypothesis. In Learning disabilities: Nature, theory, and treatment. Springer-Verlag: pp. 246–269. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lyon, G. R., S. E. Shaywitz, and B. A. Shaywitz. 2003. A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 53, 1: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Marinus, E., M. Mostard, E. Segers, T. M. Schubert, A. Madelaine, and K. Wheldall. 2016. A Special Font for People with Dyslexia: Does it Work and, if so, why? Dyslexia 22, 3: 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Measso, G., G. Zappalà, F. Cavarzeran, T. H. Crook, L. Romani, F. J. Pirozzolo, F. Grigoletto, L. A. Amaducci, D. Massari, and B. D. Lebowitz. 1993. Raven’s colored progressive matrices: a normative study of a random sample of healthy adults. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 88, 1: 70–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Miller, A. C., J. M. Keenan, R. S. Betjemann, E. G. Willcutt, B. F. Pennington, and R. K. Olson. 2013. Reading Comprehension in Children with ADHD: Cognitive Underpinnings of the Centrality Deficit. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 41, 3: 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Munoz, D. P., I. T. Armstrong, K. A. Hampton, and K. D. Moore. 2003. Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology 90, 1: 503–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Olson, R. K., F. Conners, and J. Rack. 1991. Edited by F. J. Stein. Eye movements in dyslexics and normal readers. In Vision and visual dyslexia. Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  52. Pavlidis, G. 1978. The dyslexics’ erratic eye movements: case studies. Dyslex. Rev. 1: 22–28. [Google Scholar]
  53. Pavlidis, G. 1980. Dyslexia: eye movements in reading and beyond. Nursing Mirror 150, 3: 22–26. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6899283.
  54. Pavlidis, G. 1981a. Dyslexia Research and its Application to Education. In Dyslexia Research and its Application to Education. Edited by G. T. Pavlidis and T. R. Miles. London: John Wiley. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  55. Pavlidis, G. 1981b. Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 19, 1: 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Pennington, B. F., D. Groisser, and M. C. Welsh. 1993. Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology 29, 3: 511–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Pereira, N., A. Costa, and M. Guerreiro. 2016. Eye-Tracking: A Diagnostic Tool for reading disabilities? Twenty-Third Annual Meeting Society for the Scientific Study of Reading; p. 41. https://www.triplesr.org/eye-tracking-diagnostictool-reading-disabilities.
  58. Peterson, R. L., and B. F. Pennington. 2012. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet (London, England) 379, 9830: 1997–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Peterson, R. L., and B. F. Pennington. 2015. Developmental Dyslexia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 11, 1: 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pollatsek, A., E. Reichle, and K. Rayner. 2003. Edited by J. Hyönä, R. Radach and H. Deubel. Modeling eye movements in reading. In The mind’s eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research. pp. 361–390. [Google Scholar]
  61. Rau, A. K., K. Moeller, and K. Landerl. 2014. The Transition From Sublexical to Lexical Processing in a Consistent Orthography: An Eye-Tracking Study. Scientific Studies of Reading 18, 3: 224–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Rau, A. K., K. Moll, M. J. Snowling, and K. Landerl. 2015. Effects of orthographic consistency on eye movement behavior: German and English children and adults process the same words differently. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 130: 92–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Raven, J., J. Court, and J. Raven. 1984. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales, Section 2, Coloured Progressive Matrices. H. K. Lewis & Co., Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  64. Raven, J., J. Court, and J. Raven. 1990. Raven Manual: Coloured Progressive Matrizes. Psychologist Press. [Google Scholar]
  65. Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124, 3: 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Rayner, K. 2009. Eye Movements in Reading: Models and Data. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Rayner, K., and S. A. Duffy. 1986. Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition 14, 3: 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Rayner, K., A. W. Inhoff, R. E. Morrison, M. L. Sloiaczek, and J. H. Bertera. 1981. Masking of foveal and parafoveal vision during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7: 167–179. [Google Scholar]
  69. Rayner, K., and G. W. McConkie. 1976. What guides a reader’s eye movements? Vision Research 16, 8: 829–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Rayner, K., and A. Pollatsek. 1981. Eye Movement Control during Reading: Evidence for Direct Control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 33, 4: 351–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rayner, K., T. J. Slattery, and N. N. Bélanger. 2010. Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17, 6: 834–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Reichle, E. D., S. P. Liversedge, D. Drieghe, H. I. Blythe, H. S. S. L. Joseph, S. J. White, and K. Rayner. 2013. Using E-Z Reader to examine the concurrent development of eye-movement control and reading skill. Developmental Review 33, 2: 110–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Reichle, E. D., A. Pollatsek, D. L. Fisher, and K. Rayner. 1998. Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review 105: 125–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Reichle, E. D., K. Rayner, and A. Pollatsek. 2003. The E-Z Reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparison to other models. Brain and Behavioral Sciences 26: 445–476. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rey, A. 1959. Manuel de Test de Copie d’une Figure Complexe. Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. [Google Scholar]
  76. Rimrodt, S. L., A. M. Clements-Stephens, K. R. Pugh, S. M. Courtney, P. Gaur, J. J. Pekar, and L. E. Cutting. 2009. Functional MRI of sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia: Beyond word recognition. Cerebral Cortex 19, 2: 402–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Rommelse, N. N. J., M. E. Altink, E. A. Fliers, N. C. Martin, C. J. M. Buschgens, C. A. Hartman, J. K. Buitelaar, S. V. Faraone, J. A. Sergeant, and J. Oosterlaan. 2009. Comorbid problems in ADHD: Degree of association, shared endophenotypes, and formation of distinct subtypes. Implications for a future DSM. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37, 6: 793–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Rubino, C. A., and H. A. Minden. 1973. An Analysis of Eye-Movements in Children with a Reading Disability. Cortex 9, 2: 217–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Schotter, E. R., E. D. Reichle, and K. Rayner. 2014. Rethinking parafoveal processing in reading : Serial-attention models can explain semantic preview benefit and N + 2 preview effects 22, 3: 309–333.
  80. Shaywitz, B. A., G. Lyon, and S. E. Shaywitz. 2006. The role of functional magnetic resonance imaging in understanding reading and dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology 30, 1: 613–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Shaywitz, S. E., M. Escobar, B. A. Shaywitz, J. Fletcher, and R. Makuch. 1992. Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribuition of reading ability. N Engl J Med 326, 3: 145–150. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  82. Simões, M. 2000. Investigações no âmbito da aferição nacional do teste das Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven (M.P.C.R). Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian/Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia. [Google Scholar]
  83. Simões, M. 2008. Edited by L. Almeida, M. Simões, C. Machado and M. Gonçalves. Recensão crítica: O Teste das Matrizes Progressivas de Raven (MPRC) em Portugal. In Avaliação Psicológica. Instrumentos validados para a população portuguesa Vol. II. Quarteto: pp. 151–191. [Google Scholar]
  84. Sjoblom, A. M., E. Eaton, and S. D. Stagg. 2016. The effects of letter spacing and coloured overlays on reading speed and accuracy in adult dyslexia. British Journal of Educational Psychology 86, 4: 630–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Smyrnakis, I., V. Andreadakis, A. Rina, N. Bοufachrentin, and I. Aslanides. 2021. Silent versus reading out loud modes: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, 2: 1–16. https://bop.unibe.ch/JEMR/article/view/7769.
  86. Soares, A. P., J. C. Medeiros, A. Simões, J. Machado, A. Costa, Á. Iriarte, J. J. de Almeida, A. P. Pinheiro, and M. Comesaña. 2014. ESCOLEX: A gradelevel lexical database from European Portuguese elementary to middle school textbooks. Behavior Research Methods 46, 1: 240–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Spichtig, A., J. Pascoe, J. Ferrara, and C. Vorstius. 2017. A comparison of eye movement measures across reading efficiency quartile groups in elementary, middle, and high school students in the U.S. Journal of Eye Movement Research 10, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Subbaram, M. V. 2005. Eye Movements and Information Processing During Reading. In Optometry and Vision Science. Vol. 82, Issue 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Sucena, A., and S. L. Castro. 2011. ALEPE–Bateria e Avaliação da Leitura em Português Europeu. CEGOC-TEA Edições. [Google Scholar]
  90. Tamm, L., C. A. Denton, J. N. Epstein, C. Schatschneider, H. Taylor, L. E. Arnold, O. Bukstein, J. Anixt, A. Koshy, N. C. Newman, J. Maltinsky, P. Brinson, R. E. A. Loren, M. R. Prasad, L. Ewing-Cobbs, and A. Vaughn. 2017. Comparing treatments for children with ADHD and word reading difficulties: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 85, 5: 434–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Test, P., and G. Bubble. 2012. BeGaze Manual Table of Contents How to Read this Document. July. [Google Scholar]
  92. Tiffin-Richards, S. P., and S. Schroeder. 2015. Word length and frequency effects on children’s eye movements during silent reading. Vision Research 113, PA: 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., A. M. Koitzsch, U. Dürrwächter, A. N. Sokolov, J. Reinhard, and G. Klosinski. 2010. Eye movements in German-speaking children with and without dyslexia when reading aloud. Acta Ophthalmologica 88, 6: 681–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Vaughn, S., P. T. Cirino, J. Wanzek, J. Wexler, J. M. Fletcher, C. D. Denton, A. Barth, M. Romain, and D. J. Francis. 2010. Response to Intervention for Middle School Students With Reading Difficulties: Effects of a Primary and Secondary Intervention. School Psychology Review 39, 1: 3–21. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3624763/pdf/nihms412728.pdf.
  95. von der Malsburg, T., R. Kliegl, and S. Vasishth. 2015. Determinants of Scanpath Regularity in Reading. Cognitive Science 39, 7: 1675–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. von der Malsburg, T., and S. Vasishth. 2013. Scanpaths reveal syntactic underspecification and reanalysis strategies. Language and Cognitive Processes 28, 10: 1545–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Vorstius, C., R. Radach, and C. J. Lonigan. 2014. Eye movements in developing readers: A comparison of silent and oral sentence reading. Visual Cognition 22, 3–4: 458–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Vurpillot, E. 1976. The Visual World of the Child. International U Press. [Google Scholar]
  99. Wechsler, D. 1991. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III): Manual. The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  100. Wechsler, D. 2003. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III)–Portuguese Version (M. R. Simões, A. M. Rocha, and C. Ferreira). CEGOCTEA Edições. [Google Scholar]
  101. Wery, J. J., and J. A. Diliberto. 2017. The effect of a specialized dyslexia font, OpenDyslexic, on reading rate and accuracy. Annals of Dyslexia 67, 2: 114–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Willcutt, E. G., R. S. Betjemann, L. M. McGrath, N. A. Chhabildas, R. K. Olson, J. C. DeFries, and B. F. Pennington. 2010. Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex 46, 10: 1345–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Willcutt, E. G., R. S. Betjemann, L. M. Mcgrath, and B. F. Pennington. 2011. Etiology and neurophysiology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple deficit models. 46, 10, 1345–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Willcutt, E. G., and B. F. Pennington. 2000. Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 41, 8: 1039–1048. https://journals.lww.com/00001504-200507000-00013. [PubMed]
  105. Willcutt, E. G., B. F. B. F. B. F. Pennington, R. K. Olson, N. Chhabildas, and J. Hulslander. 2005. Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 1: 35–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Willcutt, E. G., B. F. Pennington, R. Boada, J. S. Ogline, R. A. Tunick, N. A. Chhabildas, and R. K. Olson. 2001. A comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability and attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 1: 157–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Zangwill, O. L., and C. Blakemore. 1972. Dyslexia: Reversal of eye-movements during reading. Neuropsychologia 10, 3: 371–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Zorzi, M., C. Barbiero, A. Facoetti, I. Lonciari, M. Carrozzi, M. Montico, L. Bravar, F. George, C. Pech-Georgel, and J. C. Ziegler. 2012. Extralarge letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, 28: 11455–11459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Reading task. A, B, and C are respectively 1st, 2nd and 3rd slides with coloured rectangles representing AOI / Target words. A’, B’ and C’ are respectively 1st, 2nd and 3rd slides with embedded target words. Total number of words = 264.
Figure 1. Reading task. A, B, and C are respectively 1st, 2nd and 3rd slides with coloured rectangles representing AOI / Target words. A’, B’ and C’ are respectively 1st, 2nd and 3rd slides with embedded target words. Total number of words = 264.
Jemr 15 00001 g001aJemr 15 00001 g001b
Figure 2. Percentage of skipped target words. Note. S = Short words. M = Medium words. L = Long words. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency.
Figure 2. Percentage of skipped target words. Note. S = Short words. M = Medium words. L = Long words. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency.
Jemr 15 00001 g002
Table 1. Group mean for age and IQ.
Table 1. Group mean for age and IQ.
MeasuresControl
ni = 19
Dyslexia
ni = 21
ADHD-I
ni = 19
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age 9.260.158.950.129.050.18
Verbal IQ 102.83.798.93.083.52.9
Performance IQ 103.04.598.22.480.91.9
Full IQ 102.54.097.52.578.51.9
Note. SD = Standard deviation.
Table 2. Demographic characteristic of the sample.
Table 2. Demographic characteristic of the sample.
Sex
Control group niDyslexia niADHD-I ni
MaleFemaleMaleFemaleMaleFemale
4158111110
Table 3. Word classification according to their frequency and length.
Table 3. Word classification according to their frequency and length.
Stimuli
Length Short (S) [4 - 6] Letters
Medium (M) [7 - 10] Letters
Long (L) [11 - 14] Letters
Frequency Low (LF) 0 - 1000 Token
Medium (MF) 1001 - 10000 Token
Length x Frequency (S + LF) Corais (corals)
(S + MF) Equipa (team)
(M + LF) Marinhas (marine)
(M + MF) Conhecer (to know)
(L + LF) Mergulhadores (sea divers)
(L + MF) Investigação (research)
Table 4. WISC-III digit span subtest results.
Table 4. WISC-III digit span subtest results.
SubtestsGroupsMean (SD) X ˜ (Q3-Q1)Multiple con^aiisons
Digit Span Total Control 10.62 (2.43) Control group Dyslexia (p = 0.023)1,2
Control group ADHD-I (p = 0.027)1,2
Dyslexia 8.42 (2.06)
ADHD-I 7.40 (2.26)
Digit Span Forward Control 7.62 (1.12) Control group Dyslexia (p = 0.041)1,2
Control group ADHD-I (p = 0.008)1,2
Dyslexia 6.47 (1.31)
ADHD - I 6.20 (1.32)
Digit Span Backwards Control 5.00 (5.00 - 4.00)ADHD-I Control group (p = 0.042)3
Dyslexia 3.00 (5.00-3.00)
ADHD-I 3.00 (4.00 - 3.00)
Note. 1 ANOVA F statistic; 2 Tukey HSD; 3 Kruskal-Wallis independent samples test. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1= 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. X ˜ = Median.
Table 5. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: First Fixation Count as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Table 5. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: First Fixation Count as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Jemr 15 00001 i006
Note. S = Short word. M = Medium word. L = Long word. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency. K-W = Kruskal-Wallis test. X ˜ = Median. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1= 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. Superscripts indicate significant group difference; *p < 0.05.
Table 6. Median and Mean SFD duration (in milliseconds) on short x medium frequency target words in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Table 6. Median and Mean SFD duration (in milliseconds) on short x medium frequency target words in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
GroupsMedium frequency (MF)K-W
Short (S)
X ˜  (Q3–Q1)Mean (SD)
Control290.00 (343.75-234.75)301.04 (117.83)Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
Dyslexia375.50 (499.75-266.75)393.50 (189.14)
ADHD-I304.00 (386.75-229.00)329.05 (148.85)
Note. K-W = Kruskal-Wallis test. X ˜ = Median. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1 = 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. Superscripts indicate significant group difference (p < 0.05); * p < 0.00.
Table 7. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: First Pass Reading Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Table 7. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: First Pass Reading Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Jemr 15 00001 i007
Note. S = Short word. M = Medium word. L = Long word. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency. K-W = Kruskal-Wallis test. X ˜ = Median. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1 = 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. Superscripts indicate significant group difference; *p <0.05.
Table 8. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: Second Pass Reading Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Table 8. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and multiple comparison test: Second Pass Reading Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Jemr 15 00001 i008
Note. S = Short word. M = Medium word. L = Long word. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency. K-W = Kruskal-Wallis test. X ˜ = Median. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1 = 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. Superscripts indicate significant group difference; *p < 0.05.
Table 9. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples test: Total Fixation Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
Table 9. Median, 1st and 3rd percentiles, mean, standard deviation, and Kruskal-Wallis independent samples test: Total Fixation Time (in milliseconds) as a Function of Word Frequency and Word Length interaction in Dyslexia, Control and ADHD-I Groups.
IVGroups X ˜  (Q3–Q1)Mean (SD)K-S
S + LF Control 468.50 (738.00 – 316.75)519.30 (354.66)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 650.50 (1280.25 – 444.75)935.55 (722.85)
ADHD-I 642.50 (906.00 – 410.25)711.61 (526.91)
S + MF Control 344.50 (455.50 – 268.75)416.49 (227.62)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 536.00 (939.00 – 402.50)737.53 (524.42)
ADHD- I 485.00 (778.50 – 318.75)667.75 (559.83)
M + LF Control 684.50 (1122.50 – 468.00)854.46 (615.81)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 1266.00 (2148.75 – 676.00)1608.22 (1372.13)
ADHD- I 881.50 (1626.00 – 505.75)1274.68 (1057.19)
M + MF Control 588.50 (894.75 – 392.25)689.37 (563.80)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 1088.00 (1809.00 – 574.50)1384.97 (1345.41)
ADHD- I 730.00 (1343.00 – 440.25)991.35 (782.40)
L + LF Control 822.00 (1380.25 – 510.50)1007.97 (788.13)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 1553.50 (2873.25 – 788.00)1885.74 (1508.13)
ADHD- I 1175.00 (1925.00 – 504.25)1541.60 (1359.32)
L + MF Control 766.00 (1098.25 – 477.00)853.32 (614.04)Control group ≠ ADHD-I*
Control group ≠ Dyslexia*
DD 1189.00 (2190.75 – 588.50)1583.02 (1390.45)
ADHD- I 1003.00 (1732.00 – 542.75)1414.46 (1173.34)
Note. S = Short word. M = Medium word. L = Long word. LF = Low frequency. MF = Medium frequency. K-W = Kruskal-Wallis test. X ˜ = Median. Q3 = 3rd percentile. Q1= 1st percentile. SD = Standard deviation. Superscripts indicate significant group difference (p < 0.05); *p < 0 .005.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pereira, N.; Costa, M.A.; Guerreiro, M. Effects of Word Length and Word Frequency Among Dyslexic, Adhd-I and Typical Readers. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2022, 15, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.1.1

AMA Style

Pereira N, Costa MA, Guerreiro M. Effects of Word Length and Word Frequency Among Dyslexic, Adhd-I and Typical Readers. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2022; 15(1):1-27. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.1.1

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pereira, Norberto, Maria Armanda Costa, and Manuela Guerreiro. 2022. "Effects of Word Length and Word Frequency Among Dyslexic, Adhd-I and Typical Readers" Journal of Eye Movement Research 15, no. 1: 1-27. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.1.1

APA Style

Pereira, N., Costa, M. A., & Guerreiro, M. (2022). Effects of Word Length and Word Frequency Among Dyslexic, Adhd-I and Typical Readers. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 15(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.1.1

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop