Next Article in Journal
Feeding, Muscle and Packaging Effects on Lipid Oxidation and Color of Avileña Negra-Ibérica Beef
Next Article in Special Issue
Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals
Previous Article in Journal
Species-Level Gut Microbiota Analysis after Antibiotic-Induced Dysbiosis in Horses
Previous Article in Special Issue
Function of Trunk-Mediated “Greeting” Behaviours between Male African Elephants: Insights from Choice of Partners
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks

1
Department of Biology, Western Kentucky University, Bowling Green, KY 42101, USA
2
Department of Environmental Science and Policy, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(10), 2860; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102860
Submission received: 30 August 2021 / Revised: 24 September 2021 / Accepted: 28 September 2021 / Published: 30 September 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Elephant Communication)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Among all taxa, messages transmitted via chemical signals are the oldest and most universal. For Asian and African elephants, odors convey information between individuals, and between elephants and their environment. Pheromones are chemical signals used within a species and while thousands of pheromones have been identified for insects, only a few dozen have been elucidated in mammals and other vertebrates. Amazingly, two pheromones are known for Asian elephants: one signals receptivity in females and the other a heightened reproductive state in males. The elephant trunk serves numerous functions including detecting airborne odors and transporting chemicals in substrates such as urine to be detected by multiple sensory systems. Obvious trunk behaviors provide clear means to assess the interest of elephants in scent sources. Thus, elephants can serve as a model system for investigating chemical signaling. Prior to the 21st century, research on elephant chemical signaling focused on within species communication. In the 21st century, these studies expanded with increasing fieldwork. Studies also revealed the use of odors to detect threats and forage for food. Chemical signaling in elephants remains a bouquet for further exploration with promising applications for the conservation of wild elephants and the management of elephants in human care.

Abstract

Chemical signals are the oldest and most ubiquitous means of mediating intra- and interspecific interactions. The three extant species of elephants, the Asian elephant and the two African species, savanna and forest share sociobiological patterns in which chemical signals play a vital role. Elephants emit secretions and excretions and display behaviors that reveal the importance of odors in their interactions. In this review, we begin with a brief introduction of research in elephant chemical ecology leading up to the 21st century, and then we summarize the body of work that has built upon it and occurred in the last c. 20 years. The 21st century has expanded our understanding on elephant chemical ecology, revealing their use of odors to detect potential threats and make dietary choices. Furthermore, complementary in situ and ex situ studies have allowed the careful observations of captive elephants to be extended to fieldwork involving their wild counterparts. While important advances have been made in the 21st century, further work should investigate the roles of chemical signaling in elephants and how these signals interact with other sensory modalities. All three elephant species are threatened with extinction, and we suggest that chemical ecology can be applied for targeted conservation efforts.

1. Introduction

Field studies from all three elephant species (Asian elephants, Elephas maximus; African savanna elephants, Loxodonta africana; and African forest elephants, L. cyclotis) indicate common sociobiological patterns [1,2]. Females are at the center of elephant social structures in which they exhibit philopatry and retain close bonds with their natal groups for life [3,4,5,6]. These groups are multileveled with older females exhibiting dominance over younger ones, and social organization is maintained by fission-fusion processes [2,4,7,8]. Upon reaching sexual maturity, male elephants remain on the periphery of these female-centered groups; younger males often form social relationships with each other before becoming more solitary at their sexual peak [9,10,11,12]. Male and female elephants are generally spatially segregated, except during periods of reproduction when females are in estrus and/or males are in a heightened reproductive period called “musth” [13,14,15]. While musth occurs regularly (although asynchronously between males), female elephants are only in estrus for several days every four to five years due to long gestation and weaning periods [16,17,18,19]. Therefore, synchronized breeding is critical, and elephants must navigate complex social environments to reproduce. Elephants rely on a milieu of chemical signals to announce their reproductive intent and to ascertain the receptivity of conspecifics. All three species are megaherbivores with broad yet selective palates [1,20,21]. Because of their size, elephants are considered largely free from predation. Nevertheless, large carnivores can be a threat to young, sick, and old elephants, and humans have hunted elephants and their relatives for millennia [22]. Thus, elephants like other species are likely to use chemical signals in food selection and predator detection.
By the end of the 20th century, we had a reasonable understanding of the importance chemical signals play in elephant society. A deeper knowledge existed about chemical signaling in Asian elephants, but the commonality of behaviors, similarities in estrus and musth, and general social dynamics of the species supported hypotheses that African elephants would use chemical signals and perhaps specific pheromones for inter- and intra-sexual communication. More work was needed in understanding the mechanisms of pheromone synthesis and reception. The degree of chemoreception sensitivity by elephants remained to be explored. Of course, chemical signals do not work in isolation. Elephants have excellent hearing and sensitive touch capabilities as well as reasonable eyesight. These senses are covered elsewhere in this issue, so they will not be reviewed here. Nevertheless, we realize the need for research on the interactions across the senses in both perception and signaling [23].
Our enhanced understanding on the importance of chemical signaling to elephants has since prompted investigations into the potential applications in science, husbandry, and management. We first give a background on elephant chemical ecology up to the 21st century; earlier reviews cover these findings in greater depth. Here, we strive to update our knowledge of elephant chemical ecology with research that has occurred over the past twenty years for both pure and applied purposes. Because so little work has been conducted on the chemical ecology of forest elephants, the use of “African elephants” throughout this review relates only to the savanna species (L. africana).

2. Background up to Turn of 21st Century

By the turn of the current century, intraspecific chemical signaling by Asian and African elephants had received considerable attention (e.g., reviews included [24,25,26,27,28]). One of the critical findings up to this time was the determination that African elephants exhibited musth similar to what had been known for centuries by Asian elephants [14,15,29]. Males of both species dribble urine, secrete an odorous, viscous fluid from the temporal gland (i.e., temporal gland secretions, TGS), can act aggressively, and focus their energies on locating receptive females (reviewed in [23,30,31,32]). Common aspects of musth in behavior and apparent function supported investigating other similarities in the chemical ecology of Asian and African elephants. A second landmark discovery was the identification of the pre-ovulatory pheromone, (Z)-7-dodecen-1-yl (Z7-12:Ac) in the Asian elephant [33,34]. The compound is common in lepidopteran chemical signals, revealing an intriguing example of convergent evolution. With only a few known pheromones in mammals at the time, the identification of a single compound that tracks the receptivity of a female Asian elephant bordered on ground-breaking. Chemical signals appeared to be mediating intersexual elephant reproductive behavior with musth excretions and secretions attractive to females, and at least in Asian elephants, a urinary signal that attracts males.
Chemical signals also play a role for interactions within the sexes. The chemical milieu of musth changes over its duration in Asian elephants with distinct stages [31,35]. In captivity, male Asian elephants distinguish musth and nonmusth urine [36]. African elephants display not only similar behaviors but also hormonal profiles and compounds from TGS comparable to Asian elephants in musth [37]. Males, especially younger ones, may avoid musth males or their secretions [30,38,39]. In Asian and African elephants, the chemistry of TGS, breath, and urine before and during musth was explored with single compounds (e.g., 2-butanone as an indicator of impending musth) or types of compounds (e.g., ketones, alcohols, and aldehydes) as candidates for chemical signals in one or both species [26,40]. Females also respond to chemicals released or emitted by same sex conspecifics (e.g., [41]). Before the 21st century, no compounds from these sources were isolated as chemical signals. Because the secretions and excretions from elephants are so chemically complex, the possibility that chemical signals are composed of blends or bouquets rather than single compounds also was considered likely.
In addition to the behavior and chemistry of these inter- and intra-sexual signals, the anatomy of the structures and physiology of the senders and receivers also had been examined [26,27,30,35]. The elephant trunk serves to sample volatile compounds in the air and chemicals in a liquid medium such as TGS and urine [42]. The African elephant trunk terminates in dorsal and ventral tips, while the Asian trunk exhibits only the dorsal. The tips house sensory vellus vibrissae that likely play tactile and chemosensory roles [43]. Mucus drips from the trunk orifice and proteins can bind with chemicals in liquids such as urine [26]. These anatomical features provide the structural foundation for the observed trunk behaviors.
The availability of discrete, obvious chemosensory behaviors by elephants makes them excellent models for studies on chemical signaling (Figure 1). The trunk can hover over a sample or be lifted to sniff the air, but it also can check a liquid by contact with the trunk tip or even flatten onto the substrate (a place behavior). Once the substrate is contacted, the trunk can transfer chemicals to the openings of the vomeronasal organ in the roof of its mouth in a behavior known as flehmen, which can serve as an indicator of male interest in a female or her urine [44]. For example, in a study with captive Asian elephants, the chemosensory responses of females to male urine depended on the estrous condition of the female and the stage of musth/nonmusth of the male (as determined by serum progesterone and testosterone, respectively) [45].
Compared to intraspecific signaling, the study of interspecific chemical signals and odors used by elephants has received less attention. Elephants are megaherbivore browsers and grazers with a broad, yet selective diet in that while they can feed on numerous plant species, they tend to prefer relatively few [21,47,48,49,50,51,52]. From these studies and others, elephant selectivity is based upon plant quality and the degree of plant chemical defenses. Similar to carnivores detecting prey via scent, elephants use the chemical composition of plants to make diet choices, but at the time further work was needed to understand the bases of such choices. Because of their large size as adults, elephants are relatively free from predation, except when elderly or ill. Young, smaller individuals also are susceptible to predation, especially by lions (Panthera leo) but also by other predators such as tigers (P. tigris), crocodiles (Crocodylus spp.), and possibly hyenas (Crocuta and Hyaena spp.) [53]. Humans have posed the greatest threat to elephants both through direct killing and indirect effects that include climate change and habitat modification [54]. The extent to which odors play a role in the detection of predators or human threats also needed further investigation.

