Next Article in Journal
Negative versus Positive Priming: When Are Distractors Inhibited?
Previous Article in Journal
Role of Expectation and Working Memory Constraints in Hindi Comprehension: An Eye-tracking Corpus Analysis
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of Magnification and Contrast on Reading Performance in Different Types of Simulated Low Vision

by
Michael Christen
and
Mathias Abegg
Department of Ophthalmology, Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2017, 10(2), 1-9; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.2.5
Submission received: 3 January 2017 / Published: 16 May 2017

Abstract

:
Low vision therapy, such as magnifiers or contrast enhancement, is widely used. Scientific evidence proving its efficacy is scarce however. The objective of this study was to investi-gate whether the benefits of magnification and contrast enhancement depended on the origin of low vision. For this purpose we measured reading speed with artificially induced low vision in 12 healthy subjects in conditions of a simulated central scotoma, blurred vision and oscillopsia. Texts were either blurred, set in motion or blanked at the gaze position by using eye tracking and gaze contingent display. The simulated visual impairment was cali-brated such that all types of low vision caused equal reading impairment. We then tested the effect of magnification and contrast enhancement among the different types of low vision. We found that reading speed improved with increasing magnification and with higher con-trast in all conditions. The effect of magnification was significantly different in the three low vision conditions: The gain from magnification was highest in simulated blur and least in central scotoma. Magnification eventually led to near normal reading speed in all condi-tions. High contrast was less effective than high magnification and the effect of contrast enhancement was similar in all low vision conditions. From these results we conclude that the type of low vision determines the benefit that can be expected from magnification. Con-trast enhancement leads to similar improved reading speed in all low vision types. We pro-vide evidence that supports the use of low vision aids.

Introduction

Low vision is defined as an uncorrectable loss of vision that restricts affected patients in their everyday life. The International Classification of Diseases assumes moderate to severe vision impairment with visual acuity below 6/18 but better than 3/60 in the better eye with the best possible correction (World Health Organization, 1992). According to estimations of 2010, globally 191 million people suffer from low vision (Stevens et al., 2013). Major causes for vision loss are cataract, age related macular degeneration, diabetic retinopathy, glaucoma or refractive error (Bourne et al., 2014).
Reading impairment is a main complaint of patients suffering from low vision. Hence improving reading speed is the primary therapeutic goal (Elliott et al., 1997). Low vision therapy has a long-standing tradition and is widely provided. Its aim is it to enable and support low vision pa-tients in reading and other daily activities. In order to de-cide which low vision aids are beneficial for an individual patient, near and distance visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual field are taken into account. Usually however it is difficult to predict whether and how much an individual patient may benefit from low vision therapy (Latham & Tabrett, 2012; Legge, Ross, Isenberg, & LaMay, 1992; Whittaker & Lovie-Kitchin, 1993). This may be due to the scarce evidence from studies about the efficacy of low vi-sion treatment, which has been examined, with some ex-ceptions (Stelmack et al., 2008, 2016), in small observa-tional studies (Virgili, Acosta, Grover, Bentley, & Giaco-melli, 2013). Improvement of reading speed (Margrain, 2000; Nguyen, Weismann, & Trauzettel-Klosinski, 2009) and quality of life (Hinds et al., 2003; Lamoureux et al., 2007) could be found. In a randomized trial Stelmack et al showed that training and instruction enabled significantly better performance than provision of low vision aids alone (Gary S. Rubin, 2016; Stelmack et al., 2016). The great majority of studies focused on patients with macular dis-ease causing a central scotoma. However low vision may have a great variety of causes, which raises the question whether all kinds of low vision respond equally to low vi-sion support, irrespective of the cause.
To investigate this we compared the effect of magnifi-cation and contrast enhancement on reading performance in three different types of low vision. Measurements were performed in healthy subjects with a computer based sim-ulation of low vision diseases. This approach gave us the possibility to compare directly and within subjects the dif-ferent low vision types and furthermore we could avoid bias induced by comorbidities that may be found in real low vision patients. Low vision types tested in this study were simulated blurred vision, simulated oscillopsia and simulated central scotoma.

Methods

Subjects

Twelve healthy subjects with a mean age of 27 years (range 25-31 years) took part in the experiment. All were native German speakers. All subjects had a visual acuity equal or better than 20/20, tested at 0.5m. The study was conducted with approval of the local ethic committee Bern, Switzerland and all the subjects gave informed con-sent in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Experimental Setup

Reading texts were presented on a 21” CRT-monitor (ViewSonic G220fb) with a refresh rate of 100Hz and a resolution of 1024 to 768 pixels. Participants viewed stim-uli binocularly from a distance of 0.5m. One screen pixel corresponded thus to a visual angle of 0.045° (=146 sec-onds of arc). Eye movements were recorded using an Eye-Link 1000 eye-tracking system (SR Research, Missis-sauga, Canada) with a sampling rate of 2000Hz. To stabi-lize the head position a chin and forehead rest was used. On the chin rest a microphone was attached to record the participants’ voice.
To simulate a central scotoma, a white disc covering the reading text at the center of gaze was displayed (illus-trated in Figure 1). For this, we used the technique of gaze contingent display, which is a function of the Eye-Link1000 system (Sheldon, Abegg, Sekunova, & Barton, 2012; Simpson, Abegg, & Barton, 2011). To simulate blurred vision, texts were blurred with a mean filter from the software ImageJ 1.46 (Public domain, National Insti-tutes of Health, USA; Figure 1). The mean filter replaces each pixel value in an image with the mean value of all pixels within a given radius. To simulate oscillopsia, read-ing texts were set in motion with a sinusoidal movement (illustrated in Figure 1). The movement pattern consisted of two overlaid sine functions, one with an amplitude of 70 pixels horizontally and the other with an amplitude of 40 pixels vertically resulting in excursion of 140 pixels (6.4°) horizontally and 80 pixels (3.6°) vertically.

Experimental Protocol

The three different simulated low vision conditions were tested in separate experimental blocks. Between these blocks, the participants took a short break. Each block started with a calibration of the eye tracker using the default nine-point calibration procedure. The calibration was validated and only accepted if the eye tracking was not off by more than a mean of 0.5° and a maximum of 1°.
In every block the parameters of the low vision simu-lation were adjusted such that the visual acuity was equally reduced for all participants in all simulations. To achieve this we measured visual acuity with numbers of 14 pixels size, which corresponds to a Snellen visual acuity of about 20/100 in our conditions. We then worsened the low vision conditions stepwise until the participants could not cor-rectly identify at least 8 of 12 numbers within 30 seconds. We then used the parameters of the preceding step for the experiment. The central scotoma was adjusted by making the diameter of the disc bigger. A scotoma (disc) with a diameter of 0.5° was chosen as starting point and was then increased in step sizes of 0.5°. To calibrate the blurred vi-sion, the test started with a blurring radius of one pixel (146” seconds of arc). This radius was then increased in steps of one pixel. In the simulated oscillopsia, we in-creased the frequency of the sinusoidal movement: The in-itial-frequency was 0.4 Hz horizontal and 1.1 Hz vertical. For each step, the horizontal movement was increased by 0.2 Hz and the vertical movement was increased by 0.3 Hz. After the calibration of the eyetracker and the adjustment of the low vision simulation were successfully completed the participants had to read texts in 6 different magnifica-tions and 5 different contrast levels. The german texts were taken out of the novel ‘Die Blendung’ (Canetti, 2007). Each text passage had a mean length of 55 words (range 48 to 62) and a mean length of 291 characters (range 272 to 309). As font we used Times New Roman. All the texts were distributed over either nine or ten lines, no matter which magnification was used.
In a series of pilot experiments we determined the range of magnification such that we were not at a floor or ceiling effect (data not shown). For contrast we used the range possible with our CRT screen. To test the effect of magnification a scale with a step size of 0.1 log unit (base10) was used, resulting in font sizes of 14, 18, 22, 28, 35 and 44 pixels (Table 1). The magnification factor be-tween each level was Jemr 10 00012 i001 =1.2589 analogue to standard visual acuity charts (ISO, 2009). Font sizes were rounded to whole numbers. For the contrast levels the text was pre-sented in different shades of grey (reduced luminance) while the background was kept white (maximal lumi-nance). Five contrast levels of 4.9, 11.9, 27.4, 56.9 and 100% corresponding to contrast sensitivity of 0, 0.4, 0.8, 1.2 and 1.6 were chosen for the experiment (Table 1). Con-trast C was calculated by using following formula: C=10-S. The Michelson-equation Jemr 10 00012 i002 was used to deter-mine the ratio between the minimal and maximal lumi-nance given the contrast C: Jemr 10 00012 i003. The texts were saved as 8-bit greyscale images: In those 0 is defined as black and 255 is defined as white. Given Lmax=255 (white background), the text luminance Lmin was calculated by Jemr 10 00012 i004. All texts of the contrast levels were pre-sented at a font size of 18 pixels (0.81°), which corre-sponds to the second magnification level. The participants were instructed to read the text aloud and as fast and as correct as possible. Words should be repeated to correct mistakes. As soon as the last word was spoken, the time was manually stopped by the examiner by pressing a key. The maximum allowed time for one trial was 60 seconds, it was aborted after that. In order to counterbalance learn-ing and fatigue effects the order of the three simulations conditions were varied and the levels within one simula-tion were randomized. To prevent effects from variations in difficulties of the texts, they were allocated different to the testing levels for every participant. Additionally to the main experiment, we tested the reading speed without any visual impairment for the same texts with the same magni-fication and contrast levels in four of the twelve subjects. This test with normal vision was performed separately from the remaining experiment.

