Attentional Fluctuations, Cognitive Flexibility, and Bilingualism in Kindergarteners
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Controversy of the Bilingual Advantage in Cognitive Control
1.2. The Role of Attentional Fluctuations in Cognitive Control
1.3. Defining and Measuring Bilingualism
1.4. Bilingualism and the Context of California
1.5. The Present Study
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Demographic Characteristics.
2.2.2. Second Language (L2) Exposure and Age of Acquisition (AoA)
2.2.3. Cognitive Flexibility
2.2.4. Attentional Fluctuations.
2.3. Analytic Plan
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations
3.2. Regression Analyses with Bilingualism as a Continuous Variable
3.3. Group Comparison with Bilingualism as a Categorical Variable
4. Discussion
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Antoniou, M. The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2018, 5, 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donnelly, S. Re-Examining the Bilingual Advantage on Interference-Control and Task-Switching Tasks: A Meta-Analysis. Ph.D. Dissertation, Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, USA, 2016. Available online: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/762 (accessed on 21 February 2019).
- Grundy, J.G.; Timmer, K. Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Lang. Res. 2017, 33, 325–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hilchey, M.D.; Klein, R.M. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011, 18, 625–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laine, M.; Lehtonen, M. Cognitive consequences of bilingualism: Where to go from here? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 33, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehtonen, M.; Soveri, A.; Laine, A.; Järvenpää, J.; De Bruin, A.; Antfolk, J. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paap, K.R.; Johnson, H.A.; Sawi, O. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 2015, 69, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuel, S.; Roehr-Brackin, K.; Pak, H.; Kim, H. Cultural Effects Rather Than a Bilingual Advantage in Cognition: A Review and an Empirical Study. Cogn. Sci. 2018, 42, 2313–2341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Struys, E.; Duyck, W.; Woumans, E. The Role of Cognitive Development and Strategic Task Tendencies in the Bilingual Advantage Controversy. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Bruin, A.; Treccani, B.; Della Sala, S. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isbell, E.; Calkins, S.D.; Swingler, M.M.; Leerkes, E.M. Attentional fluctuations in preschoolers: Direct and indirect relations with task accuracy, academic readiness, and school performance. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 167, 388–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unsworth, N. Consistency of attentional control as an important cognitive trait: A latent variable analysis. Intelligence 2015, 49, 110–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, B.; Krott, A. Bilingualism enhances attentional control in non-verbal conflict tasks—Evidence from ex-Gaussian analyses. Biling. Cogn. 2016, 21, 162–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, E.K. The prefontral cortex and cognitive control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 1, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. The Bilingual Adaptation: How Minds Accommodate Experience. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 233–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alladi, S.; Bak, T.H.; Duggirala, V.; Surampudi, B.; Shailaja, M.; Shukla, A.K.; Chaudhuri, J.R.; Kaul, S. Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology 2013, 81, 1938–1944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Greenberg, Z.I. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 66, 232–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antón, E.; Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A. The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0206770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Costa, A.; Hernández, M.; Costa-Faidella, J.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 2009, 113, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antón, E.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Estévez, A.; Hernández, J.A.; Castillo, A.; Fuentes, L.J.; Davidson, D.J.; Carreiras, M. Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 398. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, E. Bilingualism and the Development of Executive Function: The Role of Attention. Child Dev. Perspect. 2015, 9, 117–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, T.H. The impact of bilingualism on cognitive ageing and dementia. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2016, 6, 205–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Haynes, B.I.; Bauermeister, S.; Bunce, D. Does within-person variability predict errors in healthy adults aged 18–90? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ihle, A.; Ghisletta, P.; Kliegel, M. Prospective memory and intraindividual variability in ongoing task response times in an adult lifespan sample: The role of cue focality. Memory 2017, 25, 370–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buzy, W.M.; Medoff, D.R.; Schweitzer, J.B. Intra-Individual Variability among Children with ADHD on a Working Memory Task: An Ex-Gaussian Approach. Child Neuropsychol. 2009, 15, 441–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walker, M.P.; Ayre, G.A.; Cummings, J.L.; Wesnes, K.; McKeith, I.G.; O’brien, J.T.; Ballard, C.G. Quantifying fluctuation in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia. Neurology 2000, 54, 1616–1625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adesope, O.O.; Lavin, T.; Thompson, T.; Ungerleider, C. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Cognitive Correlates of Bilingualism. Rev. Educ. Res. 2010, 80, 207–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paap, K.R.; Anders-Jefferson, R.; Mason, L.; Alvarado, K.; Zimiga, B. Bilingual Advantages in Inhibition or Selective Attention: More Challenges. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovelman, I.; Baker, S.A.; Petitto, L.-A. Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Biling. Cogn. 2008, 11, 203–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bedore, L.M.; Peña, E.D.; Summers, C.L.; Boerger, K.M.; Resendiz, M.D.; Greene, K.; Bohman, T.M.; Gillam, R.B. The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Biling. Cogn. 2012, 15, 616–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canseco-Gonzalez, E.; Brehm, L.; Brick, C.A.; Brown-Schmidt, S.; Fischer, K.; Wagner, K. Carpet or Cárcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Cogn. Process. 2010, 25, 669–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davison, M.D.; Hammer, C.S. Development of 14 English grammatical morphemes in Spanish–English preschoolers. Clin. Linguistics Phon. 2012, 26, 728–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silverberg, S.; Samuel, A.G. The effect of age of second language acquisition on the representation and processing of second language words. J. Mem. 2004, 51, 381–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archila-Suerte, P.; Zevin, J.; Hernandez, A.E. The effect of age of acquisition, socioeducational status, and proficiency on the neural processing of second language speech sounds. Brain Lang. 2015, 141, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klein, D.; Mok, K.; Chen, J.-K.; Watkins, K.E. Age of language learning shapes brain structure: A cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain Lang. 2014, 131, 20–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, X.; Tu, L.; Wang, J.; Jiang, B.; Gao, W.; Pan, X.; Li, M.; Zhong, M.; Zhu, Z.; Niu, M.; et al. Onset age of L2 acquisition influences language network in early and late Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Brain Lang. 2017, 174, 16–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abrahamsson, N. Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2012, 34, 187–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynch, A. Bilingualism and Second Language Acquisition; Springer Nature: Basingstoke, UK, 2017; Volume 109, pp. 43–55. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas-Sunesson, D.; Hakuta, K.; Bialystok, E. Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2018, 21, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mukadam, N.; Sommerlad, A.; Livingston, G. The Relationship of Bilingualism Compared to Monolingualism to the Risk of Cognitive Decline or Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 2017, 58, 45–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchman, V.A.; Martínez-Sussmann, C. Concurrent Validity of Caregiver/Parent Report Measures of Language for Children Who Are Learning Both English and Spanish. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2002, 45, 983–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, J.A.; Mak, L.; Chahi, A.K.; Bialystok, E. The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 250–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abutalebi, J.; Weekes, B.S. The Cognitive Neurology of Bilingualism in the Age of Globalization. Behav. Neurol. 2014, 2014, 536727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Snyder, T.D.; Dillow, S. Digest of Education Statistics 2013; National Center for Education Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2015.
- California Department of Education. Facts about English Learners in California—CalEdFacts; Data Reporting Office: Sacramento, CA, USA, 2019.
- Haman, E.; Wodniecka, Z.; Marecka, M.; Szewczyk, J.; Białecka-Pikul, M.; Otwinowska, A.; Mieszkowska, K.; Łuniewska, M.; Kołak, J.; Miękisz, A.; et al. How Does L1 and L2 Exposure Impact L1 Performance in Bilingual Children? Evidence from Polish-English Migrants to the United Kingdom. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearson, B.Z. Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2007, 28, 399–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zelazo, P.D.; Anderson, J.E.; Richler, J.; Wallner-Allen, K.; Beaumont, J.L.; Weintraub, S. II. NIH Toolbox cognition battery (cb): Measuring executive function and attention. Monogr. Soc. Child Dev. 2013, 78, 16–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isquith, P.K.; Roth, R.M.; Gioia, G.A. Tasks of Executive Control: Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Morey, R.D.; Rouder, J.N.; Jamil, T. BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs, R Package Version 0.9.9; 2015. Available online: https://rdrr.io/cran/BayesFactor/ (accessed on 22 February 2019).
- Dienes, Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jeffreys, H. Theory of Probability, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Morton, J.B.; Harper, S.N. What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 719–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wei, M.; Joshi, A.A.; Zhang, M.; Mei, L.; Manis, F.R.; He, Q.; Beattie, R.L.; Xue, G.; Shattuck, D.W.; Leahy, R.M.; et al. How age of acquisition influences brain architecture in bilinguals. J. Neurolinguistics 2015, 36, 35–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Poarch, G.J.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Dev. Rev. 2015, 35, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wiseheart, M.; Viswanathan, M.; Bialystok, E. Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cognit. 2016, 19, 141–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duñabeitia, J.A.; Hernández, J.A.; Antón, E.; Macizo, P.; Estévez, A.; Fuentes, L.J.; Carreiras, M. The Inhibitory Advantage in Bilingual Children Revisited. Exp. Psychol. 2014, 61, 234–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prior, A.; Gollan, T.H. The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 622–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Von Bastian, C.C.; Souza, A.S.; Gade, M. No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. 2016, 145, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antón, E.; García, Y.F.; Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? J. Mem. 2016, 90, 147–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.; Luk, G. Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 240–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gathercole, V.C.M.; Thomas, E.M.; Kennedy, I.; Prys, C.; Young, N.; Viñas-Guasch, N.; Roberts, E.J.; Hughes, E.K.; Jones, L. Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chung-Fat-Yim, A.; Sorge, G.B.; Bialystok, E. The relationship between bilingualism and selective attention in young adults: Evidence from an ambiguous figures task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 366–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treccani, B.; Mulatti, C. No matter who, no matter how … and no matter whether the white matter matters. Why theories of bilingual advantage in executive functioning are so difficult to falsify. Cortex 2015, 73, 349–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartsuiker, R.J. Why it is pointless to ask under which specific circumstances the bilingual advantage occurs. Cortex 2015, 73, 336–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Redick, T.S.; Hulme, C. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 11, 512–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jylkkä, J.; Soveri, A.; Laine, M.; Lehtonen, M. Assessing bilingual language switching behavior with ecological momentary assessment. Presented at the Third Learning and Plasticity Meeting, Akäslompolo, Finland, 2–5 April 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, D.M. PPVT-4: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; Pearson Assessments: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.M.; Padilla, E.R.; Lugo, D.E.; Dunn, L.M. Manual del Examinador para el Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
Overall Sample (N = 120) | No L2 Exposure (N = 16) | Some L2 Exposure (N = 104) | L2 Exposure Since Birth (Subsample) (N = 24) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD)/% | Mean (SD)/% | Mean (SD)/% | Mean (SD)/% | |
Age (y) | 5.68 (0.36) | 5.83 (0.36) | 5.66 (0.36) | 5.63 (0.37) |
Parent Education (y) | 16.6 (2.00) | 16.1 (1.95) | 16.7 (2.01) | 16.5 (2.39) |
Attentional Fluctuations | 47.1 (10.8) | 45.8 (8.3) | 47.3 (11.2) | 48.0 (13.5) |
Cognitive Flexibility | 101 (13.3) | 105 (12.5) | 101 (13.4) | 99 (12.1) |
Gender (% male) | 55.4 | 56.2 | 54.8 | 58.3 |
Ethnicity (%) | ||||
Hispanic/Latino | 17.5 | 6.25 | 19.2 | 16.7 |
Not Hispanic/Latino | 82.5 | 93.8 | 80.8 | 83.3 |
Race (%) | ||||
Asian | 22.5 | 12.5 | 24.0 | 50.0 |
Black | 1.67 | 0.00 | 1.92 | 0.00 |
White | 49.2 | 56.3 | 48.1 | 20.8 |
Multiracial | 22.5 | 25.0 | 22.1 | 20.8 |
Unknown | 4.17 | 6.25 | 3.85 | 8.33 |
L2 Exposure (y) | 3.19 (2.40) | 5.63 (0.37) | ||
Age of Acquisition (y) | 0.83 (1.49) | 0.00 (0.00) | ||
Second Language (% of sample) | ||||
Spanish | 53.3 | 29.2 | ||
Cantonese | 22.2 | 25.0 | ||
Mandarin | 7.78 | 4.17 | ||
Arabic | 3.33 | 0 | ||
French | 2.22 | 0 | ||
Ilocano | 2.22 | 4.17 | ||
Other | 8.89 | 37.5 |
Model 1 (Predicting Attentional Fluctuations) | ||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 54.14 | 10.31 | 5.25 | 1.04 × 10 −6 *** |
L2 Exposure | −0.66 | 0.69 | −0.95 | 0.34 |
AoA | −1.15 | 0.90 | −1.28 | 0.20 |
Race | −1.14 | 2.32 | −0.49 | 0.63 |
SES | −0.15 | 0.58 | −0.25 | 0.80 |
Model 2 (Predicting Cognitive Flexibility) | ||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t-value | p-value | |
(Intercept) | 88.17 | 13.34 | 6.61 | 3.00 × 10 −9 *** |
L2 Exposure | 0.044 | 0.89 | 0.05 | 0.96 |
AoA | −0.081 | 1.20 | −0.07 | 0.95 |
Race | 1.48 | 3.02 | 0.49 | 0.63 |
SES | 0.70 | 0.76 | 0.91 | 0.36 |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Haft, S.L.; Kepinska, O.; Caballero, J.N.; Carreiras, M.; Hoeft, F. Attentional Fluctuations, Cognitive Flexibility, and Bilingualism in Kindergarteners. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9050058
Haft SL, Kepinska O, Caballero JN, Carreiras M, Hoeft F. Attentional Fluctuations, Cognitive Flexibility, and Bilingualism in Kindergarteners. Behavioral Sciences. 2019; 9(5):58. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9050058
Chicago/Turabian StyleHaft, Stephanie L., Olga Kepinska, Jocelyn N. Caballero, Manuel Carreiras, and Fumiko Hoeft. 2019. "Attentional Fluctuations, Cognitive Flexibility, and Bilingualism in Kindergarteners" Behavioral Sciences 9, no. 5: 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9050058
APA StyleHaft, S. L., Kepinska, O., Caballero, J. N., Carreiras, M., & Hoeft, F. (2019). Attentional Fluctuations, Cognitive Flexibility, and Bilingualism in Kindergarteners. Behavioral Sciences, 9(5), 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9050058