Next Article in Journal
Digitization of Accounting: The Premise of the Paradigm Shift of Role of the Professional Accountant
Next Article in Special Issue
Test–Retest Reliability in Automated Emotional Facial Expression Analysis: Exploring FaceReader 8.0 on Data from Typically Developing Children and Children with Autism
Previous Article in Journal
A Simultaneous Analysis of the User Safety and Resilience of a Twin-Tube Road Tunnel
Previous Article in Special Issue
To Game or Not to Game? Efficacy of Using Tablet Games in Vocabulary Intervention for Children with DLD
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Auditory and Visual Components in Reading Training: No Additional Effect of Synchronized Visual Cue in a Rhythm-Based Intervention for Dyslexia

1
Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, 20123 Milan, Italy
2
Developmental Neurology Unit—Language and Learning Disorders Service, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, 20133 Milan, Italy
3
Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Postgraduate School of Child Neuropsychiatry, University of Milan, 20122 Milan, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(7), 3360; https://doi.org/10.3390/app12073360
Submission received: 15 February 2022 / Revised: 19 March 2022 / Accepted: 23 March 2022 / Published: 25 March 2022

Abstract

:
Based on the transfer effects of music training on the phonological and reading abilities of children with dyslexia, a computerized rhythmic intervention—the Rhythmic Reading Training (RRT)—was developed, in which reading exercises are combined with a rhythmic synchronization task. This rehabilitation program was previously tested in multiple controlled clinical trials, which confirmed its effectiveness in improving the reading skills of children and adolescents with dyslexia. In order to assess the specific contribution of the visual component of the training, namely, the presence of a visual cue supporting rhythmic synchronization, a controlled experimental study was conducted. Fifty-eight students with dyslexia aged 8 to 13 years were assigned to three conditions: (a) RRT auditory and visual condition, in which a visual cue was synchronized with the rhythmic stimulation; (b) RRT auditory-only condition, in which the visual cue was excluded; (c) no intervention. Comparisons of the participants’ performance before, after, and 3 months after the end of the intervention period revealed the significant immediate and long-term effect of both RRT conditions on reading, rapid naming, phonological, rhythmic, and attentional abilities. No significant differences were found between visual and auditory conditions, therefore showing no additional contribution of the visual component to the improvements induced by the RRT. Clinical Trial ID: NCT04995991.

1. Introduction

The potential of music in improving reading in developmental dyslexia has been explored with promising results [1]. More precisely, evidence from research on auditory and music-based interventions for dyslexia supported the notion that music has a positive impact on phonological abilities and auditory temporal processing [1]. The hypothesis of a transfer effect of music on reading stemmed from theoretical frameworks, which linked the dyslexia-related phonological deficits to a perceptual auditory impairment, specific for the temporal aspect of sound processing. Among those, the Temporal Sampling (TS) theory [2,3] focused on the role of temporal acoustic components carrying information about the rhythmical structure of speech and non-speech sound streams. The sensitivity to speech rhythm via a synchronous oscillatory neural activity at Delta and Theta bands in the auditory cortex, which were found to be disrupted in dyslexia [2,4], is indeed crucial for the development of phonological abilities [5,6]. It is worth mentioning that although the phonological deficit is extensively described in dyslexia literature, e.g., [7,8,9], other explanatory theories have been posited, such as the visuo-spatial attention deficit, e.g., [10] and the magnocellular theory, e.g., [11]. Considering the multimodal nature of the disorder, the most recent advances in dyslexia research converged on a multi-componential etiological theory [12,13].
The potential use of music for dyslexia rehabilitation is further supported by the notion that rhythm generates temporal expectancies in the listener. When we expect something to happen, we react quicker and more efficiently to events [14,15,16]. Expectancies enable individuals to anticipate future events, both in terms of what is going to happen and when it will occur. Beneficial effects of temporal expectancies on verbal processing are reported when monitoring phonetic form [5], detecting word changes [17], perceiving spoken words [16], or resolving syntactic ambiguities [18]. An impaired rhythmic sound perception was described in children with dyslexia [19]. Such impairment was hypothesized to stem from a difficulty in anticipating and, therefore, predicting events in temporal succession [20]. To confirm this hypothesis, other studies have shown that students who received a timing/rhythm intervention, designed to reduce response latency to a metronome beat, improved significantly in reading fluency [21].
To take advantage of the rhythm potential, an innovative approach to dyslexia rehabilitation, the Rhythmic Reading Training (RRT), was devised by Cancer and colleagues [22] by embedding reading tasks into rhythm-based training activities. The hypothesis supporting the development of RRT was that synchronizing speech production during reading to a regular acoustic stimulus would improve temporal processing by coupling the cross-frequency oscillatory activity in auditory and visual areas, thus enhancing the precision of timing perception.
Previous controlled studies tested the efficacy of RRT in samples of children and adolescents with dyslexia. In the first trial [23], significant improvements in reading speed and accuracy were found in comparison to a no intervention condition after 9 RRT sessions. The following comparison studies included active control conditions, namely, a language-based traditional intervention [24] and a technologically advanced visual-based intervention for reading [25]. In both trials RRT produced global reading improvements which did not differ from the control interventions. However, significant larger effects of RRT on non-word reading speed were found. Furthermore, medium-term significant effects on the outcome measures were found in a longitudinal study [26]. RRT efficacy was recently confirmed in a study testing the effect of a telerehabilitation version of the intervention [27].
The RRT software includes the option to add a visual cue to the reading exercises, which highlights portions of the text which has to be read in synchrony with the rhythmic base. Such a setting was originally introduced to facilitate the synchronization task during reading and it was meant to be turned off by the trainer as soon as the participant was able to accurately coordinate their reading to the beat without any visual aid. The personalized use of the visual cue setting in RRT made it impossible to control for the specific effect of the visual component of the intervention in the previous RRT clinical trials.
Temporal anticipation can, indeed, be generated not only by auditory stimuli, such as the beats of a metronome or a music piece, but also by visual stimuli. Selective attention to a spatial location has been shown to enhance perception and facilitate response times to attended visual stimuli compared to unattended visual locations [28]. Several authors have investigated the relationship between temporal and spatial expectancies proving an additive effect when both are matched [29]. Likewise, in rhythm task, multisensory rhythms temporally aligned (e.g., auditory and motor) can also enhance expectancies and boost human behaviour [17]. Reading, by definition, begins in the visual system with a perceptual segmentation of the letters string into its constituent graphemes and involves an accurate and rapid shifting of visuo-spatial attention. A deficit in visual attention shifting has been demonstrated to play a critical role in dyslexia [30,31]. Although the role of auditory temporal expectancies has already been investigated in dyslexia [20], to our knowledge the combined role of visuospatial and auditory expectancy was not. In naturalistic environments, we perceive objects through our different senses and redundant intersensory information is bound together, with cross-modal enhancement taking place at multiple levels. A striking example comes from face-to-face dialogue where audio-visual synchronized information facilitates detection of amodal features (e.g., prosody) [32], as predicted by intersensory redundancy hypothesis [33]. Based on previous literature on the additional anticipatory effect of auditory and visual rhythms, it is possible that the combination of the auditory and visual components of RRT would maximize the effect of the intervention in terms of reading improvements.
The present study aimed to assess the specific contribution of the visual component of the training, namely, the presence of a visual cue supporting rhythmic synchronization. To do so, two RRT administration conditions were implemented: (a) RRT auditory and visual condition, in which a visual cue is synchronized with the rhythmic stimulation (i.e., Visual cue); (b) RRT auditory only condition, in which the visual cue was excluded (i.e., Rhythm).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited among patients of the Learning Disorders (LDs) Service of the Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta of Milan, Italy, on the basis of the following inclusion criteria: (a) formal diagnosis of developmental dyslexia (ICD-10 code: F81.0) [34]; (b) age 8–14 years; (c) normal intellectual functioning, as measured by the Total IQ composite score derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Forth Edition [35] (TIQ ≥ 85); and (d) absence of psychiatric and/or neurological conditions.
Fifty-eight children and adolescents, aged 8–14 years (M = 10.8; S.D. = 1.64), were selected on the basis of the reported criteria and enrolled in the study after written informed consent was obtained from their parents. A sample size of 58 was a priori calculated to be enough to detect a medium effect size (η2 = 0.06) in the planned GLMs and achieve a power of 0.97, setting alpha at 0.05.

2.2. Procedure

The clinical trial was pre-recorded on ClinicalTrial.gov (Clinical Trial ID: NCT04995991).
Participants were assigned to one of three experimental conditions (Visual cue: n = 21; Rhythm: n = 21; No intervention: n = 16) by stratifying the sample for age, sex, total IQ, and baseline reading performance. The no intervention condition was included in order to control for test–retest learning effects.
Participants assigned to the Visual cue and Rhythm conditions received 10 individual RRT training sessions of 45 min, supervised by an expert trainer, over 5 weeks. All participants underwent three assessment sessions (i.e., pre-training, post-training, and 3-month follow-up), in which the primary outcomes of the intervention (i.e., reading speed and reading accuracy) were measured. To further analyse the specific differences between RRT conditions, additional testing, including secondary outcome measures (i.e., rapid automatized naming, phonological, attentional, and rhythmic abilities), was applied only to participants assigned to the intervention conditions (i.e., Visual cue and Rhythm).

2.3. Interventions

RRT is a rhythm-based intervention for reading. Exercises are delivered through a desktop app, which is managed by an expert RRT practitioner during one-on-one training sessions with the participant. The app includes specific modules of reading tasks combined with the synchronization to an isochronous rhythm [22]. Each module includes exercises addresses specific reading sub-process: (a) in the ‘Syllable’ module, participants are asked to read aloud lists of syllables presented on the screen in synchrony with the pace of a rhythmic base. (b) In the ‘Merging’ module, syllables are sequentially presented on the screen, in synchrony with a rhythmic base; participants are required to merge syllables together and pronounce the resulting word or non-word. (c) In the “Words and Non-words” module, participants are asked to read aloud lists of word and non-words, adjusting the pace of their reading to that of the rhythmic base. Furthermore, this module includes phrases and nursery-rhymes reading exercises. Either a regular metronomic beat or a simple pentatonic melody serves as the rhythmic base in each exercise. The accented pattern on the linguistic material matches the metrical structure of the rhythmic base, so that each music beat stresses the accented components of language during reading. The tempo of the rhythmic base is gradually increased throughout each training session to improve reading fluency. The exercises’ settings can be adjusted by the trainer by modulating the rhythm speed and the complexity of the verbal material. Reading accuracy is constantly monitored by the trainer, who is instructed to advance based on accuracy rate (90% threshold) in each activity.
In the present study, RRT exercises were delivered in two modalities: (a) during rhythmic reading, a visual cue–synchronized with the rhythmic beats–sequentially highlights each verbal stimulus on the screen through changing-colour text (the text turns red simultaneously to each rhythmic beat); (b) During rhythmic reading, static verbal stimuli are presented on the screen, without any additional visual aid. In both modalities, the verbal material (i.e., syllables, word, non-words, phrases) is presented on the screen in rows and it has to be read from left to right, from the top row to the bottom row, as in typical reading. Only the modules which allowed the selection of the visual cue setting (syllable, word, non-word, and sentence reading exercises) were used in the present trial.

2.4. Measures

Prior to enrolment, the clinical documentation of each participant was analysed to check for inclusion criteria. Such clinical documentation included the latest certification of dyslexia, previously made by clinical practitioners of the IRCCS LDs Service following the formal Italian Learning Disorders’ diagnostic practice [36]. Medical history and a measure of intellectual functioning (i.e., total IQ derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Forth Edition [35]) were retrieved from the dyslexia certification.

2.4.1. Primary Outcomes

A battery of standardized tests was applied to measure reading abilities. Word and non-word reading was assessed using the “Assessment battery for Developmental Reading and Spelling Disorders-2” [37], which comprises 4 lists of 28 words and 2 lists of 16 non-words. Text reading was assessed using the age-normed Reading task from the ‘MT-3 Clinical Tests’ [38]. For all reading tasks, z-scores of reading speed (measured as syllable per second) and reading accuracy (measured as number of errors) were computed.

2.4.2. Secondary Outcomes

The following standardized tests were used to assess the secondary outcome measures.
‘Word and pseudo-word reading test’ [39]. Additional reading tasks were used to collect further measures of the reading subprocesses. More precisely, the test included 4 lists of 30 words, varying according to length and frequency of use, and 2 lists of 30 non-words, varying according to length. Reading speed (as number of seconds) and accuracy (as number of errors) z-scores were computed.
‘Rapid Automatized Naming test (RAN)–Figures’ test [40]. In this test, rapid and sequential naming of 50 black and white figures (i.e., pear, train, dog, star, hand) is recorded. RAN speed (as number of seconds) z-scores were considered.
‘Phonemic awareness task–Phonemic elision’ [41]. In this task, the participants listen to 40 words pronounced by the examiner. For each word they are instructed to omit a given phoneme from the word and to pronounce the resulting non-word. Scores are expressed in total number of errors.
‘NEPSY-II–Visual Attention Test’ [42]. In this cancellation test participants are required to mark out all visual targets distributed on a paper sheet among similar stimuli (i.e., black and white cartoon faces) within 180 s. Accuracy standard scores were computed by subtracting the number of commission errors (i.e., marked distractors) from the number of correctly detected targets.
‘Rhythm reproduction task’ [43]. In this task participants are instructed to reproduce a sequence of rhythmic patterns, composed by 3–8 beats of increasing complexity, by tapping a pencil on the desk, after the examiner’s presentation. Accuracy scores are recorded as number of errors.
‘Tapping and Continuation Task (TCT)’ [44]. In this computerized tapping task, participants are instructed to tap along with an isochronous rhythm for 15 s and to continue tapping at the same tempo (ISI = 500 ms) for 15 s after the rhythm stops, using a computer mouse. A tapping variability score is calculated as the average SD of the inter-tap intervals (ITIs) synchrony across six trials, dived by the average ITI.
Unfortunately, it was not possible to administer the complete battery of additional tests in the follow-up assessment session for organizational reasons. More precisely, the Visual attention and Tapping scores were collected only for the first two time-points (pre- and post-training).

2.5. Analytic Plan

The first step of the analyses was to test the homogeneity of the stratified subgroups (i.e., Visual cue, Rhythm, No intervention) based on age, gender, total IQ, and baseline reading abilities. To do so, global reading speed and reading accuracy scores were computed by averaging pre-training word, non-word, and text reading scores. Comparisons were tested using one-way ANOVA for continuous variables and Chi-squared test for categorical variables.
Secondly, we compared immediate effects on the primary outcome measures (i.e., reading speed and accuracy) between the conditions (i.e., Visual cue vs. Rhythm vs. No intervention). More precisely, a mixed factorial ANOVAs 2 × 3 × 3 (Phase: pre vs. post; Test: word vs. non-word vs. text; Intervention type: Visual cue vs. Rhythm vs. No intervention) was performed for each reading outcome parameter (i.e., speed and accuracy).
Thirdly, we tested the specific immediate effects of each RRT condition (i.e., Visual cue vs. Rhythm) on the secondary outcome measures (i.e., additional reading tasks, RAN, phonemic elision, visual attention, rhythm reproduction, tapping) using a mixed factorial ANOVA 2 × 2 (Phase: pre vs. post; Intervention type: visual cue vs. rhythm).
Afterwards, we compared medium-term effects between three conditions using a mixed factorial ANOVA 2 × 3 × 3 (Phase: pre vs. follow-up; Test: word vs. non-word vs. text; Intervention type: Visual cue vs. Rhythm vs. No intervention) for each reading outcome parameter (i.e., speed and accuracy).
Finally, we tested the specific medium-term effects of each RRT condition (i.e., Visual cue vs. Rhythm) on the secondary outcome measures using a mixed factorial ANOVA 2 × 2 (Phase: pre vs. follow-up; Intervention type: Visual cue vs. Rhythm).
For each GLM, post hoc pairwise comparisons were computed using Tukey’s Test.

3. Results

3.1. Participants’ Characteristics

Participants were attending school grade 3rd to 8th and had normal intelligence (total IQ: M = 103, S.D. = 11.3).
Comparisons between groups (i.e., Visual cue, Rhythm, No intervention) revealed nonsignificant differences in age (F(2,55) = 0.31; p = 0.73), sex (χ2 = 0.24; p = 0.89), general intelligence (TIQ: F(2,46) = 0.22; p = 0.81) and baseline reading measures (speed: F(2,55) = 0.24; p = 0.78; accuracy: F(2,55) = 1.30; p = 0.28). Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1.

3.2. RRT Immediate Effects

3.2.1. Pre-Post Effects on Primary Outcomes

Regarding reading speed, a significant Phase × Condition interaction effect (F(2,110) = 83.4; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.01) was found. Post hoc analyses revealed pre-post significant improvements in both Visual cue (t(55) = −7.81; p < 0.001) and Rhythm conditions (t(55) = −6.92; p < 0.001). Conversely, pre-post differences within the No intervention control condition were nonsignificant (t(55) = −1.65; p = 0.57).
Although the Phase × Condition interaction effect on reading accuracy was nonsignificant (F(2,18) = 2.1; p = 0.132), post hoc comparisons showed a significant pre-post effect of the Visual-cue intervention on reading accuracy (t(54) = −3.23; p = 0.024). Reading accuracy did not change in either the Rhythm (t(54) = −1.98; p = 0.367) nor the No intervention (t(54) = −0.05; p = 0.100) conditions.

3.2.2. Pre-Post Effects on Secondary Outcomes

Reading speed of words and non-words improved significantly after intervention within both RRT conditions (i.e., Visual cue and Rhythm) and no difference was found for any of the considered psycholinguistic characteristics of the verbal materials (i.e., length and frequency of use). More precisely, a significant Phase main effect was found on reading speed of short non-words (F(1,35) = 34.55; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.11), long non-words (F(1,35) = 17.55; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.06), high-frequency short words (F(1,35) = 11.77; p = 0.002; η2 = 0.04), high-frequency long words (F(1,35) = 23.07; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.04), low-frequency short words (F(1,35) = 15.85; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.05), and low-frequency long words (F(1,35) = 13.30; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.02). No significant Phase × Condition interaction effect was found (ps ranging from 0.14 to 0.94).
Reading accuracy was improved after RRT for low-frequency short words (Phase: F(1,35) = 6.71; p = 0.014; η2 = 0.03) and low-frequency long words (F(1,35) = 5.11; p = 0.030; η2 = 0.01), without significant Phase × Condition effects (low-frequency short words: F(1,35) = 0.62; p = 0.435; low-frequency long words: F(1,35) = 0.32; p = 0.574). Accuracy of the other reading materials (i.e., short non-words, long non-words, high-frequency short words, and high-frequency long words) was not improved after RRT (ps ranging from 0.11 to 0.96).
A significant pre-post RAN improvement was found (Phase: F(1,35) = 5.90; p = 0.020; η2 = 0.03) with no difference between conditions (Phase × Condition: F(1,35) = 0.42; p = 0.520).
Performance in phonemic elision improved significantly after both interventions (Phase: F(1,35) = 9.39; p = 0.004; η2 = 0.02) and no difference was found between conditions (Phase × Condition: F(1,35) = 0.002; p = 0.963).
Consistently with the other secondary outcomes, visual attention was increased after treatment in both conditions (Phase: F(1,18) = 13.26; p = 0.002; η2 = 0.23) and the changes did not differ between interventions (Phase × Condition: F(1,18) = 6.79; p = 0.370).
Rhythmic abilities improved significantly after RRT, following the same pattern. Pre-post changes were found in the rhythmic reproduction (Phase: F(1,34) = 8.17; p = 0.007; η2 = 0.01) and tapping (Phase: F(1,16) = 11.39; p = 0.004; η2 = 0.21) tasks and the Phase × Condition interaction effect was nonsignificant (Reproduction: Phase: F(1,34) = 3.19; p = 0.083; Tapping: F(1,16) = 0.15; p = 0.703).

3.3. RRT Medium-Term Effects

3.3.1. Pre-Follow-Up Effects on Primary Outcomes

Figure 1 shows reading speed improvements for each test (word, non-word, and text) in each condition. Phase × Condition interaction effect on reading speed did not reach statistical significance (F(2,92) = 2.96; p = 0.06) in the pre-follow-up comparison. Indeed, Phase × Test × Condition pairwise comparisons revealed a test–retest effect on Text reading speed specifically, as showed by the pre-follow-up significant difference in the No intervention control group (t(46) = −4.88; p = 0.002). Conversely, word reading speed increased significantly at follow-up only in the Visual cue group (t(46) = –5.20; p < 0.001), but not in the Rhythm (t(46) = −3.22; p = 0.155) nor the No intervention group (t(46) = −2.18; p = 0.753). Finally, a pre-follow-up effect on Non-word reading speed was found in both RRT conditions (Visual cue: t(46) = −5.20; p < 0.001; Rhythm: t(46) = −4.64; p = 0.003) but not in the control group (t(46) = −1.61; p = 0.973).
Reading accuracy did not change 3 months after intervention in any condition (Phase: F(1,92) = 3.42; p = 0.071; Phase × Condition: F(2,92) = 0.11; p = 0.888).

3.3.2. Pre-Follow-Up Effects on Secondary Outcomes

Reading speed of all verbal materials was still improved 3 months after the end of the interventions, as showed by significant Phase main effects (short non-words: F(1,31) = 32.9; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.13; long non-words: F(1,31) = 23.60; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.08; high-frequency short words: F(1,31) = 28.78; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.09; high-frequency long words: F(1,31) = 12.73; p = 0.001; η2 = 0.04; low-frequency short words: F(1,31) = 16.94; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.06; low-frequency long words: F(1,31) = 11.08; p = 0.002; η2 = 0.03). No significant Phase × Condition effect was found on any reading speed test (ps ranging from 0.22 to 0.72).
Non-significant pre-follow-up accuracy changes were found in any reading test (ps ranging from 0.06 to 0.96).
Pre-follow-up comparisons were significant for RAN (Phase: F(1,31) = 4.16; p = 0.050; η2 = 0.03), with no difference between RRT conditions (Phase × Condition: F(1,31) = 0.27; p = 0.607).
Similarly, phonemic elision was improved 3 months after intervention (Phase: F(1,31) = 6.65; p = 0.015; η2 = 0.04) and no difference between conditions emerged (Phase × Condition: F(1,31) = 0.02; p = 0.884).
Finally, Rhythm reproduction was significantly higher at follow-up (Phase: F(1,28) = 14.64; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.04) in both RRT conditions (Phase × Condition: F(1,28) = 1.81; p = 0.190).
Descriptive statistics of pre, post, and follow-up measures are reported in Table S1.

4. Discussion and Conclusions

Results showed significant reading improvements in both RRT conditions (i.e., auditory and visual cue and auditory rhythm only), as compared to the No Intervention control condition. The overall results revealed that reading speed of words and non-words was significantly improved after both RRT conditions, both immediately after and three months after the end of the intervention. In addition, reading accuracy improvements were specifically recorded for low-frequency words immediately after both RRT conditions. Since a test–retest significant effect on text reading was found at follow-up, as measured by improvement in the No Intervention control group, this measure could not be considered as a medium-term outcome of the intervention.
RRT’s foundation lies within the fact that children with dyslexia fail to identify supra-segmental speech cues, namely, rhythm, pitch, and stress [45,46], prior to showing weaknesses in manipulating segmental cues, this being an essential ability to acquire grapheme-phoneme correspondences [47]. RRT might support children in segmenting hierarchical acoustic rhythm structures in language connected to phonological units by stressing syllables and on-set-rimes. The found effects on reading speed and accuracy support such hypothesis and are consistent with previous RRT studies [22,23,25,26].
Interestingly, all secondary outcome measures (i.e., rapid automatized naming, phonological, attentional, and rhythmic abilities) improved in both conditions, without significant differences. These findings are consistent with the relationships between reading abilities and non-linguistic abilities, as visuo-spatial attention, rhythmic abilities, and rapid automatized naming previously described in dyslexia [31]. A previous study in which RRT was compared with a training aimed at the visual components of reading [25] reported better results for the latter in the RAN test, while better results for RRT in the phonological tests. Based on these results, we initially expected that visuo-attentional abilities and RAN would be better targeted by the RRT Visual cue condition; however, the results showed that the auditory anticipation in the Rhythm condition was enough to ameliorate the visuo-spatial components and visuo-auditory integrations involved in the reading process.
In summary, no significant differences were found between the auditory and visual conditions, therefore showing no additional contribution of the visual component to the RRT effect. Such results suggest that RRT’s effect on reading is mainly supported by the improvement in auditory temporal processing.
The literature pointed to a potential additional effect of the visual component of the intervention on reading-related cognitive processes, through a facilitation of anticipatory mechanisms [17], no specific contribution of the visual cue setting was found. Such a discrepancy can be read by considering the contrasting neurophysiological literature on temporal processing. Pasinski and colleagues [48] used ERP to investigate whether temporal processing was modality specific or modality general. The contingent negative variation (CNV), a negative potential which has been strongly linked to temporal encoding and anticipation, showed a larger amplitude for the auditory modality compared to the visual modality, suggesting that the brain is more responsive during the encoding of auditory timing information compared to during encoding of visual timing. According to these findings, the visual modality may be less equipped to form temporal expectancies [48]. This hypothesis is consistent with the results from the RRT training, which demonstrated that rhythm processing depended on the auditory modality and multimodal presentation did not add advantages. In line with this, according to Goswami [2], visual attention and auditory-visual integration anomalies in dyslexia [49] would stem from difficulties in forming an internal representation of rhythmic timing and would be mostly explained by the impaired rhythmic auditory entrainment. Moreover, dyslexia has been associated with deficits in multisensory integration and cross-modal learning [50]. A recent study on individuals with dyslexia confirmed impairments in audio-visual speech integration and demonstrated lack of cross-modal enhancement, which has been ascribed to dysfunctions in superior temporal regions [51].
Other training interventions for dyslexia involving visual processing only, without any linguistic component, were previously studied [52,53,54]. More precisely, training children with dyslexia using action videogames (AVG), without any concomitant phonological training, was found to significantly improve reading scores in Italian children with dyslexia [52,53] through improved visuospatial attention capabilities.
Although attentional shifting is also supported by the Visual cue of RRT through visual anticipation, in RRT the visual guide is based on a predictable rhythmic base. Therefore, it differs from AVG training based on attention shifting, in which the target appears in random locations on the screen at randomized time-intervals. These features make it difficult to specifically compare the AVG and the RRT Visual cue modality results. However, in a previous study the effects of the RRT traditional version (which included a non-controlled use of the visual cue) was compared to those of the AVG training combined with a linguistic tachistoscopic intervention [25]. Findings from this study revealed comparable overall results on reading performance, although different cognitive mechanisms were found to support the outcomes in the two types of training, namely, phonological awareness for RRT and rapid automatized naming for AVG and tachistoscopic training.
A potential methodological limitation of the present study is the increased probability of incorrectly rejecting the null hypotheses (Type I error) due to multiple hypotheses being tested simultaneously. Nevertheless, the probability of a type I error cannot be decreased without increasing that of a type II error, such that real differences may not be detected. Although we opted against the application of p-value adjustments, tests were carried out with pre-planned hypotheses and convergent measures for reading skills were used. Therefore, the reported results can be considered sufficiently reliable.
Overall, RRT efficacy in improving reading skills, producing both immediate and medium-term effect, was confirmed by the present study. Narrative experiential reports from the clinical practitioners who administered RRT revealed that the absence of a visual cue enhanced children’s engagement by making the synchronized reading task more challenging, and thus supporting motivation. On the other hand, children’s comments during training suggested that the visual cue was particularly useful to reinforce the auditory modality. To further the understanding of the perceived utility of RRT settings by children and practitioners, future studies should include self-report measures of effectiveness and usability collected from RRT users.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/app12073360/s1, Table S1: Descriptive statistics of pre, post and follow-up scores for all outcome measures.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C., E.G., A.A. and D.S.; Data curation, A.C. and M.D.S.; Formal analysis, A.C.; Investigation, M.D.S., E.G. and D.S.; Methodology, A.C. and D.S.; Project administration, E.G., A.A. and D.S.; Supervision, E.G., A.A. and D.S.; Writing—Original draft, A.C. and M.D.S.; Writing—Review and editing, A.C., M.D.S., A.A., L.A. and D.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee of the Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Milan, Italy (Approval Number: 30–18; date of approval: 23 June 2018) and by the Ethics Committee of the Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta of Milan, Italy (Approval Number: 82/2021; date of approval: 17 March 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from parents of all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank Marco Guida, Natalia Pelucchi, Rebecca Minoliti, Benedetta Guggiari, Annalisa Pavanello, Chiara Pradella for their contribution in the present study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cancer, A.; Antonietti, A. Music-Based and Auditory-Based Interventions for Reading Difficulties: A Literature Review. Heliyon Psychol. under revision.
  2. Goswami, U. A Temporal Sampling Framework for Developmental Dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Goswami, U.; Power, A.J.; Lallier, M.; Facoetti, A. Oscillatory “Temporal Sampling” and Developmental Dyslexia: Toward an over-Arching Theoretical Framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Giraud, A.-L.; Ramus, F. Neurogenetics and Auditory Processing in Developmental Dyslexia. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013, 23, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Cason, N.; Schön, D. Rhythmic Priming Enhances the Phonological Processing of Speech. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 2652–2658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Torppa, R.; Faulkner, A.; Huotilainen, M.; Järvikivi, J.; Lipsanen, J.; Laasonen, M.; Vainio, M. The Perception of Prosody and Associated Auditory Cues in Early-Implanted Children: The Role of Auditory Working Memory and Musical Activities. Int. J. Audiol. 2014, 53, 182–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Castles, A.; Friedmann, N. Developmental Dyslexia and the Phonological Deficit Hypothesis. Mind Lang. 2014, 29, 270–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Vellutino, F.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Scanlon, D.M. Specific Reading Disability (Dyslexia): What Have We Learned in the Past Four Decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Snowling, M.J.; Melby-Lervåg, M. Oral Language Deficits in Familial Dyslexia: A Meta-Analysis and Review. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 498–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Franceschini, S.; Gori, S.; Ruffino, M.; Pedrolli, K.; Facoetti, A. A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 814–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Stein, J. The Magnocellular Theory of Developmental Dyslexia. Dyslexia Chichester Engl. 2001, 7, 12–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ramus, F.; Marshall, C.R.; Rosen, S.; Van der Lely, H.K.J. Phonological Deficits in Specific Language Impairment and Developmental Dyslexia: Towards a Multidimensional Model. Brain 2013, 136, 630–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Menghini, D.; Finzi, A.; Benassi, M.; Bolzani, R.; Facoetti, A.; Giovagnoli, S.; Ruffino, M.; Vicari, S. Different Underlying Neurocognitive Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia: A Comparative Study. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 863–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bolger, D.; Coull, J.T.; Schön, D. Metrical Rhythm Implicitly Orients Attention in Time as Indexed by Improved Target Detection and Left Inferior Parietal Activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 593–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Jones, E.G. Thalamic Circuitry and Thalamocortical Synchrony. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2002, 357, 1659–1673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Quené, H.; Port, R.F. Effects of Timing Regularity and Metrical Expectancy on Spoken-Word Perception. Phonetica 2005, 62, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Falk, S.; Dalla Bella, S. It Is Better When Expected: Aligning Speech and Motor Rhythms Enhances Verbal Processing. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 31, 699–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Roncaglia-Denissen, M.P.; Schmidt-Kassow, M.; Kotz, S.A. Speech Rhythm Facilitates Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution: ERP Evidence. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e56000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Flaugnacco, E.; Lopez, L.; Terribili, C.; Zoia, S.; Buda, S.; Tilli, S.; Monasta, L.; Montico, M.; Sila, A.; Ronfani, L.; et al. Rhythm Perception and Production Predict Reading Abilities in Developmental Dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Pagliarini, E.; Scocchia, L.; Granocchio, E.; Sarti, D.; Stucchi, N.; Guasti, M.T. Timing Anticipation in Adults and Children with Developmental Dyslexia: Evidence of an Inefficient Mechanism. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 17519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Taub, G.E.; Lazarus, P.J. The Effects of Training in Timing and Rhythm on Reading Achievement. Contemp. Issues Educ. Res. 2012, 5, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Cancer, A.; Bonacina, S.; Lorusso, M.L.; Lanzi, P.L.; Antonietti, A. Rhythmic Reading Training (RRT): A Computer-Assisted Intervention Program for Dyslexia. In Pervasive Computing Paradigms for Mental Health; Serino, S., Matic, A., Giakoumis, D., Lopez, G., Cipresso, P., Eds.; Communications in Computer and Information Science; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 249–258. ISBN 978-3-319-32269-8. [Google Scholar]
  23. Bonacina, S.; Cancer, A.; Lanzi, P.L.; Lorusso, M.L.; Antonietti, A. Improving Reading Skills in Students with Dyslexia: The Efficacy of a Sublexical Training with Rhythmic Background. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Cancer, A.; Stievano, G.; Pace, G.; Colombo, A.; Brembati, F.; Donini, R.; Antonietti, A. Remedial Interventions for Italian Children with Developmental Dyslexia: Comparing the Rhythmic Reading Training to the ‘Abilmente’ Approach. Psychol. Music. under revision.
  25. Cancer, A.; Bonacina, S.; Antonietti, A.; Salandi, A.; Molteni, M.; Lorusso, M.L. The Effectiveness of Interventions for Developmental Dyslexia: Rhythmic Reading Training Compared with Hemisphere-Specific Stimulation and Action Video Games. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Cancer, A.; Stievano, G.; Pace, G.; Colombo, A.; Antonietti, A. Cognitive Processes Underlying Reading Improvement during a Rhythm-Based Intervention. A Small-Scale Investigation of Italian Children with Dyslexia. Children 2019, 6, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Cancer, A.; Sarti, D.; de Salvatore, M.; Granocchio, E.; Chieffo, D.P.R.; Antonietti, A. Dyslexia Telerehabilitation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Results of a Rhythm-Based Intervention for Reading. Children 2021, 8, 1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jones, A. Independent Effects of Bottom-up Temporal Expectancy and Top-down Spatial Attention. An Audiovisual Study Using Rhythmic Cueing. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2015, 8, 96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Doherty, J.R. Synergistic Effect of Combined Temporal and Spatial Expectations on Visual Attention. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 8259–8266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Cestnick, L.; Coltheart, M. The Relationship between Language-Processing and Visual-Processing Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia. Cognition 1999, 71, 231–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Facoetti, A.; Zorzi, M.; Cestnick, L.; Lorusso, M.L.; Molteni, M.; Paganoni, P.; Umiltà, C.; Mascetti, G.G. The Relationship between Visuo-Spatial Attention and Nonword Reading in Developmental Dyslexia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2006, 23, 841–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Bahrick, L.E.; McNew, M.E.; Pruden, S.M.; Castellanos, I. Intersensory Redundancy Promotes Infant Detection of Prosody in Infant-Directed Speech. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 183, 295–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bahrick, L.E.; Lickliter, R. Intersensory Redundancy Guides Early Perceptual and Cognitive Development. In Advances in Child Development and Behavior; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2002; Volume 30, pp. 153–187. ISBN 978-0-12-009730-2. [Google Scholar]
  34. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 1992; ISBN 978-92-4-154422-1. [Google Scholar]
  35. Weschler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) Administration and Scoring Manual; Psychological Association: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  36. ISS Consensus Conference “Disturbi Specifici Dell’apprendimento”. Available online: https://www.aiditalia.org (accessed on 4 August 2021).
  37. Sartori, G.; Job, R. DDE-2: Batteria per la Valutazione della Dislessia e della Disortografia Evolutiva-2 [Assessment Battery for Developmental Reading and Spelling Disorders]; Giunti O.S.: Florence, Italy, 2007; ISBN 978-88-09-40301-7. [Google Scholar]
  38. Cornoldi, C.; Carretti, B. Prove MT-3 Clinica: La Valutazione delle Abilità di Lettura e Comprensione per la Scuola Primaria e Secondaria di I Grado; Giunti Edu: Florence, Italy, 2016; ISBN 978-88-09-84159-8. [Google Scholar]
  39. Zoccolotti, P.; de Luca, M.; Di Filippo, G.; Judica, A.; Spinelli, D. Prova di Lettura di Parole e Non Parole. [Word and Pseudo-Word Reading Test—WPRT]; IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia: Rome, Italy, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  40. De Luca, M.; Di Filippo, G.; Judica, A.; Spinelli, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Test di Denominazione Rapida e Ricerca Visiva di Colori, Figure e Numeri. [Rapid Naming Test and Visual Search of Colours, Figures, and Numbers]; IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia: Rome, Italy, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  41. Marinelli, C.V.; Zoccolotti, P.; Romani, C. The Ability to Learn New Written Words Is Modulated by Language Orthographic Consistency. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0228129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY-II; Pearson: London, UK, 2007; ISBN 978-0-15-823427-4. [Google Scholar]
  43. Stambak, M. Le Problème du Rythme Dans le Développement de l’enfant et Dans les Dyslexies d’évolution. Enfance 1951, 4, 480–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Ireland, K.; Parker, A.; Foster, N.; Penhune, V. Rhythm and Melody Tasks for School-Aged Children with and without Musical Training: Age-Equivalent Scores and Reliability. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Goswami, U.; Thomson, J.; Richardson, U.; Stainthorp, R.; Hughes, D.; Rosen, S.; Scott, S.K. Amplitude Envelope Onsets and Developmental Dyslexia: A New Hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 10911–10916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Huss, M.; Verney, J.P.; Fosker, T.; Mead, N.; Goswami, U. Music, Rhythm, Rise Time Perception and Developmental Dyslexia: Perception of Musical Meter Predicts Reading and Phonology. Cortex 2011, 47, 674–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Richardson, U.; Thomson, J.M.; Scott, S.K.; Goswami, U. Auditory Processing Skills and Phonological Representation in Dyslexic Children. Dyslexia 2004, 10, 215–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Pasinski, A.C.; McAuley, J.D.; Snyder, J.S. How Modality Specific Is Processing of Auditory and Visual Rhythms?: Rhythm Processing. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Facoetti, A.; Trussardi, A.N.; Ruffino, M.; Lorusso, M.L.; Cattaneo, C.; Galli, R.; Molteni, M.; Zorzi, M. Multisensory Spatial Attention Deficits Are Predictive of Phonological Decoding Skills in Developmental Dyslexia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1011–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hahn, N.; Foxe, J.J.; Molholm, S. Impairments of Multisensory Integration and Cross-Sensory Learning as Pathways to Dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 384–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Ye, Z.; Rüsseler, J.; Gerth, I.; Münte, T.F. Audiovisual Speech Integration in the Superior Temporal Region Is Dysfunctional in Dyslexia. Neuroscience 2017, 356, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Franceschini, S.; Gori, S.; Ruffino, M.; Viola, S.; Molteni, M.; Facoetti, A. Action Video Games Make Dyslexic Children Read Better. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 462–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Franceschini, S.; Bertoni, S.; Ronconi, L.; Molteni, M.; Gori, S.; Facoetti, A. “Shall We Play a Game?”: Improving Reading through Action Video Games in Developmental Dyslexia. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2015, 2, 318–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Franceschini, S.; Trevisan, P.; Ronconi, L.; Bertoni, S.; Colmar, S.; Double, K.; Facoetti, A.; Gori, S. Action Video Games Improve Reading Abilities and Visual-to-Auditory Attentional Shifting in English-Speaking Children with Dyslexia. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 5863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Reading speed z-scores under the three conditions (i.e., Visual cue, Rhythm, No intervention) at each timepoint (i.e., Pre, Post, Follow-up).
Figure 1. Reading speed z-scores under the three conditions (i.e., Visual cue, Rhythm, No intervention) at each timepoint (i.e., Pre, Post, Follow-up).
Applsci 12 03360 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviations) of participants’ characteristics and baseline reading measures.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviations) of participants’ characteristics and baseline reading measures.
Visual CueRhythmNo Intervention
Age10.7 (1.71)10.6 (1.41)11.0 (1.89)
Sex 1
Male12 (57.1)12 (57.1)8 (50.0)
Female9 (42.9)9 (42.9)8 (50.0)
Total IQ 2104 (13.3)104 (10.3)101 (10.4)
Pre-test reading
Speed (z-scores)−1.81 (0.71)−1.73 (0.69)−1.66 (0.57)
Accuracy (z-scores)−2.62 (2.17)−2.11 (1.49)−3.25 (2.72)
1 Frequencies (Percentages). 2 Total IQ composite score derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Forth Edition.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cancer, A.; De Salvatore, M.; Granocchio, E.; Andreoli, L.; Antonietti, A.; Sarti, D. The Role of Auditory and Visual Components in Reading Training: No Additional Effect of Synchronized Visual Cue in a Rhythm-Based Intervention for Dyslexia. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12073360

AMA Style

Cancer A, De Salvatore M, Granocchio E, Andreoli L, Antonietti A, Sarti D. The Role of Auditory and Visual Components in Reading Training: No Additional Effect of Synchronized Visual Cue in a Rhythm-Based Intervention for Dyslexia. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(7):3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12073360

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cancer, Alice, Marinella De Salvatore, Elisa Granocchio, Luca Andreoli, Alessandro Antonietti, and Daniela Sarti. 2022. "The Role of Auditory and Visual Components in Reading Training: No Additional Effect of Synchronized Visual Cue in a Rhythm-Based Intervention for Dyslexia" Applied Sciences 12, no. 7: 3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12073360

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop