One, Two, Three, Four, Five, Once I Caught a Fish Alive: Numerical Phrases in Child and Adult Heritage Russian
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Setting the Stage
2.1. Numeral-Noun Expressions (NQPs) in Russian
(1) | a. | sideli | tri | zajčik-a |
sat | [three | bunny-paucal].nom | ||
b. | sideli | sem’ | zajčik-ov | |
sat | [seven | bunny-gen.pl].nom | ||
c. | s | tremja | zajčik-ami | |
with | [three | bunny]-ins | ||
d. | s | sem’ju | zajčik-ami | |
with | [seven | bunny]-ins |
(2) | a. | gorod | gorod-ov |
city.nom.sg | city-gen.pl | ||
b. | sosed | sosed-ej | |
neighbor.nom.sg | neighbor-gen.pl | ||
c. | čulok | čulok-∅ | |
stocking.nom.sg | stocking-gen.pl | ||
(3) | a. | oblak-o | oblak-ov |
cloud-nom.sg | cloud-gen.pl | ||
b. | mor-e | mor-ej | |
sea-nom.sg | sea-gen.pl | ||
c. | jablok-o | jablok-∅ | |
apple-nom.sg | apple-gen.pl |
2.2. Numeral-Noun Expressions in Hebrew
2.3. Acquisition of Numeral-Noun Expression in L1 and HL Russian
(4) | četyre | bymažek | (correct form: četyre | bymažki) | |
four | paper.f.gen.pl | four | paper.f.paucal | ||
(Gvozdev 1961, p. 299) |
3. Core Questions and Predictions
- (i)
- reliance on the default form: HL speakers will use the unmarked form (nominative plural) in place of the paucal and genitive plural
- (ii)
- frequency effect: HL speakers will overuse more frequent forms in non-target responses
- (iii)
- salience effect: in the marking of masculine genitive plural, HL speakers will overuse the endings -ov/-ax as most salient from the phonetic (perceptual) standpoint.
4. Experimental Study
4.1. Participants and Procedure
4.2. Materials
4.2.1. Lexical Proficiency
4.2.2. Experimental Task: Numeral-Noun Phrases (NQPs)
4.3. Results
5. Discussion
- (i)
- reliance on default forms, wherein HL speakers use the unmarked form (in this case, nominative plural) in place of the paucal and genitive plural;
- (ii)
- reliance on more frequent forms, wherein HL speakers overuse more frequent forms as non-target responses, i.e., they replace the less frequent genitive plural form with the more frequent paucal form
- (iii)
- reliance on perceptually more salient forms: in the marking of the genitive plural, HL speakers will overuse the endings -ov/-ax as most salient.
6. Conclusions and Prospects for Future Research
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | The effect of the frequency of a numeral is observed in the choice of singular vs. plural marking on verbs; lower numerals, which tend to be more frequent, are associated with the plural, while higher numerals, which tend to be less frequent, are associated with the singular (Nesset and Janda 2023). |
References
- Akinina, Yulia, Svetlana Malyutina, Maria Ivanova, Ekaterina Iskra, Elena Mannova, and Olga Dragoy. 2015. Russian normative data for 375 action pictures and verbs. Behavioral Research Methods 47: 691–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albirini, Abdulkafi, Elabbas Benmamoun, and Brahim Chakrani. 2013. Gender and number agreement in the oral production of Arabic heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersen, Henning. 2006. Some thoughts on the history of Russian numeral syntax. Harvard Ukrainian Studies 28: 57–67. [Google Scholar]
- Antonova Ünlü, Elena, and Wei Li. 2018. The acquisition of the weaker language: Evidence from the acquisition of Russian cases by a Turkish-Russian child. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8: 637–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Babby, Leonard H. 1987. Case, prequantifiers, and discontinuous agreement in Russian. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 5: 91–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bailyn, John F., and Andrew Nevins. 2008. Russian genitive plurals are impostors. In Inflectional Identity. Edited by Asaf Bachrach and Andrew Nevins. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 237–70. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benmamoun, Elabbas, Silvina Montrul, and Maria Polinsky. 2013. Heritage languages and their speakers: Opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 39: 129–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daskalaki, Evangelia, Blom Elma, Vasiliki Chondrogianni, and Johanne Paradis. 2020. Effects of parental input quality in child heritage language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 47: 709–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daskalaki, Evangelia, Vasiliki Chondrogianni, Elma Blom, Froso Argyri, and Johanne Paradis. 2019. Input effects across domains: The case of Greek subjects in child heritage language. Second Language Research 35: 421–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franks, Steven. 1995. Parameters of Slavic Morphosyntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Fridman, Clara, Adina Livni, Sagit Bar On, and Natalia Meir. 2024. Modeling lexical abilities of heritage language and L2 speakers of Hebrew and English in Israel and the United States: A network approach. Frontiers in Psychology 15: 1331801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fridman, Clara, and Natalia Meir. 2023. Lexical production and innovation in child and adult Russian Heritage speakers dominant in English and Hebrew. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 26: 880–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fridman, Clara, Maria Polinsky, and Natalia Meir. 2023. Cross-linguistic influence meets diminished input: A comparative study of heritage Russian in contact with Hebrew and English. Second Language Research. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gagarina, Natalia, and Maria Voeikova. 2009. Acquisition of case and number in Russian. In Development of Nominal Inflection in First Language Acquisition: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Edited by Ursula Stephany and Maria D. Voeikova. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 179–215. [Google Scholar]
- Gerasimova, Anastasia, and Ekaterina Lyutikova. 2018. Soglasuemye i substantsirovannye prilagatel’nye v russkix konstrukcijax s malymi čislitel’nymi: èksperimental’noje issledovanije [Agreeing and substantivized adjectives in Russian constructions with small numerals: An experimental study]. Rema 4: 49–70. [Google Scholar]
- Gonen, Einat, and Doron Rubinstein. 2015. Gender neutralization in Hebrew: The case of the numerals in colloquial Hebrew. Brill’s Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 7: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grashchenkov, Pavel V. 2002. Roditel’nyj padež pri russkix čislitel’nyx: Tipologičeskoje rešenije odnoj “sugubo vnutrennej” problemy [Genitive case with Russian numerals: A typological solution to a “purely internal” problem]. Voprosy yazykoznaniya 3: 74–119. [Google Scholar]
- Gvozdev, Aleksandr. 1961. Voprosy izučenija detskoj reči. Saint Petersburg: Detstvo-Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hopp, Holger, and Michael T. Putnam. 2015. Syntactic restructuring in heritage grammars: Word order variation in Moundridge Schweitzer German. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5: 180–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ionin, Tania, and Ora Matushansky. 2006. The composition of complex cardinals. Journal of semantics 23: 315–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ionin, Tania, and Ora Matushansky. 2018. Cardinals: The Syntax and Semantics of Cardinal-Containing Expressions. Cambridge: MIT Press, vol. 79. [Google Scholar]
- Isurin, Ludmila, and Tanya Ivanova-Sullivan. 2008. Lost in between: The case of Russian heritage speakers. Heritage Language Journal 6: 72–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kasatkin, Leonid Leonidovič, ed. 2005. Russkaja dialektologija [Russian Dialectology]. Moscow: Academia. [Google Scholar]
- Kholodionova, Svetlana. 2019. Stilističeskaja xarakteristika variantnyx form imeni čislitel’nogo [Stylistic Characteristics of Variational Forms of Numerals]. Aktual’nye voprosy filologičeskix issledovanij 107: 107–12. [Google Scholar]
- Kornishova, Dana. 2010. First Language Acquisition of Russian Numeral Expressions. Essex: University of Essex, Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Kupisch, Tanja, and Maria Polinsky. 2022. Language history on fast forward: Innovations in heritage languages and diachronic change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 25: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laleko, Oksana, and Yana Miroshnychenko. 2022. Grammars in contact: A linguistic study of Russian in Brighton Beach, New York. Russian Language Journal 72: 10. Available online: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/rlj/vol72/iss1/10 (accessed on 30 June 2024).
- López, Luis. 2020. Case, concord and the emergence of default. Languages 5: 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyskawa, Paulina. 2021. Coordination without grammar-internal feature resolution. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Łyskawa, Paulina, and Naomi Nagy. 2020. Case marking variation in heritage Slavic languages in Toronto: Not so different. Language Learning 70: 122–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez-Nieto, Lourdes, and Maria Adelaida Restrepo. 2023. Grammatical gender in Spanish child heritage speakers: Incomplete or different acquisition? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 13: 267–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meir, Irit. 2015. Numerals: Modern Hebrew. In Encyclopedia of Hebrew Language and Linguistics. Edited by Geoffrey Khan. Leiden: Brill, pp. 903–8. [Google Scholar]
- Meir, Natalia, and Bibi Janssen. 2021. Child heritage language development: An interplay between cross-linguistic influence and language-external factors. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 651730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meir, Natalia, and Maria Polinsky. 2021. Restructuring in heritage grammars: Adjective-noun and numeral-noun expressions in Israeli Russian. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 11: 222–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meir, Natalia, Avramenko Marina, and Verkhovtceva Tatiana. 2021. Israeli Russian: Case morphology in a bilingual context. Russian Journal of Linguistics 25: 886–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mel’čuk, Igor. 1985. Poverxnostnyj sintaksis russkix čislovyx vyraženij [Surface Syntax of Russian Numeral Expressions]. Wien: Wiener Slawistischer Almanach. [Google Scholar]
- Mel’čuk, Igor. 2024. Nepričesannye mysli po povodu “Akadmičeskoj grammatiki soveremennogo russkogo jazyka”: Na primere čislenno-imennyx slovosočetanij [Unfinished Thoughts on the ‘Academic Grammar of the Modern Russian Language’: Using Numeral-Nominal Phrases as an Example]. Russian Linguistics 48: 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Mikulski, Ariana M. 2010. Age of onset of bilingualism, language use, and the volitional subjunctive in heritage learners of Spanish. Heritage Language Journal 7: 28–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitrofanova, Natalia, Yulia Rodina, Olga Urek, and Marit Westergaard. 2018. Bilinguals’ sensitivity to grammatical gender cues in Russian: The role of cumulative input, proficiency, and dominance. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2016. The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Nesset, Tore. 2020. A long birth: The development of gender-specific paucal constructions in Russian. Diachronica 37: 514–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nesset, Tore, and Laura A. Janda. 2023. A network of allostructions: Quantified subject constructions in Russian. Cognitive Linguistics 34: 67–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pesetsky, David. 2013. Russian Case Morphology and the Syntactic Categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, vol. 66. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, Maria. 2006. Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 14: 191–262. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/24599616 (accessed on 30 June 2024).
- Polinsky, Maria. 2008. Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal 6: 40–71. Available online: http://www.heritagelanguages.org (accessed on 30 June 2024).
- Polinsky, Maria. 2011. Reanalysis in adult heritage language: New evidence in support of attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33: 305–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polinsky, Maria. 2018. Heritage Languages and Their Speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, Maria, and Gregory Scontras. 2020a. A roadmap for heritage language research. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 50–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polinsky, Maria, and Gregory Scontras. 2020b. Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2021. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Rappaport, Gilbert C. 2002. Numeral phrases in Russian: A minimalist approach. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 10: 327–40. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/24599687 (accessed on 30 June 2024).
- Ravid, Dorit. 1995. Neutralization of gender distinctions in Modern Hebrew numerals. Language Variation and Change 7: 79–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rothman, Jason. 2009. Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism 13: 155–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scontras, Gregory, and Michael T. Putnam. 2020. Lesser-studied heritage languages: An appeal to the dyad. Heritage Language Journal 17: 152–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slioussar, Natalia, and Maria Samoilova. 2015. Frequencies of different grammatical features and inflectional affixes in Russian nouns: A database. Available online: http://www.slioussar.ru/freqdatabase.html (accessed on 30 June 2024).
- Unsworth, Sharon, Froso Argyri, Leonie Cornips, Aafke Hulk, Antonella Sorace, and Ianthi Tsimpli. 2014. The role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics 35: 765–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Voeikova, Maria. 2007. Usvoenie det’mi kategorii količestvennosti [Acquisition of the number category by childre]. In Semantičeskie kategorii v detskoj reči. Edited by S. N. Cejtlin. Saint Petersburg: Nestor-Istoria, pp. 266–99. [Google Scholar]
- Voeikova, Maria. 2011. Rannije etapy usvojenija det’mi imennoj morfologii russkogo jazyka [Early Stages of Child Acquisition of Nominal Forms of the Russian Language]. Moscow: Znak. [Google Scholar]
- Xiang, Ming, Boris Harizanov, Maria Polinsky, and Ekaterina Kravtchenko. 2011. Processing morphological ambiguity: An experimental investigation of Russian numerical phrases. Lingua 121: 548–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaliznyak, Andrey A. 1967. Russkoe imennoe slovoizmenenie [Russian Nominal Inflection]. Moscow: Nauka. [Google Scholar]
- Zaliznyak, Andrey A. 1980. Grammatičeskij slovar’ russkogo jazyka [Grammar Dictionary of the Russian Language]. Moscow: Russkij jazyk. [Google Scholar]
- Zaliznyak, Andrey A. 1987. Istoričeskaja grammatika russkogo jazyka [Historical Grammar of the Russian Language]. Moscow: Nauka. [Google Scholar]
Odin ‘one’ | Paucal (1.5, 2–4) | Plural (5 and Above) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Masculine | odin | stol | tri | stol-a | pjat’ | stol-ov |
one | table | three | tables | five | tables | |
Neuter | odno | okn-o | tri | okn-a | pjat’ | okon-∅ |
one | window | three | windows | five | windows | |
Feminine | odna | knig-a | tri | knig-i | pjat’ | knig-∅ |
one | book | three | books | five | books |
‘one’ | Plural | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Masculine | sefer | axad | šloš-a | sfar-im |
book | one | three | books | |
Feminine | siml-a | exat | šaloš | smal-ot |
dress | one | three | dresses |
Hypothesis | Predictions |
---|---|
H1: Null hypothesis | All four groups perform the same |
H2: Uniform divergence | Both bilingual groups perform less-target like than monolinguals; no difference between child and adult bilinguals |
H3: Divergence + acquisition lag | Adult bilinguals perform more target-like than child bilinguals |
H4: Divergence + restructuring and attrition over the lifespan | Child bilinguals perform more target-like than adult bilinguals |
HL-Russian Speakers | Monolingual Russian Speakers | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
HL-Child (n = 27) | HL-Adult (n = 37) | MO-Child (n = 20) | MO-Adult (n = 21) | |
Age | 6 (1) | 26 (4) | 6 (1) | 40 (14) |
Sex | 19f/8m | 23f/14m | 10f/10m | 18f/3m |
AoB | 2 (2) | 2 (2) | n/a | n/a |
Masculine | Feminine | |
---|---|---|
Paucal | 4 šarf-a 4 scarves.M-PAUC | 3 ložk-i 3 spoons.F-PAUC |
Plural | 5 samoljot-ov 5 airplanes.M-GEN.PL | 6 kartin 6 paintings.F.GEN.PL |
Predictors | Estimates | SE | CI | Statistic | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
▪ (Intercept) | 1.00 | 0.04 | 0.92–1.08 | 24.38 | <0.001 |
▪ Group [MO-Child] | −0.03 | 0.06 | −0.15–0.08 | −0.53 | 0.595 |
▪ Group [HL-Child] | −0.43 | 0.05 | −0.54–−0.32 | −7.85 | <0.001 |
▪ Group [HL-Adult] | −0.22 | 0.05 | −0.32–−0.12 | −4.30 | <0.001 |
Random Effects | |||||
▪ σ2 | 0.05 | ||||
▪ τ00 Code | 0.02 | ||||
▪ ICC | 0.30 | ||||
▪ N Code | 105 | ||||
▪ Observations | 420 | ||||
▪ Marginal R2/Conditional R2 | 0.267/0.486 |
Condition | Non-Target Inflection Used | Non-Target Response | HL-Child | HL-Adult | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | |||
FEM-PAUC | -ok | 3 šapok/gorok/ložek | 20 | 33% | 24 | 60% |
-ov | 3 šapkov/bankov | 19 | 32% | 2 | 5% | |
-a | 3 šapka/gorka/kniga | 8 | 13% | 4 | 10% | |
-ax | 3 šapkax/knigax | 7 | 12% | 1 | 3% | |
zero | 3 knig/gruš | 6 | 10% | 9 | 23% | |
other | 3 knigim (Hebrew pl.maker) | 4 | 7% | |||
FEM-PLUR | -y/i | 5 šapki/butylki/banki | 30 | 45% | 41 | 80% |
-ov | 5 šapkov/sobačkov | 22 | 33% | 3 | 6% | |
-a | 5 šapka/lopata | 7 | 11% | 4 | 8% | |
-ax | 5 šapkax/sobakax | 5 | 8% | 3 | 6% | |
other | 5 šakot/šapkim (Hebrew pl.maker) | 2 | 3% | |||
MASC-PAUC | -ov | 3 šarfov/domov/šakfov | 44 | 51% | 16 | 43% |
-y/i | 3 šarfy/chemodany | 18 | 21% | 18 | 48% | |
zero | 3 šarf/stol/arbuz | 16 | 19% | 3 | 8% | |
-ax | 3 šarfax | 4 | 5% | |||
other | 3 šarfim (Hebrew pl.maker) | 4 | 5% | |||
MASC-PLUR | -y/i | 5 šarfy/pakety/arbuzy | 31 | 51% | 35 | 56% |
zero | 5 šarf/samolet/čemodan | 14 | 23% | 11 | 17% | |
-ax | 5 šarfax/paketax/krokodilax | 9 | 15% | 3 | 5% | |
-a | 5 šarfa | 4 | 7% | 14 | 22% | |
other | 5 šarfim (Hebrew pl.maker) | 3 | 5% |
Predictors | Odds Ratios | SE | CI | Statistic | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.08 | 0.02 | 0.05–0.12 | −11.41 | <0.001 |
Group [HL-Child vs. HL-Adult] | 1.31 | 0.19 | 1.00–1.73 | 1.93 | 0.053 |
AoB | 1.11 | 0.04 | 1.02–1.20 | 2.58 | 0.010 |
Lexical Proficiency (Noun production) | 1.11 | 0.01 | 1.10–1.13 | 12.52 | <0.001 |
Observations | 1487 | ||||
R2 | 0.208 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Meir, N.; Polinsky, M. One, Two, Three, Four, Five, Once I Caught a Fish Alive: Numerical Phrases in Child and Adult Heritage Russian. Languages 2024, 9, 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080261
Meir N, Polinsky M. One, Two, Three, Four, Five, Once I Caught a Fish Alive: Numerical Phrases in Child and Adult Heritage Russian. Languages. 2024; 9(8):261. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080261
Chicago/Turabian StyleMeir, Natalia, and Maria Polinsky. 2024. "One, Two, Three, Four, Five, Once I Caught a Fish Alive: Numerical Phrases in Child and Adult Heritage Russian" Languages 9, no. 8: 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080261
APA StyleMeir, N., & Polinsky, M. (2024). One, Two, Three, Four, Five, Once I Caught a Fish Alive: Numerical Phrases in Child and Adult Heritage Russian. Languages, 9(8), 261. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080261