3. 21st Century Gains in Understanding

3.1. Introduction

The importance of chemical sending and receiving to elephants continues to be solidified in the new century. One of the most powerful recent discoveries in this vein determined that the African elephant genome contained the largest repertoire of olfactory receptor (OR) genes (some 2000) of the 13 placental mammals investigated [55]. When intact gene clusters were determined, elephants had a similar quantity of clusters, but with more genes per cluster, compared to mice and humans. The authors conjectured that the number of OR genes correlates with olfactory discriminatory ability. In a study along such lines with Asian elephants, trained individuals could discriminate among 11 aliphatic acetic esters [56]. The compounds used had been tested with other species, and elephants showed a similar or faster rate of learning to discriminate. In addition, elephants performed better at these olfactory than comparable visual trials. In further exploration of their olfactory abilities, an additional homologous series of aliphatic compounds were tested along with enantiomeric pairs [57]. Elephants were capable of such discrimination within like pairs, and a ten-fold reduction in concentration did not affect their ability. African elephants have been trained to successfully identify trinitrotoluene (TNT) used in land mines [58]. Thus, compounds that may have intra- or inter-specific meaning and even those that likely have no evolutionary context can be perceived and discriminated by elephants.
The anatomy of the chemoreceptive sensory system has received somewhat less attention than the behavioral aspects. The main olfactory bulb in elephants is similar to other mammals with its large size being a notable feature [59]. Interestingly, despite the prevalence of vomerolfactory behavior (i.e., flehmen responses) exhibited by elephants, no accessory olfactory bulb has been located as is found in many mammals [60,61]. The VNO develops in the womb and appears fully functional in newborns, supporting the observation of flehmen-like behaviors in young calf elephants [62,63]. Two decades ago, interdigital glands were identified in Asian elephants that are akin to human eccrine glands [64,65]. We still do not understand the function of these glands and if they contribute to chemical signaling by elephants. Another intriguing finding that has potential for chemical signaling is the antimicrobial properties of TGS [66]. While this could have evolved as a defensive mechanism against infection, the possibility exists that the microbes play a role in chemical signaling as the TGS streams down the cheek and is rubbed on objects in the environment.

3.2. Intraspecific Chemical Signaling and Capabilities

Chemical signaling plays an important role both within and between the sexes in elephants (Figure 2). We illustrate some of the landmark discoveries in this section.
In the new century, perhaps the most profound contribution recently was the identification of the compound frontalin (1,5-dimethyl-6,8-dioxabicyclo[3.2.1]octane) as a musth pheromone in the TGS of Asian elephants [67]. Frontalin is not contained in the temporal secretions of young males; these males release a sweet, honey-like TGS [68]. As males age, frontalin levels increase and the ratio of the two enantiomeric forms changes from a greater proportion of the (+) [1R,5S] enantiomer to an almost racemic mixture in older, mid-musth males [69]. Female Asian elephants in their follicular phase distinguish among ratios and concentration of frontalin [70], but this line of investigation was never completed (Dr. L.E.L. Rasmussen was conducting this investigation when she passed away). A study by LaDue et al. [46] used a range of concentrations of racemic frontalin in bioassays with male and female Asian elephants at zoological facilities throughout North America. Older males that had undergone musth were more responsive than younger males who had not been through musth. Similarly, older, sexually experienced females also were more responsive than their younger counterparts. The roles of frontalin still require further investigation; the compound is not only present in TGS but also elephant blood, breath, and urine [30,71].
Research on pheromones in African elephants used the progress with Asian elephants as a starting point. Chemical analyses of urinary volatiles from captive and wild African elephant males revealed similarities across location and provided prospective signals of musth [72]. The Asian musth pheromone, frontalin, also occurs in the ovulatory urine of African elephants [73]. However, bioassays of frontalin in water with wild elephants in Tanzania elicited no heightened responses compared to a vanilla extract control [74]. Additional study suggested that microbes in these secretions may play a role in the signals of musth [75,76,77]. Similar to Asian elephants, hormones, behavior, secretions, and excretions show complementary alterations in African male elephants during musth [78,79]. In general, hormones alter gene expression and cellular function, the effects of which are often observable through behavior. Yet, we also know that the environment (e.g., interactions with conspecifics) can modify behavior that affects hormonal activity. In this regard, we still need a better understanding of the interaction between hormones and intraspecific behavior in elephants.
In situ and ex situ studies with African elephants pursued pure and applied scientific objectives (e.g., [80,81]). Such studies led to an expanded chemosensory ethogram, incorporating not only the main chemosensory behaviors of sniff, check, place, and flehmen [45] but also accessory trunk behaviors [82]. While the displays of main and accessory chemosensory behaviors are not limited to intraspecific communication, their correlation was elucidated through such studies. Using these behaviors, the chemosensory behavior of elephants in response to conspecific urine and feces from two populations—one in Tanzania and the other in South Africa—showed similar patterns across age and sex classes, although somewhat different patterns emerged developmentally between the sexes across the populations [80,83]. Interestingly, chemosensory response patterns in response to luteal and estrous conspecific urine were similar by male African elephants housed at facilities in North America to males in the wild [84]. In the South African population at Addo Elephant National Park, males expressed chemosensory behaviors to a greater extent than females, thus appearing more reliant than females on signals emanating from conspecific feces and urine [85,86]. However, more work on female elephant olfaction needs to be conducted including the role of familiarity (e.g., see [87]) and the possibility of estrous synchrony mediated through chemical signals [88]. Together these findings support the generality of chemosensory behavior at least in African savanna elephants.
Following the discovery of the Asian elephant estrous and musth pheromones, studies were conducted to determine if such signals existed in African elephants. In bioassays at zoological facilities, male African elephants distinguished estrous status from urinary signals [84], yet even with the advent of new techniques that provided candidate compounds [73,89,90,91], a pre-ovulatory pheromone has not been identified. Like Asian elephant females, African elephant females can distinguish the estrous stage of conspecifics but unlike Asian elephants not through urinary signals [92]. In a study of male African elephants at zoological facilities, males performed significantly fewer flehmen behaviors to musth than nonmusth urine but did not show differential responses to urine from early and late stages of musth [93]. In addition, the responding elephants did not show different rates of olfactory behaviors, only flehmen that activates the vomeronasal system. Thus, behavioral studies support the existence of chemical signals—perhaps single compound pheromones—as indicators of estrus and musth in African elephant, but chemical identification and verification is lacking at the present. African elephant intraspecific signals may not be single compounds, but it also seems likely that they are not overly complicated chemical images [94].
While we have been aware of elephants’ abilities to detect intraspecific signals in breath, TGS, and urine, other potential primary or secondary sources of chemical signals are just starting or have yet to be investigated. Male Asian elephants exhibit greater interest in dung from pre- (follicular) compared to post-ovulatory (luteal) phase females [95]. African elephants use olfactory cues (e.g., urine-soaked earth) along pathways and around waterholes potentially to determine the identity and location of conspecifics [86,96,97,98]. Other secretions that potentially release pheromones for intraspecific signaling include saliva, mucus, and fluid from the ears and interdigital glands [24,99]. In particular, saliva may be a potentially rich source of pheromones (or hormones functionally serving as pheromones), as has been observed in other mammals [100,101,102]. The existence of salivary pheromones could explain trunk-to-mouth exchanges frequently observed between elephants [103,104]. Elephants also exhibit a variety of chemosensory behaviors when examining a dead conspecific, although we have little understanding of what information they might be gathering [86,105,106,107]. The ubiquity of intraspecific chemical signaling by elephants challenges our imagination as to the types of information they are gathering. As stated above, a better understanding of their natural products chemistry would help along with further work on the role of microbes [76,108] and anatomy. All these avenues of future research could inform us on not only intraspecific but also interspecific chemical signaling.

3.3. Interspecific Chemical Ecology

Animals should be expected to use chemical information from their predators to exhibit a wide range of appropriate responses based on where, when, and how the predator cue is perceived [109]. Except for humans and large cats, elephants are largely free from the danger of predators; in many cases, humans pose the biggest threat. African elephants distinguished between clothing worn by Maasai and Kamba tribesmen using olfactory and visual cues [110]. Their response to the Maasai clothing was more aggressive, perhaps reflective of the increased threat by this tribe in which a rite of passage for men involves spearing an elephant. Lions and tigers are known to prey on elephants where they co-occur, especially calves [53,111,112], and elephants can distinguish the sounds of these predators from controls, responding in a way indicative of delineating degree of threat [113,114]. Using dung from lion and other cats or animals not known to regularly prey on elephants as well as chemicals unique to lion dung, five semi-tame African elephants in South Africa responded in a manner indicative of greatest threat from the lion samples [115]. Interestingly, in this same study, elephant responses did not differentiate between lion and cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus), yet the latter would never prey on elephants. Further work is needed to determine if elephants are generalizing across large cats, large carnivores, carnivores in general, or some other categorization. For African elephants, perceived threat can affect movements and grouping behavior [7,113,116], and chemical cues are likely indicators of such threat.
In addition to detecting potential predators, the query arises as to what extent elephants may use odors to inform foraging decisions. One concern for elephants while foraging is the presence of bees. Elephants foraging on trees with hives in them may disrupt the hive, resulting in angry bees attacking them. The sound of angrily buzzing bees evokes alarm calls and avoid behavior in elephants [105]. The smell of honey is associated with bees, and honey odor couples with bee buzzing sounds may indicate danger to elephants [106]. The multimodal signal may be more meaningful and thus resistant to habituation compared to single modality signals [107]. In addition, plant secondary metabolites (PSMs) have been proposed to have evolved in part to reduce mammalian herbivory [117,118], although the relationship between types of herbivory and chemical responses can be complex [119,120]. Elephant foraging behavior has received attention regularly (e.g., [121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130]), with African elephants classified as mixed-feeders demonstrating selectivity in species and plant parts [131,132] that is influenced by nutritional value, the palatability:tannin ratio, and levels of plant toxins ([133] and citations therein). A study conducted with handled elephants in South Africa showed that foraging choices were based on plant volatiles, especially monoterpenes, that were correlated with PSM content [134,135]. Because male and female African elephants differ in their feeding patterns [121], it would be interesting to examine intersexual differences in preferences to plant volatiles. Asian elephants can use odor but not sound to locate hidden food [136], and they can assess the quantity of a single food type through olfaction [137]. Recently, African elephants were shown to detect olfactory cues associated with water [138]. The body of work to date suggests that odors play an important role in elephant foraging decisions, but more research is needed.

3.4. Conservation and Management Applications of Chemical Ecology of Elephants

All three elephant species are threatened with extinction [139,140,141], and many in situ and ex situ elephant populations are intensely managed to control dynamics such as movement, foraging patterns, health, and reproduction that maximize sustainability. The use of olfaction for intraspecific signaling, threat detection, and foraging suggests that a push-pull approach [142] could be useful for elephant management [143,144] (Figure 3).
The ex situ propagation of elephants may be vital for the long-term viability of E. maximus and L. africana, but even many established captive populations are currently unsustainable [1,145,146]. Therefore, elephant husbandry has been an ongoing area of study with interest in a variety of topics relevant to captive breeding efforts including health, welfare, and enrichment [147,148,149,150,151]. For most terrestrial mammals, the detection of odors is a major means of evaluating their environment and communicating with conspecifics [152], and thus scents have a high potential for enhancing welfare and providing enrichment [153]. However, the selection, implementation, and assessment of scents related to animal well-being can be difficult and requires additional research [154]. The indiscriminate application of chemical signals could result in sensory habituation to these odors, decreasing their efficacy in captive applications. Therefore, a goal-centered approach to olfactory enrichment is necessary, incorporating well-defined behavioral outcomes that indicate success or that suggest modifications are necessary [155,156]. For instance, because many captive elephants commonly exhibit stereotypies and inactivity [157], zoo elephant managers may wish to use pheromones to stimulate activity and increase behavioral diversity. The functionality of these pheromones is sex- and age-specific, so their application should be targeted. Indeed, a recent study of captive Asian elephants showed that while both sex pheromones can effectively increase behavioral diversity and activity, the degree to which these compounds function is influenced by sex, sexual experience, age, and social access [158]. One of the benefits of using pheromones is their inherent biological relevance; thus, we propose that they would be resistant to habituation. Behavioral and physical health is often important in determining reproductive success [159,160]. The ex situ propagation of other threatened mammals (e.g., the giant panda, Ailuropoda melanoleuca) has been bolstered by using chemical signals to stimulate reproductive function and activity [161]. Because of the unique reproductive biology of elephants and the artificial social environments of many captive elephants, sex pheromones may also hold promise in enhancing captive breeding efforts in these species. More experimental work is needed to assess how chemical signals could benefit elephant husbandry and captive sustainability.
In the wild, elephant conservation is complicated by the issues of illegal take by humans and competition with humans for limited resources in the forms of habitat, water, and food [162,163,164,165,166]. Chemical ecology has not played a major role in the issue of poaching, although the chemical composition of poached ivory can be used to trace the date of death [167] and DNA analysis helps to isolate location [168]. However, for mitigating human–wildlife conflicts (HWC) including human–elephant conflict (HEC), chemical ecology has been an important contributor [143]. Human–elephant interactions (HEI) can turn into conflict when the two species vie for a common, limited resource such as space, food, or water. HEC may not be an issue of too many elephants or people [169], but rather their distribution when resources are limited (e.g., water during a drought) or at a premium (e.g., crops approaching harvest). At the population level, carrying capacities for elephants have been estimated by considering the negative effects of PSMs on elephant foraging [170]. Intraspecific chemical signals are posited as honest means of communication and thus have potential for modifying the behavior of particular categories of elephants (e.g., adult males or females) in predictable ways [80]. To date, only pheromones for Asian elephants have been identified. Yet, even with Asian elephants, application of the pheromones in the field for conservation purposes has not been conducted in part because the compounds are not readily available, can be somewhat expensive, and a practical means of dispensing has not been formulated. Fortunately, research on the responses of African elephants to bee pheromones [171] has used SPLAT® (Specialized Pheromone and Lure Application Technology) formulated to reduce insect pests [172] in order to dispense the bee pheromones. This matrix might be useful for dispensing the Asian elephant pheromones as well.
In addition to bee pheromones, other odors have been used to deter elephants from human habitations and crops. One avenue is to grow aromatic plants that elephants avoid [173,174]. A problem so far with this approach is that aromatic plants often have medicinal or spice uses but do not provide sustenance. In contrast, sustenance crops such as rice and maize are not highly aromatic. Thus, aromatic plants may be consumed less by elephants, but they may not enhance food security for people. Chili peppers (Capsicum spp. or other hot chilis) have been used for as a deterrent in a variety of forms, including growing chilis around sustenance crops, making a motor-oil based extract and creating a cloth and rope fence, by burning, and via gas dispensers (e.g., see these papers and references therein: [175,176,177,178,179,180]). A recent study used high performance liquid chromatography coupled with mass spectrometry to quantify the two major active ingredients, capsaicin and dihydrocapsaicin, [181]. Success using chilis to protect crops has varied widely. The active ingredients are capsaicinods that activate the trigeminal nerve in the elephant trunk, leading to an aversive reaction. The reasons for variable success are potentially many. Further work is needed to examine the responses of elephants to known concentrations of the capsaicinoids. Predatory odors in the form of lion dung and its major, signatory chemical components evoked aversive behavior in handled elephants in South Africa [115]. Field testing is needed to see if these responses translate to effective deterrence in the wild. Santiapillai and Read [182] conjectured as to the possibility of masking the odor of ripening crops such as rice to reduce elephant consumption. General unpleasant odor mixtures also are displeasing to elephants also may aid in deterring elephants. For example, Oniba and Robertson [183] used a mixture of chili, garlic, ginger, neem leaves, eggs, and cow or elephant dung, which in this preliminary study was effective at reducing crop raiding. Rasmussen and Riddle [184] tried a mixture of plant and animal products as an olfactory deterrent, and they concluded that olfaction alone was insufficient to deter elephants from food. This is a good recommendation to keep in mind; it is unlikely that a single sensory ‘silver bullet’ will be identified to mitigate crop raiding and similar forms of human-elephant conflict. Nevertheless, chemical signals are vitally important to elephants, so their potential roles for husbandry and conservation should not be underestimated.

4. Conclusions

Elephants serve as an excellent model system for mammalian chemical ecology. Behavioral, biochemical, genetic, and physiological studies all point to elephants as highly reliant on their chemical senses. Their presence in zoological facilities, their capability of learning, and the relative ease at which they can be observed in the wild make them good candidates as a model system for further understanding mammalian chemical ecology [185]. African elephants use urinary odors to recognize conspecifics and create expectations of where particular individuals are located spatially [96]. African and Asian elephants can distinguish scents from different humans through operant conditioning [186,187]. Studies like these, the ability to work with and train elephants, and their distinct, readily displayed chemosensory behaviors show that elephants and their use of odors make them good subjects for the investigation of cognition related to olfaction in non-primate mammals.
Despite the social complexity and cognitive capabilities of elephants, single or a small set of compounds may well be important in orchestrating elephant society (e.g., [94] for chemical signaling in mammals). Challenges for the 21st century remain to identify additional pheromones in elephants and to examine further how these signals work in elephant society, especially in a multimodal way with other signal types (e.g., [188]). As Rasmussen and her colleagues have shown, the identification of the pheromone (Z)-7-dodecen-1-yl can be used as a tool to understand pheromone transport, associated physiological conditions, and condition-dependent behavioral responses. We should eagerly pursue identifying the intraspecific chemical signals (i.e., pheromones) and relevant context in elephants and all vertebrates [189]. Further, as illustrated in this review, numerous intraspecific and interspecific interactions are mediated at least in part by chemical signals in elephants. Thus, we should continue to explore the broad chemical ecology of elephants.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.A.S. and C.A.L.; investigation, B.A.S. and C.A.L.; writing—original draft preparation, B.A.S. and C.A.L.; writing—review and editing, B.A.S. and C.A.L.; visualization, C.A.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding directly; however, over the years multiple sources have provided support for research on elephants by the authors: Animal Behavior Society, Asian Elephant Support, American Society of Mammalogists, Cosmos Club Foundation, Earthwatch Institute, Elephant Managers Association, Explorers Club, Feld Entertainment Inc., Gates Foundation, IDEA WILD, International Elephant Foundation, Kentucky Academy of Sciences, the Richard Lounsbery Foundation, National Geographic Society, National Science Foundation, Oklahoma City Zoo Conservation Action Now, Saint Louis Zoo WildCare Institute, Sigma Xi, US Fulbright Student Program (US–Sri Lanka Fulbright Commission), and Virginia Academy of Science.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