Analysis

For each trial, incorrect words (i.e. words that were not correctly read or not read within 60 seconds) were counted manually after the experiment by listening to the record-ings. The number of characters of these words were sub-tracted from the total number of characters of the text in order to get the number of correctly read characters in a given trial. The reading speed was then expressed in cor-rectly read characters per second cs:
11
For statistical analysis we used a linear mixed effects model with reading speed as dependent variable. Low vi-sion condition (central scotoma, oscillopsia, blur) and level of magnification (14, 18, 22, 28, 35, 44) or contrast (243, 226, 186, 110, 0) were used as independent variables. Subjects were used as random effects. To select between different fitting models (random-intercept, random-slope, or combined) we used Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) and chose the best model by the principle ‘smaller-is-better’. F-statistics, p-values and the Bayesian infor-mation criterion (BIC) are reported. Statistical significance was assumed if p<0.05. Analyses were performed using the MIXED procedure in SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 21).

Results

The calibration of the low vision condition was chosen such that a letter requiring a Snellen visual acuity of about 20/100 was just visible (see methods for details). To achieve this with a central scotoma, a blanked area with a diameter of 3.16° (range 2.5° – 4.0°) was necessary. For the same vision a blur radius of 329 seconds of arc (range 292 – 438 seconds of arc) was necessary. To induce the same reduction of vision a horizontal movement speed of 1.3Hz (range 1.0 – 1,6Hz) and a vertical movement speed of 2.45Hz (range 2.0 – 2.9Hz) was required. Next we measured reading speed and we found that in all types of simulated low vision reading speed improved with magni-fication (oscillopsia: F(5,55)=82, p<0.001, BIC=284; blur: F(5,55)=90, p<0.001, BIC=308; central scotoma: F(5,50)=55, p<0.001, BIC=236; figure 2) and in all low vision conditions reading speed improved with better con-trast (oscillopsia: F(4,44)=30, p<0.001, BIC=249; blur: F(4,44)=65,p<0.001, BIC=238; central scotoma: F(4,40)=16, p<0.001, BIC=213, figure 3). In contrast to subjects experiencing low vision, the reading speed in sub-jects with normal vision did neither change with contrast (F(4,12)=0.9, p=0.5, BIC=69) nor with magnification (F(5,15) = 2.4, p = 0.1, BIC=77). At the second but lowest magnification and contrast condition reading speed was similar in all three conditions (magnification: F(2,33)=0.1, p=0.9, BIC=167; contrast: F(2,33)=1.0, p=0.4, BIC=154), indicating that all types of low vision led to a comparable impairment of reading at this particular contrast and mag-nification. We found that the reading speed with the high-est magnification was similar to the reading speed meas-ured in normal viewing conditions (F(1,37)=3, p=0.1, BIC=177) and it was significantly better than the reading speed with the highest contrast (F(1,68)=72, p<0.001, BIC=328). The reading speed with the latter was signifi-cantly below the reading speed measured in normal view-ing conditions (F(3,35)=9.8, p<0.001, BIC=176).
Importantly we found a significant interaction term of magnification and low vision condition (F(2,193)=8.2, p<0.001,BIC=947), showing that the effect of magnifica-tion on reading is significantly different among the three conditions: Little increase in magnitude led to the most im-provement of reading speed if low vision was induced by blur, where the benefit plateaued at a font size of 28 pixels. In contrast, low vision induced by a central scotoma showed a more gradual increase of reading speed with in-creased magnitude, while the effect of magnification on low vision induced by text movement was at an intermedi-ate level. Unlike for magnification we found that the inter-action term of contrast and low vision condition showed no significance, indicating that the benefit of reading speed from enhanced contrast is not significantly different be-tween the three low vision conditions (F(2,158)=0.9, p=0.39, BIC=897).
Figure 2. The effect of magnification on reading speed. Six magnification levels were tested with normal vision (●), simulated blurred vision (▼), simulated oscillopsia (■) and simulated central scotoma (▲). Reading speed improved with magnification in all low vision conditions, while with normal vision reading speed remained at a maximum. The effect of magnification depended on the low vision type. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.
Figure 2. The effect of magnification on reading speed. Six magnification levels were tested with normal vision (●), simulated blurred vision (▼), simulated oscillopsia (■) and simulated central scotoma (▲). Reading speed improved with magnification in all low vision conditions, while with normal vision reading speed remained at a maximum. The effect of magnification depended on the low vision type. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.
Jemr 10 00012 g002
Figure 3. The effect of contrast enhancement on reading speed. Five contrast levels were tested with normal vision (●), simulated blurred vision (▼), simulated oscillopsia () and simulated central scotoma (▲). As for magnification, the reading speed improved with enhanced contrast in all low vision conditions but not for normal vision. The effect of enhanced contrast was not statistically significant between the different low vision conditions. The maximal reading speed with the highest contrast remained below the normal reading speed. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.
Figure 3. The effect of contrast enhancement on reading speed. Five contrast levels were tested with normal vision (●), simulated blurred vision (▼), simulated oscillopsia () and simulated central scotoma (▲). As for magnification, the reading speed improved with enhanced contrast in all low vision conditions but not for normal vision. The effect of enhanced contrast was not statistically significant between the different low vision conditions. The maximal reading speed with the highest contrast remained below the normal reading speed. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.
Jemr 10 00012 g003

Discussion

We found that artificially induced low vision led to re-duced reading speed that could be improved with both magnification and better contrast. We found a near normal reading speed with the highest magnification but not with the best contrast in all types of simulated low vision. Im-portantly we found that the effect of magnification signif-icantly depended on the type of low vision: The best effect of magnification was found for blur associated low vision, followed by low vision associated with text motion. The least effect was found for subjects with a simulated central scotoma. Contrast enhancement on the other hand, showed benefits that were independent from the type of vision loss.
Even though the reading speed with small text size was not different in the three conditions, the same increase in magnification led a bigger effect for blur associated low vision than for central scotoma and nystagmus. This ob-servation is to our knowledge unprecedented and the rea-sons for this are unknown. We speculate that the rate lim-iting factors for reading may be different in the three con-ditions and these factors may respond differently to mag-nification. For example, two point discrimination may be the rate limiting step in the blur condition, which prevents to differentiate the letter “U” from the letter “O” for ex-ample. A small increase in size will enable this differenti-ation and thus facilitate reading. In contrast to blur, the number of obscured letters may be critical for reading with a central scotoma. Possibly the magnification required two recognize more letters of a word is more than the magnifi-cation required to improve two point discrimination. Sim-ilar speculations could be made for oscillopsia and reading with nystagmus.
Where data is available, our results correspond well with findings from patients, where too a benefit on reading speed was found with magnification (Legge, Rubin, Pelli, & Schleske, 1985). While we found a near normal reading speed with the highest magnification for all low vision conditions, Legge et al. found near normal reading speed only in patients with intact central vision while patients with loss of central vision only reached a median maximal reading speed of 25 words/minute (Legge et al., 1985). Letters with font size between 12° and 24° were used in the latter condition, while the font size was about 2° in our study. Thus our results may only be applied to patients without loss of central vision as an optimal font size be-yond 2° is usually associated with reduced maximal read-ing speed (Legge & Bigelow, 2011). The finding of near normal reading speed with the highest simulated magnifi-cation in all simulated low vision types confirms our pre-vious finding of near normal reading speed in both patients with nystagmus and simulated nystagmus (Dysli & Abegg, 2016). In another study by Barot et. al. about reading speed in 71 patients with infantile nystagmus normal reading speed was found for most patients if texts were provided with optimal font size. Only the group of patients (n=12) with visual acuity below or equal 20/80 showed slightly reduced reading speed (Barot, McLean, Gottlob, & Proud-lock, 2013). Again, all these findings indicate that up on a certain amount of visual impairment reading speed is not limited if text is presented in optimal conditions (Legge et al., 1985).
Also with regard to contrast our data agree with data obtained from patients. We too found an improved reading speed with increased contrast as others had found in pa-tients (Giacomelli et al., 2010; G. S. Rubin & Legge, 1989). Data from patients showed that the effect of con-trast enhancement depended on individual contrast sensi-tivity and that it is independent of the type of low vision (G. S. Rubin & Legge, 1989). These clinical results corre-spond well to our finding of an absent effect of the type of low vision on contrast enhancement. Giacomelli et al. found that contrast reduction at a given text size led to more pronounced impairment of reading in patients with advanced low vision than in patients with mild low vision (Giacomelli et al., 2010), thus suggesting that contrast de-pends on the amount of visual loss. We found that contrast affected reading speed barely if no low vision was present. In a low vision situation, i.e. about 20/100, reduced con-trast affected reading significantly however. Both findings indicate that patients with low vision have a smaller band-width of contrast where optimal reading is possible than subjects with normal vision.
Simulating visual impairment, especially simulating central field loss with the technique of gaze contingent dis-play has been used by many researchers in the past. Re-search has been conducted on visual search with central scotoma (Geringswald & Pollmann, 2015; Walsh & Liu, 2014), visual sensitivity in peripheral vision (Johnson & Gurnsey, 2010) or on reading with central scotoma (Bow-ers, Woods, & Peli, 2004; Harvey & Walker, 2014). There is virtually no literature on the effect of magnification and contrast on reading in simulated low vision however. In one study of Fine & Rubin reading speed was measured with different magnifications in three subjects with simu-lated central scotoma, simulated cataract and both com-bined (Fine & Rubin, 1999). They found that if the two low vision conditions were combined, more magnification was needed to reach the same reading speed as when only one low vision condition was simulated. Similar to our re-sults, they also found that simulated cataract had almost no impact on reading performance with large letters.
The simulated low vision as used in the current paper has limitations which make a direct comparison with af-fected patients difficult. The simulated conditions differ from affected patients in several respects. First, in our con-ditions subjects are tested in an unadapted state, ie. sub-jects had no time to adjust to the situation. It is possible that reading speed in conditions of a central scotoma would improve over time, i.e. once subjects develop a new preferred retinal locus or learn to optimally use a pseudofovea (for an overview see for example (Lingnau, Thorsten, Schwarzbach, & Vorberg, 2014)). Second, we tested in a group of young subjects whereas affected pa-tients are usually older. Third, low vision from ophthalmic disease itself is not directly comparable to the simulated low vision. The shape of the central scotoma is not a circle in real life, a nystagmus has a motor component, which probably interacts with the sensory component of oscil-lopsia and blur in a patients may not be gaussian but may rather result from higher order optic aberrations. All this prevents a direct and quantitative comparison of our find-ings with data from patients. And yet we are convinced that the use of a simplified model allows correct qualitative data which also apply to patients and thus our main con-clusion that different causes for low vision respond differ-ently to low vision aids is valid. To prove this in a group of patients will be difficult though because different types of low vision are associated with different types of age, genetic background and co-morbidities thus making an in-vestigation of the low vision aid effect alone difficult.
Taken together, our data strongly support the use of magnification and contrast enhancement in all types of low vision. Our results suggest that for a given visual acuity and a given visual impairment the benefit of low vision therapy depends on the cause. This advocates measures that are adjusted to the origin of low vision, which today is achieved empirically (Latham & Tabrett, 2012; Legge et al., 1985).

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://bib-lio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publica-tion of this paper.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Swiss National Sci-ence Foundation.

References

  1. Barot, N., R. J. McLean, I. Gottlob, and F. A. Proudlock. 2013. Reading performance in infantile nystagmus. Ophthalmology 120, 6: 1232–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bourne, R. R. A., J. B. Jonas, S. R. Flaxman, J. Keeffe, J. Leasher, and K. Naidoo. 2014. Vision Loss Expert Group of the Global Burden of Disease Study. Preva-lence and causes of vision loss in high-income coun-tries and in Eastern and Central Europe: 1990–2010. The British Journal of Ophthalmology 98, 5: 629–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bowers, A. R., R. L. Woods, and E. Peli. 2004. Preferred retinal locus and reading rate with four dynamic text presentation formats. Optometry and Vision Science: Official Publication of the American Academy of Op-tometry 81, 3: 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Canetti, E. 2007. Die Blendung: Roman (37. Aufl). Fischer-Taschenbuch-Verl. [Google Scholar]
  5. Dysli, M., and M. Abegg. 2016. Nystagmus Does Not Limit Reading Ability in Albinism. PloS One 11, 7: e0158815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Elliott, D. B., M. Trukolo-Ilic, J. G. Strong, R. Pace, A. Plotkin, and P. Bevers. 1997. Demographic char-acteristics of the vision-disabled elderly. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 38, 12: 2566–2575. [Google Scholar]
  7. Fine, E. M., and G. S. Rubin. 1999. The effects of simu-lated cataract on reading with normal vision and sim-ulated central scotoma. Vision Research 39, 25: 4274–4285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Geringswald, F., and S. Pollmann. 2015. Central and pe-ripheral vision loss differentially affects contextual cueing in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psy-chology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41, 5: 1485–1496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Giacomelli, G., R. Volpe, G. Virgili, A. Farini, R. Arrighi, C. Tarli-Barbieri, and U. Menchini. 2010. Con-trast reduction and reading: assessment and reliability with the Reading Explorer test. European Journal of Ophthalmology 20, 2: 389–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Harvey, H., and R. Walker. 2014. Reading with periph-eral vision: A comparison of reading dynamic scroll-ing and static text with a simulated central scotoma. Vision Research 98: 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hinds, A., A. Sinclair, J. Park, A. Suttie, H. Paterson, and M. Macdonald. 2003. Impact of an interdiscipli-nary low vision service on the quality of life of low vision patients. The British Journal of Ophthalmol-ogy 87, 11: 1391–1396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. ISO. 2009. ISO 8596:2009-Ophthalmic optics--Vis-ual acuity testing--Standard optotype and its presen-tation. Available online: http://www.iso.org/iso/cata-logue_detail.htm?csnumber=52487.
  13. Johnson, A., and R. Gurnsey. 2010. Size scaling compen-sates for sensitivity loss produced by a simulated cen-tral scotoma in a shape-from-texture task. Journal of Vision 10, 12: 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lamoureux, E. L., J. F. Pallant, K. Pesudovs, G. Rees, J. B. Hassell, and J. E. Keeffe. 2007. The effective-ness of low-vision rehabilitation on participation in daily living and quality of life. Investigative Ophthal-mology & Visual Science 48, 4: 1476–1482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Latham, K., and D. R. Tabrett. 2012. Guidelines for pre-dicting performance with low vision AIDS. Optome-try and Vision Science: Official Publication of the American Academy of Optometry 89, 9: 1316–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Legge, G. E., and C. A. Bigelow. 2011. Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography. Journal of Vision 11, 5: 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Legge, G. E., J. A. Ross, L. M. Isenberg, and J. M. LaMay. 1992. Psychophysics of reading. Clinical predic-tors of low-vision reading speed. Investigative Oph-thalmology & Visual Science 33, 3: 677–687. [Google Scholar]
  18. Legge, G. E., G. S. Rubin, D. G. Pelli, and M. M. Schleske. 1985. Psychophysics of reading--II. Low vision. Vision Research 25, 2: 253–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lingnau, A., A. Thorsten, J. Schwarzbach, and D. Vorberg. 2014. Visual search without central vision–no single pseudofovea location is best. Journal of Eye Movement Research 7, 4: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Margrain, T. H. 2000. Helping blind and partially sighted people to read: the effectiveness of low vision aids. The British Journal of Ophthalmology 84, 8: 919–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nguyen, N. X., M. Weismann, and S. Trauzettel-Klosinski. 2009. Improvement of reading speed after provid-ing of low vision aids in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Acta Ophthalmologica 87, 8: 849–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rubin, G. S. 2016. Demonstrating the Effectiveness of Low-Vision Rehabilitation With Outcomes of the Veterans Affairs Low Vision Intervention Trial II (LOVIT II). JAMA Ophthalmology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rubin, G. S., and G. E. Legge. 1989. Psychophysics of reading. VI--The role of contrast in low vision. Vision Research 29, 1: 79–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Sheldon, C. A., M. Abegg, A. Sekunova, and J. J. S. Barton. 2012. The word-length effect in acquired alexia, and real and virtual hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 50, 5: 841–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Simpson, S. A., M. Abegg, and J. J. S. Barton. 2011. Rapid adaptation of visual search in simulated hemi-anopia. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. 1991) 21, 7: 1593–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Stelmack, J. A., X. C. Tang, D. J. Reda, S. Rinne, R. M. Mancil, R. W. Massof, and LOVIT Study Group. 2008. Outcomes of the Veterans Affairs Low Vision Intervention Trial (LOVIT). Archives of Oph-thalmology (Chicago, Ill.: 1960) 126, 5: 608–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Stelmack, J. A., X. C. Tang, Y. Wei, D. T. Wilcox, T. Mo-rand, K. Brahm, and LOVIT II Study Group. 2016. Outcomes of the Veterans Affairs Low Vision Intervention Trial II (LOVIT II): A Randomized Clin-ical Trial. JAMA Ophthalmology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stevens, G. A., R. A. White, S. R. Flaxman, H. Price, J. B. Jonas, J. Keeffe, and Vision Loss Expert Group. 2013. Global prevalence of vision impairment and blindness: magnitude and temporal trends, 1990-2010. Ophthalmology 120, 12: 2377–2384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Virgili, G., R. Acosta, L. L. Grover, S. A. Bentley, and G. Giacomelli. 2013. Reading aids for adults with low vision. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Re-views 10: CD003303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Walsh, D. V., and L. Liu. 2014. Adaptation to a simu-lated central scotoma during visual search training. Vision Research 96: 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Whittaker, S. G., and J. Lovie-Kitchin. 1993. Visual re-quirements for reading. Optometry and Vision Sci-ence: Official Publication of the American Academy of Optometry 70, 1: 54–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. World Health Organization. 1992. ICD-10 Ver-sion:2016. Geneva: World Health Organization. Available online: http://apps.who.int/classifica-tions/icd10/browse/2015/en#/H54.3.
Figure 1. Illustration of the low vision types. Left panel, shows simulated central scotoma, middle panel shows simulated blurred vision and right panel illustrates simulated oscillopsia. To ensure all types of simulated low vision lead to the same reduced visual acuity in all subjects, a calibration was performed with adjustments of the scotoma size, the blur radius and the frequency of the oscillopsia.
Figure 1. Illustration of the low vision types. Left panel, shows simulated central scotoma, middle panel shows simulated blurred vision and right panel illustrates simulated oscillopsia. To ensure all types of simulated low vision lead to the same reduced visual acuity in all subjects, a calibration was performed with adjustments of the scotoma size, the blur radius and the frequency of the oscillopsia.
Jemr 10 00012 g001
Table 1. Size and contrast characteristics of the tested levels.
Table 1. Size and contrast characteristics of the tested levels.
Magnification levelFont size [pixels]Font size [degree]Contrast C [%]Text luminance L
1140.6327.4185
2180.8127.4185
3220.9827.4185
4281.2527.4185
5351.5727.4185
6441.9727.4185
Contrast level
1180.814.9243
2180.8111.9225
3180.8127.4185
4180.8156.9110
5180.811000
The value of the text luminance is derived from an 8-bit image where 0 is defined as black and 255 is defined as white.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Christen, M.; Abegg, M. The Effect of Magnification and Contrast on Reading Performance in Different Types of Simulated Low Vision. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2017, 10, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.2.5

AMA Style

Christen M, Abegg M. The Effect of Magnification and Contrast on Reading Performance in Different Types of Simulated Low Vision. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2017; 10(2):1-9. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.2.5

Chicago/Turabian Style

Christen, Michael, and Mathias Abegg. 2017. "The Effect of Magnification and Contrast on Reading Performance in Different Types of Simulated Low Vision" Journal of Eye Movement Research 10, no. 2: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.2.5

APA Style

Christen, M., & Abegg, M. (2017). The Effect of Magnification and Contrast on Reading Performance in Different Types of Simulated Low Vision. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 10(2), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.2.5

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop