An Airflow Analysis of Spanish and English Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization among Heritage Bilinguals
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
2.1. Theoretical Framework
2.2. Heritage Speaker’s Phonetics and Phonology
2.3. Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization across Languages
3. Research Questions
- RQ 1: Can heritage speakers acquire distinct time setting patterns of nasality in /VN/ structures of English and Spanish?
- Hypothesis 1: Yes. According to Martínez (2021) and Beristain (2022, forthcoming), I expect that HL speakers will show the ability to adjust their two timing settings in relation to /VN/ structures. These participants have an early language acquisition onset and have long been using both languages, and are therefore expected to show distinct categories for nasalized vowels in Spanish and English. The fact that anticipatory nasalization is phonologized in English and phonetic in Spanish should allow for this differentiation to happen more easily. It is expected that the gestural overlap of English /VN/ sequences will be greater than those of Spanish, thus resulting in higher proportion of nasal airflow in the former.
- RQ 2: Is there correlation between degree of bilingualism and degree of anticipatory vowel nasalization?
- Hypothesis 2: Yes. Considering Amengual’s (2018, 2019) findings, speakers showing a more ‘balanced’ bilingualism should exhibit more distinct results between the two languages. The more dominant a heritage speaker is in one language, the greater shift towards that language’s expected patterns we should encounter. That is, participants that show higher dominance scores, thus being more English-dominant, will exhibit a greater degree of nasalization overall, whereas speakers that present lower dominance scores, thus being more Spanish-dominant, will display a lower overall degree of nasalization.
4. Methodology
4.1. Participants
4.2. Materials
4.3. Equipment
4.4. Data Collection and Procedure
4.5. Data Analysis
4.5.1. Signal Processing
4.5.2. Statistical Analysis
5. Results
5.1. RQ1 Cross-Linguistic Time Settings Adjustment: Group Analysis
5.2. RQ2 Effect of Degree of Bilingualism: Individual-Centric Analysis
6. Discussion
7. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Speaker | Sex | History | Use | Proficiency | Attitudes | BLP Score | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
weight = 0.454 | weight = 1.09 | weight = 2.27 | weight = 2.27 | [−218, 218] | ||||||
EN | SP | EN | SP | EN | SP | EN | SP | |||
65 | F | 42.67 | 26.33 | 49.05 | 5.45 | 54.45 | 22.7 | 54.48 | 49.94 | 96.26 |
8 | M | 43.13 | 27.24 | 47.96 | 6.54 | 54.48 | 29.51 | 54.48 | 43.13 | 93.63 |
53 | M | 42.67 | 24.51 | 49.05 | 5.45 | 54.48 | 52.21 | 54.48 | 49.94 | 68.57 |
26 | M | 43.13 | 21.79 | 44.69 | 9.81 | 54.48 | 47.67 | 47.67 | 49.94 | 60.75 |
52 | M | 48.12 | 10.89 | 34.88 | 14.17 | 54.48 | 52.21 | 49.94 | 54.48 | 55.66 |
30 | M | 37.22 | 27.24 | 39.24 | 15.26 | 54.48 | 36.32 | 43.13 | 49.94 | 45.31 |
20 | M | 42.22 | 34.95 | 37.06 | 17.44 | 49.94 | 40.86 | 43.13 | 52.21 | 26.88 |
42 | F | 30.87 | 29.05 | 41.42 | 13.08 | 54.48 | 52.21 | 45.4 | 52.21 | 25.61 |
55 | M | 32.68 | 28.60 | 39.24 | 15.26 | 54.48 | 40.86 | 34.05 | 52.21 | 23.52 |
3 | M | 29.96 | 30.87 | 38.15 | 16.35 | 54.48 | 52.21 | 45.4 | 54.58 | 14.08 |
27 | F | 25.88 | 37.68 | 42.51 | 11.99 | 47.67 | 43.13 | 36.32 | 52.21 | 7.37 |
72 | F | 36.77 | 39.96 | 35.97 | 18.53 | 54.48 | 40.86 | 11.35 | 49.94 | −0.72 |
1 | F | 24.51 | 36.77 | 37.06 | 18.53 | 45.4 | 45.4 | 40.86 | 49.94 | −2.80 |
54 | F | 38.13 | 39.95 | 33.79 | 27.25 | 45.4 | 45.4 | 40.86 | 52.21 | −6.62 |
6 | F | 19.06 | 42.22 | 34.88 | 20.71 | 52.21 | 54.48 | 36.32 | 47.67 | −22.60 |
58 | F | 15.89 | 39.95 | 26.16 | 30.52 | 43.13 | 54.48 | 20.43 | 49.94 | −69.28 |
1 | It needs to be mentioned that the author of the current study is a native speaker of Spanish and was the main and only person in charge when the experiment occurred in Spanish. When the experimental session was in English, two of the author’s six research assistants conducted the sessions. They were all native speakers of Chicagoland English, which was the variety spoken by the participants as well. |
2 | This is the highest sample rate that is allowed in AcqKnowledge. No further analyses were conduced in the acoustic data; it was only used for segmentation purposes to add an additional layer of veracity to segment boundaries. |
3 | The misalignment between the voicing band, F0, and audio signal could be due to the presence of the mask participants were wearing. |
4 | The range of the dependent variable is 0–1, with 1 representing 100% nasal airflow proportion. Thus, if the nasal proportion is 0.25, this would equal 25% of nasal airflow proportion out of the total airflow produced. |
5 | Recently, Bongiovanni (2021a) has found that it is in velarizing varieties such as Dominican Spanish where anticipatory vowel nasalization has been phonologized, but not in non-velarizing ones, such as the one under study in the current paper. |
References
- Akaike, Hirotsugu. 1974. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control 19: 716–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amengual, Mark. 2012. Interlingual influence in bilingual speech: Cognate status effect in a continuum of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 517–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amengual, Mark. 2018. Asymmetrical interlingual influence in the production of Spanish and English laterals as a result of competing activation in bilingual language processing. Journal of Phonetics 69: 12–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amengual, Mark. 2019. Type of early bilingualism and its effect on the acoustic realization of allophonic variants: Early sequential and simultaneous bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 23: 954–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlaz, Marissa S., Ryan K. Shosted, Maojing Fu, and Brad Sutton. 2018. Oropharyngeal articulation of phonemic and phonetic nasalization in Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Phonetics 71: 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers, and Harry J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68: 255–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beristain, Ander. 2022. The Acquisition of Acoustic and Aerodynamic Patterns of Coarticulation in Second and Heritage Languages. Doctoral thesis, University Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Beristain, Ander. forthcoming. Gestural timing patterns of nasality in Spanish learners of English: Aerodynamic evidence. Language and Speech, under review.
- BIOPAC Systems Inc. 2020. AcqKnowledge Data Acquisition and Analysis Software. version 3.9.1. Goleta: BIOPAC Hardware Systems, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Birdsong, David P., Libby M. Gertken, and Mark Amengual. 2012. Bilingual Language Profile: An Easy-to-Use Instrument to Assess Bilingualism; COERLL, University of Texas at Austin. Available online: https://sites.la.utexas.edu/bilingual/ (accessed on 15 April 2020).
- Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2020. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer, Version 6.1.10; Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 25 March 2020).
- Bongiovanni, Silvina. 2021a. Acoustic investigation of anticipatory vowel nasalization in a Caribbean and a non-Caribbean dialect of Spanish. Linguistics Vanguard 7: 20200008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bongiovanni, Silvina. 2021b. On covariation between nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization: Evidence from a Caribbean and a non-Caribbean dialect of Spanish. Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology 12: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradlow, Ann R. 1995. A comparative acoustic study of English and Spanish vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97: 1916–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Browman, Catherine P., and Louis Goldstein. 1992. Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica 49: 155–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carignan, Christopher, Ryan K. Shosted, Chilin Shih, and Panying Rong. 2011. Compensatory articulation in American English nasalized vowels. Journal of Phonetics 39: 668–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casillas, Joseph V., and Miquel Simonet. 2016. Production and perception of the English /æ/–/ɑ/ contrast in switched-dominance speakers. Second Language Research 32: 171–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chafcouloff, Michel, and Alain Marchal. 1999. Velopharingeal coarticulation. In Coarticulation: Theory, Data and Techniques. Edited by William J. Hardcastle and Nigel Hewlett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 69–79. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, Charles B. 2021. Phonetics and phonology of heritage languages. In The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages and Linguistics. Edited by Silvina Montrul and Maria Polinsky. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 581–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, Andrew. 2021. Maintenance of phonetic and phonological distance in the English and Korean back vowel contours of heritage bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics 89: 101109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clumeck, Harold. 1976. Patterns of soft palate movements in six languages. Journal of Phonetics 4: 337–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colantoni, Laura, Alexei Kochetov, and Jeffrey Steele. 2021. Articulatory settings and L2 English coronal consonants. Phonetica 78: 273–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desmeules-Trudel, Félix, and Marc Brunelle. 2018. Phonotactic restrictions condition the realization of vowel nasality and nasal coarticulation: Duration and airflow measurements in Québécois French and Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Phonetics 69: 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farnetani, Edda. 1999. Labial coarticulation. In Coarticulation: Theory, Data and Techniques. Edited by William J. Hardcastle and Nigel Hewlett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 144–63. [Google Scholar]
- Flege, James E. 1995. Second language speech learning. Theory, findings, and problems. In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Theoretical and Methodological Issues in Cross-Language Speech Research. Edited by Winifred Strange. Baltimore: York Press, pp. 233–77. [Google Scholar]
- Flege, James E., and Ocke-Schwen Bohn. 2021. The Revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress. Edited by Ratree Wayland. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fletcher, Janet, and Jonathan Harrington. 1999. Lip and jaw coarticulation. In Coarticulation: Theory, Data and Techniques. Edited by William J. Hardcastle and Nigel Hewlett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 164–78. [Google Scholar]
- Goodin-Mayeda, C. Elizabeth. 2016. Nasals and Nasalization in Spanish and Portuguese: Perception, Phonetics, and Phonology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, François. 2008. Studying Bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Honikman, Batrice. 1964. Articulatory settings. In In Honour of Daniel Jones. Edited by David Abercrombie, Dennis Butler Fry, Peter MacCarthy, Norman C. Scott and John L. M. Trim. London: Longman, pp. 73–84. [Google Scholar]
- Hualde, José I. 2014. Los Sonidos del Español. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Ji-Young. 2019. Heritage speakers’ use of prosodic strategies in focus marking in Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism 23: 986–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kochetov, Alexei. 2020. Research methods in articulatory phonetics II: Studying other gestures and recent trends. Language and Linguistics Compass 14: e12371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per Bruun Brockhoff, Rune Haubo Bojesen Christensen, and Sogie Pødenphant Jensen. 2015. lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Version: 2.0-25. Available online: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html (accessed on 15 April 2020).
- Martínez, Ruth M. 2021. The Bidirectional Acquisition of Oral and Nasal Gestural Timing in L2 Brazilian Portuguese and Rioplatense Spanish Nasal Structures. Doctoral thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- MATLAB. 2020. 9.8.0.1323502 (R2020a). Natick: The MathWorks Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2008. Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-Examining the Age Factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2015. The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polinsky, Maria, and Gregory Scontras. 2020. Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Ian Cunnings, Fatih Bayram, David Miller, Susagna Tubau, and Jason Rothman. 2018. Language dominance affects bilingual performance and processing outcomes in adulthood. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 15 April 2020).
- R Studio Team. 2020. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Available online: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed on 15 April 2020).
- Rao, Rajiv. 2014. On the status of the phoneme /b/ in heritage speakers of Spanish. Sintagma: Revista de lingüística 26: 37–54. [Google Scholar]
- Recasens, Daniel. 1999. Lingual coarticulation. In Coarticulation: Theory, Data and Techniques. Edited by William J. Hardcastle and Nigel Hewlett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 80–104. [Google Scholar]
- Romano, Francesco. 2021. L1 versus dominant language transfer effects in L2 and heritage speakers of Italian: A structural priming study. Applied Linguistics 42: 945–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ronquest, Rebecca. 2013. A acoustic examination of unstressed vowel reduction in heritage Spanish. In Selected Proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Edited by Chad Howe, Sarah Blackwell and Margaret Lubbers Quesada. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 151–71. [Google Scholar]
- Ronquest, Rebecca. 2016. Stylistic variation in heritage Spanish vowel production. Heritage Language Journal 13: 275–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sampson, Rodney. 1999. Nasal Vowel Evolution in Romance. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shosted, Ryan K. 2009. The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives: An Instrumental Study in Phonetic Typology. Saarbrucken: VDM Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Shosted, Ryan K., Christopher Carignan, and Panying Rong. 2012. Managing the distinctiveness of phonemic nasal vowels: Articulatory evidence from Hindi. The Journal of Acoustical Society of America 131: 455–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solé, Maria-Josep. 1992. Phonetic and phonological processes: The case of nasalization. Language and Speech 35: 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, Ian, and Bryan Gick. 2014. Bilinguals use language-specific articulatory settings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57: 361–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zellou, Georgia, and Ioana Chitoran. 2023. Lexical competition influences coarticulatory variation in French: Comparing competition from nasal and oral vowel minimal pairs. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 8: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Speaker | BLP Score | |
---|---|---|
[−218, 218] | ||
65 | 96.26 | (English-dominant) |
8 | 93.63 | |
53 | 68.57 | |
26 | 60.75 | |
52 | 55.66 | |
30 | 45.31 | |
20 | 26.88 | |
42 | 25.61 | |
55 | 23.52 | |
3 | 14.08 | |
27 | 7.37 | |
72 | −0.72 | |
1 | −2.80 | |
54 | −6.62 | |
6 | −22.60 | |
58 | −69.28 | (Spanish-dominant) |
CVCV | CVN | |
---|---|---|
/i/ | tita ‘aunt’ (informal) | patín ‘rollerblade’ |
/e/ | cateto ‘ignorant’ | ten ‘you have, imp.’ |
/a/ | tato ‘little brother’ | tan ‘so’ |
/o/ | pitote ‘fuss’ | botón ‘button’ |
/u/ | batuta ‘baton’ | atún ‘tuna’ |
CV(C) | CVN | |
---|---|---|
/i/ | tea | teen |
/e/ | tape | attain |
/ɑ/ | top | futon 1 |
/o/ | toe | tone |
/u/ | two | tune |
English M (SD) | Spanish M (SD) | |
---|---|---|
CVN | 0.22 (0.19) | 0.11 (0.13) |
CV(C)(V) | 0.04 (0.05) | 0.03 (0.03) |
Fixed Effects | Est. (β) | Std. Error | df | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.22 | 0.03 | 9.59 | 7.54 | <0.001 |
Language: Spanish | −0.11 | 0.04 | 7.92 | −3.04 | <0.05 |
Context: CVCV | −0.19 | 0.03 | 8.51 | −6.87 | <0.001 |
BLP Score | 0.0003 | 0.0003 | 5.69 | 0.85 | 0.42 |
Language x Context | 0.11 | 0.04 | 9.58 | 2.76 | <0.05 |
Speaker | BLP Score | English | Spanish |
---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | ||
65 | 96.26 | 0.61 (0.26) | 0.48 (0.34) |
8 | 93.63 | 0.49 (0.32) | 0.51 (0.44) |
53 | 68.57 | 0.41 (0.25) | 0.47 (0.38) |
26 | 60.75 | 0.47 (0.31) | 0.38 (0.24) |
52 | 55.66 | 0.68 (0.31) | 0.53 (0.33) |
30 | 45.31 | 0.23 (0.15) | 0.37 (0.38) |
20 | 26.88 | 0.68 (0.29) | 0.42 (0.38) |
42 | 25.61 | 0.41 (0.29) | 0.46 (0.42) |
55 | 23.52 | 0.32 (0.27) | 0.18 (0.16) |
3 | 14.08 | 0.31 (0.27) | 0.33 (0.29) |
27 | 7.37 | 0.39 (0.19) | 0.42 (0.38) |
72 | −0.72 | 0.53 (0.23) | 0.43 (0.3) |
1 | −2.80 | 0.48 (0.33) | 0.5 (0.33) |
54 | −6.62 | 0.36 (0.35) | 0.42 (0.42) |
6 | −22.60 | 0.41 (0.26) | 0.21 (0.2) |
58 | −69.28 | 0.52 (0.33) | 0.40 (0.29) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Beristain, A. An Airflow Analysis of Spanish and English Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization among Heritage Bilinguals. Languages 2023, 8, 205. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030205
Beristain A. An Airflow Analysis of Spanish and English Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization among Heritage Bilinguals. Languages. 2023; 8(3):205. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030205
Chicago/Turabian StyleBeristain, Ander. 2023. "An Airflow Analysis of Spanish and English Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization among Heritage Bilinguals" Languages 8, no. 3: 205. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030205
APA StyleBeristain, A. (2023). An Airflow Analysis of Spanish and English Anticipatory Vowel Nasalization among Heritage Bilinguals. Languages, 8(3), 205. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030205