Support over multiple decades of research on elephants by the authors has been provided by their respective institutions (Oregon Graduate Institute, Providence College, George Mason University, Georgia Southern University, and Western Kentucky University), and collaborators too numerous to mention. The authors are especially grateful for the enlightening insight gleaned through conversations with many people around the globe who have dedicated their lives and careers to elephants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sukumar, R. The Living Elephants: Evolutionary Ecology, Behavior, and Conservation; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; p. 478. [Google Scholar]
  2. De Silva, S.; Wittemyer, G. A Comparison of Social Organization in Asian Elephants and African Savannah Elephants. Int. J. Primatol. 2012, 33, 1125–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Moss, C.J.; Poole, J.H. Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In Primate Social Relationships: An Integrated Approach; Hinde, R.A., Ed.; Blackwell Scientific: Oxford, UK, 1983; pp. 315–325. [Google Scholar]
  4. Wittemyer, G.; Getz, W. Hierarchical dominance structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 671–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Fishlock, V.; Lee, P. Forest elephants: Fission–fusion and social arenas. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 357–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Schuttler, S.G.; Philbrick, J.A.; Jeffery, K.J.; Eggert, L.S. Fine-Scale Genetic Structure and Cryptic Associations Reveal Evidence of Kin-Based Sociality in the African Forest Elephant. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e88074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Wittemyer, G.; Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Getz, W. The socioecology of elephants: Analysis of the processes creating multitiered social structures. Anim. Behav. 2005, 69, 1357–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Archie, E.A.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. The ties that bind: Genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 513–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Evans, K.E.; Harris, S. Adolescence in male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, and the importance of sociality. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 779–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chiyo, P.I.; Archie, E.A.; Hollister-Smith, J.A.; Lee, P.; Poole, J.H.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. Association patterns of African elephants in all-male groups: The role of age and genetic relatedness. Anim. Behav. 2011, 81, 1093–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chiyo, P.I.; Wilson, J.W.; Archie, E.A.; Lee, P.C.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. The influence of forage, protected areas, and mating prospects on grouping patterns of male elephants. Behav. Ecol. 2014, 25, 1494–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Keerthipriya, P.; Nandini, S.; Vidya, T.N.C. Effects of Male Age and Female Presence on Male Associations in a Large, Polygynous Mammal in Southern India: The Asian Elephant. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2021, 9, 616666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Jainudeen, M.R.; McKay, G.M.; Eisenberg, J.F. Observations on musth in the domesticated Asiatic elephant (Elephas maximus). Mammalia 1972, 36, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Poole, J.H. Rutting behavior in African elephants: The phenomenon of musth. Behaviour 1987, 102, 283–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Poole, J.H.; Kasman, L.H.; Ramsay, E.C.; Lasley, B.L. Musth and urinary testosterone concentrations in the African elephant (Loxodonta africana). J. Reprod. Fertil. 1984, 70, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Sukumar, R. The Asian Elephant: Ecology and Management; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989; p. 255. [Google Scholar]
  17. Freeman, E.W.; Whyte, I.; Brown, J.L. Reproductive evaluation of elephants culled in Kruger National Park, South Africa between 1975 and 1995. Afr. J. Ecol. 2009, 47, 192–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Moss, C.J.; Lee, P.C. Female reproductive strategies: Individual life histories. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 187–204. [Google Scholar]
  19. Brown, J.L. Comparative reproductive biology of elephants. In Reproductive Sciences in Animal Conservation: Progress and Prospects; Holt, W.V., Brown, J.L., Comizzoli, P., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 135–169. [Google Scholar]
  20. Owen-Smith, R.N. Megaherbivores: The Influence of Very Large Body Size on Ecology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  21. Sukumar, R. Ecology of the Asian elephant in southern India. II. Feeding habits and crop raiding patterns. J. Trop. Ecol. 1990, 6, 33–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Agam, A.; Barkai, R. Elephant and mammoth hunting during the Paleolithic: A review of the relevant archaeological, ethnographic and ethno-historical records. Quaternary 2018, 1, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. LaDue, C.A.; Schulte, B.A.; Kiso, W.K.; Freeman, E.W. Musth and sexual selection in elephants: A review of signaling properties and potential fitness consequences. Behaviour 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B. Chemical signals in the reproduction of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 1998, 53, 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.A. Ecological and biochemical constraints on pheromonal signaling systems in Asian elephants and their evolutionary implications. In Advances in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates; Johnston, R.E., Muller-Schwarze, D., Sorenson, P.W., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  26. Rasmussen, L.E.L. Evolution of chemical signals in the Asian elephant, Elephas maximus: Behavioural and ecological influences. J. Biosci. 1999, 24, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rasmussen, L.E.L. Elephant olfaction: Smell detectors extraordinaire. ChemoSense 1999, 2, 4–5. [Google Scholar]
  28. Langbauer, W.R. Elephant communication. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 425–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Poole, J.H.; Moss, C.J. Musth in the African elephant, Loxodonta africana. Nature 1981, 292, 830–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Krishnamurthy, V. How chemical signals integrate Asian elephant society: The known and the unknown. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 405–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Schulte, B.A.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Musth, sexual selection, testosterone, and metabolites. In Advances in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates; Johnston, R.E., Müller-Schwarze, D., Sorensen, P.W., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 383–397. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ananth, D. Musth in elephants. Zoos Print J. 2000, 15, 259–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Lee, T.D.; Roelofs, W.L.; Zhang, A.; Daves, G.D., Jr. Insect pheromone in elephants. Nature 1996, 379, 684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Lee, T.D.; Zhang, A.; Roelofs, W.L.; Daves, G.D., Jr. Purification, identification, concentration and bioactivity of (Z)-7-dodecen-1-yl acetate: Sex pheromone of the female Asian elephant, Elephas maximus. Chem. Senses 1997, 22, 417–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Perrin, T.E. Physiological correlates of musth: Lipid metabolites and chemical composition of exudates. Physiol. Behav. 1999, 67, 539–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Scott, N.L.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Chemical communication of musth in captive male Asian elephants, Elephas maximus. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10; Mason, R.T., LeMaster, M.P., Müller-Schwarze, D., Eds.; Springer: Corvallis, OR, USA, 2005; pp. 118–127. [Google Scholar]
  37. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Hall-Martin, A.J.; Hess, D.L. Chemical profiles of male African elephants, Loxodonta africana: Physiological and ecological implications. J. Mammal. 1996, 77, 422–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Poole, J.H. Announcing intent: The aggressive state of musth in African elephants. Anim. Behav. 1989, 37, 140–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rasmussen, L.E.L. Chemosensory responses in two species of elephants to constituents of temporal gland secretion and musth urine. J. Chem. Ecol. 1988, 14, 1687–1711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Goodwin, T.E.; Rasmussen, E.L.; Guinn, A.C.; McKelvey, S.S.; Gunawardena, R.; Riddle, S.W.; Riddle, H.S. African Elephant Sesquiterpenes. J. Nat. Prod. 1999, 62, 1570–1572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rasmussen, L.E.L. Chemical communication: An integral part of functional Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) society. Écoscience 1998, 5, 410–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rasmussen, L.E.L. Chemical, tactile, and taste sensory systems. In Biology, Medicine, and Surgery of Elephants; Fowler, M.E., Mikota, S.K., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing: Ames, IA, USA, 2006; pp. 409–414. [Google Scholar]
  43. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Munger, B.L. The sensorineural specializations of the trunk tip (finger) of the Asian elephant, Elephas maximus. Anat. Rec. 1996, 246, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Rasmussen, L.E.; Schmidt, M.J.; Henneous, R.; Groves, D.; Daves, G.D., Jr. Asian bull elephants: Flehmen-like responses to extractable components in female elephant estrous urine. Science 1982, 217, 159–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Schulte, B.A.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Signal-receiver interplay in the communication of male condition by Asian elephants. Anim. Behav. 1999, 57, 1265–1274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. LaDue, C.A.; Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Concentration-dependent chemosensory responses towards pheromones are influenced by receiver attributes in Asian elephants. Ethology 2018, 124, 387–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bax, P.N.; Sheldrick, D.L.W. Some preliminary observations on the food of elephant in the Tsavo Royal National Park (East) of Kenya. East Afr. Wildl. J. 1963, 1, 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Field, C.R.; Ross, I.C. The savanna ecology of Kidepo Valley National Park, II. Feeding ecology of elephant and giraffe. East Afr. Wildl. J. 1976, 14, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Short, J. Diet and feeding behaviour of the forest elephant. Mammalia 1981, 45, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Tchamba, M.N.; Seme, P.M. Diet and feeding behaviour of the forest elephant in the Santchou Reserve, Cameroon. Afr. J. Ecol. 1993, 31, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. White, L.J.T.; Tutin, C.E.G.; Fernandez, M. Group composition and diet of forest elephants, Loxodonta africana cyclotis Matschie 1900, in the Lopé Reserve, Gabon. Afr. J. Ecol. 1993, 31, 181–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Stokke, S. Sex differences in feeding-patch choice in a megaherbivore: Elephants in Chobe National Park, Botswana. Can. J. Zool. 1999, 77, 1723–1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Loveridge, A.J.; Hunt, J.E.; Murindagomo, F.; Macdonald, D.W. Influence of drought on predation of elephant (Loxodonta africana) calves by lions (Panthera leo) in an African wooded savannah. J. Zool. 2006, 270, 523–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Naughton, L.; Rose, R.; Treves, A. The Social Dimensions of Human-Elephant Conflict in AFRICA: A Literature Review and Case Studies from Uganda and Cameroon; IUCN African Elephant Specialist Group, Human-Elephant Task Conflict Task Force: Glands, Switzerland, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  55. Niimura, Y.; Matsui, A.; Touhara, K. Extreme expansion of the olfactory receptor gene repertoire in African elephants and evolutionary dynamics of orthologous gene groups in 13 placental mammals. Genome Res. 2014, 24, 1485–1496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Arvidsson, J.; Amundin, M.; Laska, M. Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination test by Asian elephants, Elephas maximus. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 105, 809–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Rizvanovic, A.; Amundin, M.; Laska, M. Olfactory discrimination ability of Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) for structurally related odorants. Chem. Senses 2012, 38, 107–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Miller, A.K.; Hensman, M.C.; Hensman, S.; Schultz, K.; Reid, P.; Shore, M.; Brown, J.; Furton, K.G.; Lee, S. African elephants (Loxodonta africana) can detect TNT using olfaction: Implications for biosensor application. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ngwenya, A.; Patzke, N.; Ihunwo, A.O.; Manger, P.R. Organisation and chemical neuroanatomy of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana) olfactory bulb. Brain Struct. Funct. 2011, 216, 403–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Halpern, M.; Martínez-Marcos, A. Structure and function of the vomeronasal system: An update. Prog. Neurobiol. 2003, 70, 245–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Mohrhardt, J.; Nagel, M.; Fleck, D.; Ben-Shaul, Y.; Spehr, M. Signal detection and coding in the accessory olfactory system. Chem. Senses 2018, 43, 667–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Johnson, E.W.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Morphological characteristics of the vomeronasal organ of the newborn Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Anat. Rec. 2002, 267, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Goöbbel, L.; Fischer, M.S.; Smith, T.D.; Wible, J.R.; Bhatnagar, K.P. The vomeronasal organ and associated structures of the fetal African elephant, Loxodonta africana (Proboscidea, Elephantidae). Acta Zool. 2004, 85, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Lamps, L.W.; Smoller, B.R.; Goodwin, T.E.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Hormone receptor expression in interdigital glands of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Zoo Biol. 2004, 23, 463–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lamps, L.W.; Smoller, B.R.; Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Slade, B.E.; Fritsch, G.; Goodwin, T.E. Characterization of interdigital glands in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Res. Vet. Sci. 2001, 71, 197–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Meyer, W. Demonstration of lysozyme and antimicrobial peptides in the temporal gland of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana). Mamm. Biol. 2007, 72, 251–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Greenwood, D.R. Frontalin: A chemical message of musth in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Chem. Senses 2003, 28, 433–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Riddle, H.S.; Krishnamurthy, V. Mellifluous matures to malodorous in musth. Nature 2002, 415, 975–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Greenwood, D.R.; Comeskey, D.; Hunt, M.B.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Chirality in elephant pheromones. Nature 2005, 438, 1097–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Rasmussen, L.E.L.B.; Greenwood, D.R.; Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Asian elephant reflections: Chirality counts. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 13; Schulte, B.A., Goodwin, T.E., Ferkin, M.H., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 229–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Riddle, H.S. Elephant breath: Clues about health, disease, metabolism and social signals. J. Elephant Man. Assoc. 2004, 15, 24–33. [Google Scholar]
  72. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Wittemyer, G. Chemosignalling of musth by individual wild African elephants (Loxodonta africana): Implications for conservation and management. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2002, 269, 853–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Goodwin, T.E.; Eggert, M.S.; House, S.J.; Weddell, M.E.; Schulte, B.A.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Insect pheromones and precursors in female African elephant urine. J. Chem. Ecol. 2006, 32, 1849–1853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Castelda, S.M.; Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Investigating chemical signals in African elephants for convergence with insects and similarities with Asian elephants. In Proceedings of the 2007 International Elephant Conservation & Research Symposium, International Elephant Foundation, Orlando, FL, USA, 2–4 November 2007; pp. 81–91. [Google Scholar]
  75. Archie, E.A.; Theis, K.R. Animal behaviour meets microbial ecology. Anim. Behav. 2011, 82, 425–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Goodwin, T.E.; Broederdorf, L.J.; Burkert, B.A.; Hirwa, I.H.; Mark, D.B.; Waldrip, Z.J.; Kopper, R.A.; Sutherland, M.V.; Freeman, E.W.; Hollister-Smith, J.A.; et al. Chemical signals of elephant musth: Temporal aspects of microbially-mediated modifications. J. Chem. Ecol. 2012, 38, 81–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Goodwin, T.E.; Harelimana, I.H.; MacDonald, L.J.; Mark, D.B.; Juru, A.U.; Yin, Q.; Engman, J.A.; Kopper, R.A.; Lichti, C.F.; Mackintosh, S.G.; et al. The role of bacteria in chemical signals of elephant musth: Proximate causes and biochemical pathways. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 13; Schulte, B.A., Goodwin, T.E., Ferkin, M.H., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 63–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Ganswindt, A.; Heistermann, M.; Hodges, K. Physical, physiological, and behavioral correlates of musth in captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 2005, 78, 505–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Ganswindt, A.; Rasmussen, H.B.; Heistermann, M.; Hodges, J.K. The sexually active states of free-ranging male African elephants (Loxodonta africana): Defining musth and non-musth using endocrinology, physical signals, and behavior. Horm. Behav. 2005, 47, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Schulte, B.A.; Bagley, K.R.; Groover, M.; Loizi, H.; Merte, C.; Meyer, J.M.; Napora, E.; Stanley, L.; Vyas, D.K.; Wollett, K.; et al. Comparisons of state and likelihood of performing chemosensory event behaviors in two populations of African elephants (Loxodonta africana). In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11; Hurst, J., Beynon, R.J., Roberts, S.C., Wyatt, T., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2007; pp. 81–92. [Google Scholar]
  81. Schulte, B.A.; Loizi, H.; Bagley, K.; Gray, A.; Stanley, L.; Correll, M.; Goodwin, T.E.; Brown, P.A.; Davis, B.L.; Dill, W.M.; et al. Elephant chemotactile communication and conservation. J. Elephant Man. Assoc. 2004, 15, 16–23. [Google Scholar]
  82. Schulte, B.A.; Bagley, K.; Correll, M.; Gray, A.; Heineman, S.M.; Loizi, H.; Malament, M.; Scott, N.L.; Slade, B.E.; Stanley, L.; et al. Assessing chemical communication in elephants. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10; Mason, R.T., LeMaster, M.P., Müller-Schwarze, D., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2005; pp. 140–151. [Google Scholar]
  83. Schulte, B.A.; Bagley, K.R.; Castelda, S.; Loizi, H.; Nasseri, N.; Vyas, D.K.; Goodwin, T.E. From exploration to selective information gathering: The development of chemosensory investigation in male African elephants (Loxodonta africana). In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 12; East, M.L., Dehnhard, M., Eds.; Springer Science Business Media: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 135–143. [Google Scholar]
  84. Bagley, K.R.; Goodwin, T.E.; Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.A. Male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, can distinguish oestrous status via urinary signals. Anim. Behav. 2006, 71, 1439–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.; Goodwin, T.; Whitehouse, A.; Loizi, H. Sexual dimorphism in the performance of chemosensory investigatory behaviours by African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Behaviour 2009, 146, 373–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Merte, C.E.; Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Male and female developmental differences in chemosensory investigations by African elephants (Loxodonta africana) approaching waterholes. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2010, 64, 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Dürckheim, K.E.M.v. Olfaction and Scent Discrimination in African Elephants (Loxodonta africana); Stellenbosch University: Stellenbosch, South Africa, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  88. Weissenböck, N.M.; Schwammer, H.M.; Ruf, T. Estrous synchrony in a group of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) under human care. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 2009, 113, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Goodwin, T.E.; Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.A.; Brown, P.A.; Davis, B.L.; Dill, W.M.; Dowdy, N.C.; Hicks, A.R.; Morshedi, R.G.; Mwanza, D.; et al. Chemical analysis of preovulatory female African elephant urine: A search for putative pheromones. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10; Mason, R.T., LeMaster, M.P., Muller-Schwarze, D., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2005; pp. 128–139. [Google Scholar]
  90. Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Prospecting for mammalian chemical signals via solventless extraction techniques: An elephantine task. ChemoSense 2009, 11, 9–15. [Google Scholar]
  91. Goodwin, T.E.; Brown, P.A.; Eggert, M.S.; Evola, M.G.; House, S.J.; Morshedi, R.G.; Weddell, M.E.; Chen, C.J.; Jackson, S.R.; Aubut, Y.; et al. Use of automated solid phase dynamic extraction (SPDE)/GC-MS and novel macros in the search for African elephant pheromones. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11; Hurst, J., Beynon, R.J., Roberts, S.C., Wyatt, T., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 25–35. [Google Scholar]
  92. Meyer, J.M.; Goodwin, T.E.; Schulte, B.A. Intrasexual chemical communication and social responses of captive female African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 163–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Hollister-Smith, J.A.; Alberts, S.C.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Do male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, signal musth via urine dribbling? Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1829–1841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Apps, P.J. Are mammal olfactory signals hiding right under our noses? Naturwissenschaften 2013, 100, 487–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Ghosal, R.; Seshagiri, P.B.; Sukumar, R. Dung as a potential medium for inter-sexual chemical signaling in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Behav. Process. 2012, 91, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bates, L.A.; Sayialel, K.N.; Njiraini, N.W.; Poole, J.H.; Moss, C.J.; Byrne, R.W. African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biol. Lett. 2008, 4, 34–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Allen, C.R.B.; Brent, L.J.N.; Motsentwa, T.; Croft, D.P. Field evidence supporting monitoring of chemical information on pathways by male African elephants. Anim. Behav. 2021, 176, 193–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Esposito, R.M. Effect of Matriarchs on Group Interactions, Kinship Fitness, and Differences in Chemosensory Behavior of African Elephants (Loxodonta africana); Georgia Southern University: Statesboro, GA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  99. Riddle, H.S.; Riddle, S.W.; Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Goodwin, T.E. First disclosure and preliminary investigation of a liquid released from the ears of African elephants. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Gröschl, M. The physiological role of hormones in saliva. Bioessays 2009, 31, 843–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Booth, W.D. Factors affecting the pheromone composition of voided boar saliva. J. Reprod. Fertil. 1987, 81, 427–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Stopka, P.; Kuntová, B.; Klempt, P.; Havrdová, L.; Černá, M.; Stopková, R. On the saliva proteome of the Eastern European house mouse (Mus musculus musculus) focusing on sexual signalling and immunity. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 32481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Allen, C.R.B.; Croft, D.P.; Testard, C.; Brent, L.J.N. Function of trunk-mediated “greeting” behaviours between male African elephants: Insights from choice of partners. Animals 2021, 11, 2718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Poole, J.H.; Granli, P. Signals, gestures, and behavior of African elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 109–124. [Google Scholar]
  105. Sharma, N.; Pokharel, S.S.; Kohshima, S.; Sukumar, R. Behavioural responses of free-ranging Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) towards dying and dead conspecifics. Primates 2019, 61, 128–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Goldenberg, S.Z.; Wittemyer, G. Elephant behavior toward the dead: A review and insights from field observations. Primates 2020, 61, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Stephan, C.; Bahamboula, J.J.D.; Brncic, T.M. Responses to a poached conspecific in wild forest elephants (Loxodonta africana cyclotis). Behaviour 2020, 157, 823–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Budd, K.; Gunn, J.C.; Finch, T.; Klymus, K.; Sitati, N.; Eggert, L.S. Effects of diet, habitat, and phylogeny on the fecal microbiome of wild African savanna (Loxodonta africana) and forest elephants (L. cyclotis). Ecol. Evol. 2020, 10, 5637–5650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Parsons, M.H.; Apfelbach, R.; Banks, P.B.; Cameron, E.Z.; Dickman, C.R.; Frank, A.S.K.; Jones, M.E.; McGregor, I.S.; McLean, S.; Müller-Schwarze, D.; et al. Biologically meaningful scents: A framework for understanding predator–prey research across disciplines. Biol. Rev. 2018, 93, 98–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Bates, L.A.; Sayialel, K.N.; Njiraini, N.W.; Moss, C.J.; Poole, J.H.; Byrne, R.W. Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1938–1942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  111. Power, R.J.; Compion, R.X.S. Lion predation on elephants in the Savuti, Chobe National Park, Botswana. Afr. Zool. 2009, 44, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Kumaraguru, A.; Saravanamuthu, R.; Brinda, K.; Asokan, S. Prey preference of large carnivores in Anamalai Tiger Reserve, India. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 2011, 57, 627–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. McComb, K.; Shannon, G.; Durant, S.M.; Sayialel, K.; Slotow, R.; Poole, J.; Moss, C. Leadership in elephants: The adaptive value of age. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 278, 3270–3276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Thuppil, V.; Coss, R.G. Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard growls according to perceived danger. Biol. Lett. 2013, 9, 20130518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Valenta, K.; Schmitt, M.H.; Ayasse, M.; Nevo, O. The sensory ecology of fear: African elephants show aversion to olfactory predator signals. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2021, 3, e333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Wittemyer, G.; Polansky, L.; Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Getz, W.M. Disentangling the effects of forage, social rank, and risk on movement autocorrelation of elephants using Fourier and wavelet analyses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 19108–19113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Iason, G. The role of plant secondary metabolites in mammalian herbivory: Ecological perspectives. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2005, 64, 123–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Hansen, S.C.; Stolter, C.; Imholt, C.; Jacob, J. Plant secondary metabolites as rodent repellents: A systematic review. J. Chem. Ecol. 2016, 42, 970–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Scogings, P.F.; Hjältén, J.; Skarpe, C. Secondary metabolites and nutrients of woody plants in relation to browsing intensity in African savannas. Oecologia 2011, 167, 1063–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  120. Scogings, P.F.; Hjältén, J.; Skarpe, C.; Hattas, D.; Zobolo, A.; Dziba, L.; Rooke, T. Nutrient and secondary metabolite concentrations in a savanna are independently affected by large herbivores and shoot growth rate. Plant Ecol. 2014, 215, 73–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. Stokke, S.; du Toit, J.T. Sexual segregation in habitat use by elephants in Chobe National Park, Botswana. Afr. J. Ecol. 2002, 40, 360–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Makhabu, S.W. Interactions between Woody Plants, Elephants and Other Browsers in the Chobe Riverfront Botswana. Ph.D. Thesis, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  123. Makhabu, S.W.; Skarpe, C. Rebrowsing by elephants three years after simulated browsing on five woody plant species in northern Botswana. S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 2006, 36, 99–102. [Google Scholar]
  124. Hemborg, Å.M.; Bond, W.J. Do browsing elephants damage female trees more? Afr. J. Ecol. 2007, 45, 41–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Joshi, R.; Singh, R. Feeding behaviour of wild Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in the Rajaji National Park. J. Am. Sci. 2008, 4, 34–48. [Google Scholar]
  126. Bal, P.; Nath, C.D.; Nanaya, K.M.; Kushalappa, C.G.; Garcia, C. Elephants also like coffee: Trends and drivers of human–elephant conflicts in coffee agroforestry landscapes of Kodagu, Western Ghats, India. Environ. Manag. 2011, 47, 789–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Shannon, G.; Mackey, R.L.; Slotow, R. Diet selection and seasonal dietary switch of a large sexually dimorphic herbivore. Acta Oecologica 2013, 46, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. English, M.; Gillespie, G.; Ancrenaz, M.; Ismail, S.; Goossens, B.; Nathan, S.; Linklater, W. Plant selection and avoidance by the Bornean elephant (Elephas maximus borneensis) in tropical forest: Does plant recovery rate after herbivory influence food choices? J. Trop. Ecol. 2014, 30, 371–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. English, M.; Gillespie, G.; Goosens, B.; Ismail, S.; Ancrenaz, M.; Linklater, W. Recursion to food plants by free-ranging Bornean elephant. PeerJ 2015, 3, e1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  130. Branco, P.S.; Merkle, J.A.; Pringle, R.M.; Pansu, J.; Potter, A.B.; Reynolds, A.; Stalmans, M.; Long, R.A. Determinants of elephant foraging behaviour in a coupled human-natural system: Is brown the new green? J. Anim. Ecol. 2019, 88, 780–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Codron, J.; Lee-Thorp, J.A.; Sponheimer, M.; Codron, D.; Grant, R.C.; de Ruiter, D.J. Elephant (Loxodonta africana) diets in Kruger National Park, South Africa: Spatial and landscape differences. J. Mammal. 2006, 87, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Owen-Smith, N.; Chafota, J. Selective feeding by a megaherbivore, the African elephant (Loxodonta africana). J. Mammal. 2012, 93, 698–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  133. Shrader, A.M.; Bell, C.; Bertolli, L.; Ward, D. Forest or the trees: At what scale do elephants make foraging decisions? Acta Oecologica 2012, 42, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Schmitt, M.H.; Shuttleworth, A.; Shrader, A.M.; Ward, D. The role of volatile plant secondary metabolites as pre-ingestive cues and potential toxins dictating diet selection by African elephants. Oikos 2019, 129, 24–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Schmitt, M.H.; Shuttleworth, A.; Ward, D.; Shrader, A.M. African elephants use plant odours to make foraging decisions across multiple spatial scales. Anim. Behav. 2018, 141, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  136. Plotnik, J.M.; Shaw, R.C.; Brubaker, D.L.; Tiller, L.N.; Clayton, N.S. Thinking with their trunks: Elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude nonrewarding alternatives. Anim. Behav. 2014, 88, 91–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Plotnik, J.M.; Brubaker, D.L.; Dale, R.; Tiller, L.N.; Mumby, H.S.; Clayton, N.S. Elephants have a nose for quantity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 12566–12571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  138. Wood, M.; Chamaillé-Jammes, S.; Hammerbacher, A.; Shrader, A.M. African elephants can detect water from natural and artificial sources via olfactory cues. Anim. Cogn. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Gobush, K.S.; Edwards, C.T.T.; Balfour, D.; Wittemyer, G.; Maisels, F.; Taylor, R.D. Loxodonta africana. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2021. Available online: https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2021-2.RLTS.T181008073A204401095.en (accessed on 12 August 2021).
  140. Gobush, K.S.; Edwards, C.T.T.; Balfour, D.; Wittemyer, G.; Maisels, F.; Taylor, R.D. Loxodonta cyclotis. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2021. Available online: https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2021-1.RLTS.T181007989A204404464.en (accessed on 12 August 2021).
  141. Williams, C.; Tiwari, S.K.; Goswami, V.R.; de Silva, S.; Kumar, A.; Baskaran, N.; Yoganand, K.; Menon, V. Elephas maximus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2020. Available online: https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-3.RLTS.T7140A45818198.en (accessed on 12 August 2021).
  142. Cook, S.M.; Khan, Z.R.; Pickett, J.A. The use of push-pull strategies in integrated pest management. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2007, 52, 375–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  143. Schulte, B.A. Learning and applications of chemical signals in vertebrates for human–wildlife conflict mitigation. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 13; Schulte, B.A., Goodwin, T.E., Ferkin, M.H., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 499–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Schulte, B.A.; Freeman, E.W.; Goodwin, T.E.; Hollister-Smith, J.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Honest signalling through chemicals by elephants with applications for care and conservation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 344–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Jackson, J.; Childs, D.Z.; Mar, K.U.; Htut, W.; Lummaa, V. Long-term trends in wild-capture and population dynamics point to an uncertain future for captive elephants. Proc. R. Soc. B 2019, 286, 20182810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  146. Brown, J.L. Update on comparative biology of elephants: Factors affecting reproduction, health and welfare. In Reproductive Sciences in Animal Conservation, 2nd ed.; Comizzoli, P., Brown, J.L., Holt, W.V., Eds.; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 243–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Schulte, B.A. Social structure and helping behavior in captive elephants. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 447–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Brown, J.L.; Paris, S.; Prado-Oviedo, N.A.; Meehan, C.L.; Hogan, J.N.; Morfeld, K.A.; Carlstead, K. Reproductive health assessment of female elephants in North American zoos and association of husbandry practices with reproductive dysfunction in African elephants (Loxodonta africana). PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0145673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Meehan, C.L.; Hogan, J.N.; Bonaparte-Saller, M.K.; Mench, J.A. Housing and social environments of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0146703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  150. Bechert, U.S.; Brown, J.L.; Dierenfeld, E.S.; Ling, P.D.; Molter, C.M.; Schulte, B.A. Zoo elephant research: Contributions to conservation of captive and free-ranging species. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2019, 53, 89–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Meehan, C.; Greco, B.; Lynn, B.; Morfeld, K.; Vicino, G.; Orban, D.; Gorsuch, C.; Quick, M.; Ripple, L.; Fournier, K.; et al. The Elephant Welfare Initiative: A model for advancing evidence-based zoo animal welfare monitoring, assessment and enhancement. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2019, 53, 45–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Wyatt, T.D. Pheromones and Animal Behavior: Chemical Signals and Signatures, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014; p. 405. [Google Scholar]
  153. Swaisgood, R.R.; Schulte, B.A. Applying knowledge of mammalian social organization, mating systems, and communication to management. In Wild Mammals in Captivity: Principles and Techniques for Zoo Management, Second Edition; Kleiman, D.G., Thompson, K.V., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 329–343. [Google Scholar]
  154. Clark, F.; King, A.J. A.J. A critical review of zoo-based olfactory enrichment. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11; Hurst, J., Beynon, R.J., Roberts, S.C., Wyatt, T., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; pp. 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Mellen, J.; MacPhee, M.S. Philosophy of environmental enrichment: Past, present, and future. Zoo Biol. 2001, 20, 211–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Alligood, C.; Leighty, K. Putting the “E” in SPIDER: Evolving trends in the evaluation of environmental enrichment efficacy in zoological settings. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2015, 2, 200–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Greco, B.J.; Meehan, C.L.; Hogan, J.N.; Leighty, K.A.; Mellen, J.; Mason, G.J.; Mench, J.A. The days and nights of zoo elephants: Using epidemiology to better understand stereotypic behavior of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American Zoos. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0144276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. LaDue, C.A.; Schulte, B.A. Pheromonal enrichment in the zoo: An empirical approach with Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 235, 105228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Mason, G.J. Species differences in responses to captivity: Stress, welfare and the comparative method. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2010, 25, 713–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  160. Martin, M.S.; Owen, M.; Wintle, N.J.P.; Zhang, G.; Zhang, H.; Swaisgood, R.R. Stereotypic behaviour predicts reproductive performance and litter sex ratio in giant pandas. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 7263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Swaisgood, R.R.; Lindburg, D.; White, A.M.; Zhang, H.; Zhou, X. Chemical communication in giant pandas. In Giant Pandas: Biology and Conservation; Lindburg, D., Baragona, K., Eds.; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2004; pp. 106–124. [Google Scholar]
  162. Thouless, C.R.; Dublin, H.T.; Blanc, J.J.; Skinner, D.P.; Daniel, T.E.; Taylor, R.D.; Maisels, F.; Frederick, H.L.; Bouché, P. African Elephant Status Report 2016: An Update from the African Elephant Database; International Union for the Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources: Gland, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  163. Calabrese, A.; Calabrese, J.M.; Songer, M.; Wegmann, M.; Hedges, S.; Rose, R.; Leimgruber, P. Conservation status of Asian elephants: The influence of habitat and governance. Biodivers. Conserv. 2017, 26, 2067–2081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. Shaffer, L.J.; Khadka, K.K.; Hoek, J.V.D.; Naithani, K.J. Human-elephant conflict: A review of current management strategies and future directions. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2019, 6, 235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  165. De Sales, A.R.; Anastácio, R.S.S.; Pereira, M.J. The African elephant (Loxodonta africana): Mini-review of an endangered species. Nat. Resour. 2020, 11, 317–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Gross, E.M.; Lahkar, B.P.; Subedi, N.; Nyirenda, V.R.; Klebelsberg, E.; Jakoby, O. Elephants in the village: Causes and consequences of property damage in Asia and Africa. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2021, 3, e343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Brunnermeier, M.J.; Schmied, S.A.K.; Schupfner, R. Distribution of 14C, 90Sr, and 228Th in an elephant tusk. J. Radioanal. Nucl. Chem. 2012, 292, 1285–1290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Wasser, S.K.; Mailand, C.; Booth, R.; Mutayoba, B.; Kisamo, E.; Clark, B.; Stephens, M. Using DNA to track the origin of the largest ivory seizure since the 1989 trade ban. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 4228–4233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  169. Guldemond, R.A.R.; Purdon, A.; van Aarde, R.J. A systematic review of elephant impact across Africa. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0178935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Schmitt, M.H.; Ward, D.; Shrader, A.M. Incorporating secondary metabolites, tannin-binding proteins, and diet breadth into carrying-capacity models for African elephants. Ecol. Model. 2016, 332, 8–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. Wright, M.G.; Spencer, C.; Cook, R.M.; Henley, M.D.; North, W.; Mafra-Neto, A. African bush elephants respond to a honeybee alarm pheromone blend. Curr. Biol. 2018, 28, R778–R780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  172. Mafra-Neto, A.; Lame, F.M.d.; Fettig, C.J.; Munson, A.S.; Perring, T.M.; Stelinski, L.L.; Stoltman, L.L.; Mafra, L.E.J.; Borges, R.; Vargas, R.I. Manipulation of insect behavior with specialized pheromone and lure application technology (SPLAT®). In Pest Management with Natural Products; Beck, J.J., Coats, J.R., Duke, S.O., Koivunen, M.E., Eds.; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; pp. 31–58. [Google Scholar]
  173. Gross, E.M.; Drouet-Hoguet, N.; Subedi, N.; Gross, J. The potential of medicinal and aromatic plants (MAPs) to reduce crop damages by Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Crop. Prot. 2017, 100, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Gross, E.M.; McRobb, R.; Gross, J. Cultivating alternative crops reduces crop losses due to African elephants. J. Pest. Sci. 2015, 89, 497–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Hedges, S.; Gunaryadi, D. Reducing human-elephant conflict: Do chillies help deter elephants from entering crop fields? Oryx 2009, 44, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  176. Le Bel, S. Repelling elephants with a chilli pepper gas dispenser: Field tests and practical use in Mozambique, Zambia and Zimbabwe. Pachyderm 2015, 56, 87–96. [Google Scholar]
  177. Le Bel, S.; Taylor, R.; Lagrange, M.; Ndoro, O.; Barra, M.; Madzikanda, H. An easy-to-use capsicum delivery system for crop-raiding elephants in Zimbabwe: Preliminary results of a field test in Hwange National Park. Pachyderm 2010, 47, 80–89. [Google Scholar]
  178. Chang’a, A.; de Souza, N.; Muya, J.; Keyyu, J.; Mwakatobe, A.; Malugu, L.; Ndossi, H.P.; Konuche, J.; Omondi, R.; Mpinge, A.; et al. Scaling-up the use of chili fences for reducing human-elephant conflict across landscapes in Tanzania. Trop. Conserv. Sci. 2016, 9, 921–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  179. Von Hagen, R.L.; Kasaine, S.; Githiru, M.; Amakobe, B.; Mutwiwa, U.N.; Schulte, B.A. Metal strip fences for preventing African elephant (Loxodonta africana) crop foraging in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor, Kenya. Afr. J. Ecol. 2020, 59, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Kiffner, C.; Schaal, I.; Cass, L.; Peirce, K.; Sussman, O.; Grueser, A.; Wachtel, E.; Adams, H.; Clark, K.; König, H.J.; et al. Perceptions and realities of elephant crop raiding and mitigation methods. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2021, 3, e372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  181. Von Hagen, R.L.; Norris, P.; Schulte, B.A. Quantifying capsaicinoids from chili pepper and motor oil mixtures used in elephant deterrent fences. Chromatographia 2020, 83, 1153–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  182. Santiapillai, C.; Read, B. Would masking the smell of ripening paddy-fields help mitigate human-elephant conflict in Sri Lanka? Oryx 2010, 44, 509–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  183. Oniba, E.; Robertson, M.R. Trialling a new scent-based repellent to mitigate elephant crop-raiding around Murchison Falls National Park, Uganda. Pachyderm 2019, 60, 123–125. [Google Scholar]
  184. Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Riddle, S.W. Development and initial testing of pheromone-enhanced mechanical devices for deterring crop raiding elephants: A positive conservation step. J. Elephant Man. Assoc. 2004, 15, 30–37. [Google Scholar]
  185. Greenwood, D.R.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. The elephant as an ideal olfactory model mammal. ChemoSense 2009, 11, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  186. Polla, E.J.; Grueter, C.C.; Smith, C.L. Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human visual and olfactory cues. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2018, 5, 279–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Dürckheim, K.E.M.v.; Hoffman, L.C.; Leslie, A.; Hensman, M.C.; Hensman, S.; Schultz, K.; Lee, S. African elephants (Loxodonta africana) display remarkable olfactory acuity in human scent matching to sample performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 200, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Partan, S.; Marler, P. Communication goes multimodal. Science 1999, 283, 1272–1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  189. Apps, P.J.; Weldon, P.J.; Kramer, M. Chemical signals in terrestrial vertebrates: Search for design features. Nat. Prod. Rep. 2015, 32, 1131–1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Series of stereotyped chemosensory behaviors present in African and Asian elephants (shown here with a male Asian elephant), adapted from [45,46]. Sniff: elephant holds trunk over signal of interest. Check: dorsal tip (“finger”) of trunk contacts signal. Place: entire end of trunk contacts signal. Flehmen: trunk transports signal to orifice of vomeronasal ducts. In order, these behaviors indicate the level of interest in a signal and may be used readily in bioassays among wild and captive elephant populations. Yellow circles are chemical signals of interest, and blue arrows show points of contact of the trunk with the chemicals.
Figure 1. Series of stereotyped chemosensory behaviors present in African and Asian elephants (shown here with a male Asian elephant), adapted from [45,46]. Sniff: elephant holds trunk over signal of interest. Check: dorsal tip (“finger”) of trunk contacts signal. Place: entire end of trunk contacts signal. Flehmen: trunk transports signal to orifice of vomeronasal ducts. In order, these behaviors indicate the level of interest in a signal and may be used readily in bioassays among wild and captive elephant populations. Yellow circles are chemical signals of interest, and blue arrows show points of contact of the trunk with the chemicals.
Animals 11 02860 g001
Figure 2. Summary of 21st-century findings in intraspecific chemical signaling among elephants. EM = Elephas maximus, the Asian elephant. LA = Loxodonta africana, the African savanna elephant. Red indicates female–female signals, blue indicates male–male signals, and purple indicates intersexual signals.
Figure 2. Summary of 21st-century findings in intraspecific chemical signaling among elephants. EM = Elephas maximus, the Asian elephant. LA = Loxodonta africana, the African savanna elephant. Red indicates female–female signals, blue indicates male–male signals, and purple indicates intersexual signals.
Animals 11 02860 g002
Figure 3. Potential applications of chemical signals for the conservation, management, and husbandry of elephants, using the push-pull approach [142] to manipulate elephant movement.
Figure 3. Potential applications of chemical signals for the conservation, management, and husbandry of elephants, using the push-pull approach [142] to manipulate elephant movement.
Animals 11 02860 g003
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Schulte, B.A.; LaDue, C.A. The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks. Animals 2021, 11, 2860. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102860

AMA Style

Schulte BA, LaDue CA. The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks. Animals. 2021; 11(10):2860. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102860

Chicago/Turabian Style

Schulte, Bruce A., and Chase A. LaDue. 2021. "The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks" Animals 11, no. 10: 2860. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102860

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop