Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Phenolic-Rich Extracts of Rubus fruticosus, R. ulmifolius and Morus nigra on Oxidative Stress and Caco-2 Inhibition Growth
Previous Article in Journal
Prognostic Nutritional Index and Instant Nutritional Assessement Are Associated with Clinical Outcomes in a Geriatric Cohort of Acutely Inpatients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Executive Function among African American and White Adults in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study

by
Marie Fanelli Kuczmarski
1,*,
Sara B. Crawford
2,
Rhonda S. Sebastian
2,
May A. Beydoun
1,
Joseph D. Goldman
2,
Alanna J. Moshfegh
2,
Michele K. Evans
1 and
Alan B. Zonderman
1
1
Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Baltimore, MD 21224, USA
2
Food Surveys Research Group, Agricultural Research Service, United States Department of Agriculture, BARC-West, Beltsville, MD 20705, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2024, 16(9), 1360; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16091360
Submission received: 22 February 2024 / Revised: 13 April 2024 / Accepted: 25 April 2024 / Published: 30 April 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Phytochemicals and Human Health)

Abstract

:
Healthy dietary patterns rich in flavonoids may benefit cognitive performance over time. Among socioeconomically disadvantaged groups, the association between flavonoid intake and measures of cognition is unclear. This study sought to identify associations between flavonoid intake and cognitive performance among Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study participants (n = 1947) across three study visits. Flavonoid intakes were assessed via two 24-h dietary recalls. Cognitive performance was assessed via the Trail Making Test (TMT)-A and TMT-B, which provide measures of attention and executive function, respectively. Mixed effects linear regression was used to model TMT scores over three study visits against visit 1 (v1) flavonoid intake, time (years from v1), and the interaction between v1 flavonoid intake and time, capturing both the cross-sectional association between flavonoid intake and time at v1 as well as the longitudinal association between v1 flavonoid intake and the change in TMT scores over time. Prior to adjustment, inverse cross-sectional associations at v1 were observed between (1) anthocyanidin intake and TMT-A scores for the overall sample and (2) total flavonoid, anthocyanidin, flavan-3-ol, flavone, and flavonol intake and TMT-B scores for the overall sample and among White adults. Only the association between anthocyanidin intake and TMT-B at v1 among White adults persisted after adjustment (for demographic characteristics such as age). One possible explanation for the few significant associations is universally low flavonoid intakes resulting from the consumption of an unhealthy dietary pattern.

1. Introduction

According to the United States (US) Health and Retirement Study, approximately two out of three American adults experience some level of cognitive impairment (but not dementia) at an average age of approximately 70 years [1]. Cognitive impairment is characterized by trouble remembering, learning new things, concentrating, or making decisions that affect everyday life and can vary from mild to severe [2]. Women have a 71% lifetime risk of experiencing cognitive impairment before death while men have a 61% lifetime risk [1]. There are large disparities in the risk by race, with 72% of Black men and 83% of Black women experiencing impairment, compared to 57% and 66% of White men and women, respectively [1]. Lifetime risk also varies with level of education, as those with less than a high school degree have the greatest risk compared to those with high school or higher educational attainment [1]. However, the mechanisms driving these disparities have not been identified. A challenge for health professionals is to identify strategies to minimize age-related cognitive decline, especially among high-risk populations.
There is evidence, although limited, that flavonoids and/or foods rich in flavonoid compounds have the potential to improve cognition at the preclinical and clinical levels [3,4]. Meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials with multiple populations suggest that flavonoid intake offers protective neurocognitive benefits across a lifespan [5]. Associations differ by food source, which may reflect the bioavailability of flavonoids and flavonoid subclasses [3,5,6,7], the ability of the flavonoid to cross the blood–brain barrier, and the diversity in gut microbiome-generated flavonoid metabolites. These metabolites can also be transported by the vagus nerve and systemic circulation to the brain [8,9,10]. However, findings from both observational and longitudinal studies describing associations between flavonoid intake and cognitive measures have been inconsistent [6,11,12,13,14,15,16]. This may be due to differences in the populations studied, the dietary and/or cognitive assessment methods employed, and/or the other explanatory variables that are included in the models [17].
Impaired executive function and overall cognition are symptoms of major depression, a disease that affects approximately 21 million Americans annually [18]. Flavonoid consumption is associated with a decreased risk of developing depression [19]. There is also evidence that diets rich in flavonoids reduce the risk of developing dementia and have the potential to improve symptoms in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease [20,21]. The role of flavonoids and their potential impact as natural therapies in mental health and neurological disorders, such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, is under investigation. Our knowledge of the roles and benefits of flavonoids on mental health and related diseases is limited and needs expansion, especially since flavonoids are abundantly available in plant-based foods and beverages [22,23].
Many studies in the literature describing associations between flavonoid intake and cognition have analyzed samples comprised primarily of White adults with average to above-average income and/or education [6,11,12,24]. As previously noted, the factors contributing to the disparities in lifetime risk of cognitive impairment among Black versus White adults warrant exploration. The Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study provides the opportunity to investigate potential differences in flavonoid–cognition associations by race, as the sample is comprised of urban African American and White adults aged 30–64 from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds. Previous research documented significantly lower flavonoid intakes for African American HANDLS study participants compared to White study participants [25]. The objective of this study was to determine if associations exist between flavonoid intake and cognitive performance among a socioeconomically diverse sample, overall, and stratified by race, using the HANDLS study.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Sample

A total of 3720 adults aged 30–64 years were enrolled in the HANDLS study, all of whom resided in Baltimore City, Maryland, USA, in 13 specifically determined neighborhoods. Race was self-reported as African American or White. The baseline visit (visit 1) of this prospective cohort study was initiated in August 2004 and ended in March 2009. A detailed description of the study design and procedures is available online [26,27].
All participants were provided with a protocol booklet and watched a video that explained the study procedures before giving written informed consent [27]. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Institutes of Health. Data for this study, a secondary analysis, were derived from visit 1 and the third and fifth follow-up examinations (visit 3, 2009–2013, and visit 5, 2017–2020, respectively).
The analytical sample consisted of 1947 participants with complete dietary data at visit 1 and at least one test result for both the Trail Making Test (TMT)-A and TMT-B, the response variables measuring cognitive performance, across visits 1, 3, and 5 (Figure 1). Sample participants were excluded if they had a score of <24 on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE), a tool assessing the subset of cognitive status including attention, language, memory, orientation, and visuospatial proficiency [28]. A score of <24 indicates cognitive impairment [29].

2.2. Assessment of Flavonoid Intake

Intake of total flavonoids and five flavonoid classes was calculated from two 24-h dietary recalls collected in person by trained interviewers using the US Department of Agriculture (USDA) Automated Multiple Pass Method [30]. Participants were provided an illustrated food model booklet and measurement aids (ruler, measuring spoons, and cups) to assist them in recalling portion sizes of foods and beverages consumed. The USDA Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies 3.0 was used to assign food codes with associated energy and nutrient profiles to all the foods and beverages reported in the recalls [31].
The USDA food codes provided the link between the dietary data and flavonoid composition data from the Database for Flavonoid Values for USDA Survey Food Codes 2007–2010 [32,33]. Mean dietary flavonoid intakes were estimated from foods and beverages only; no data were collected on baseline supplement intakes. For this study, 2-day mean intakes of energy, anthocyanidins, flavan-3-ols (inclusive of catechins, theaflavins, and thearubigins), flavanones, flavones, flavonols, and total flavonoids (summative total of the five previously listed classes plus isoflavones) were calculated.

2.3. Cognitive Measures

The TMT, a neuropsychological test, consists of two parts, A and B. It requires a variety of mental abilities for successful performance, including letter and number recognition, mental flexibility, visual scanning, and motor function [34]. TMT-A is generally regarded as measuring the attention, visual search, and motor skills domains that are associated with the non-dominant (right) cerebral hemisphere [35]. TMT-B is generally seen as a measure of the executive function, motor skills, speed of attention, and visual search domains, which are associated with the dominant (left) cerebral hemisphere [35].
Scores for both the TMT-A and TMT-B were obtained from participants at visits 1, 3, and 5. Prior to the start of both assessments, sample tests were performed to ensure the participant understood the task. In TMT-A, the participant drew a line to connect consecutive numbers, from 1 to 25. In TMT-B, the participant connected numbers and letters in an alternating progressive sequence, from a number to a letter, then a letter to a number, i.e., the sequence was from 1 to A, A to 2, 2 to B, B to 3, and so on. A stopwatch was used to record the time to complete each part, in seconds. The examiner started timing each part as soon as the instructions were completed and the participant was signaled to begin. Participants were allowed 5 min to complete each part of the TMT. The time to completion of TMT-A and TMT-B, in seconds, was recorded separately and used in the analyses.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

2.4.1. Basic Model

Longitudinal mixed linear models were used to explore the associations between the response variables, TMT-A and TMT-B, collected over three time points (visits 1, 3, and 5) and visit 1 flavonoid intake. The aim of the modeling was to assess the associations between (1) flavonoid intake and TMT scores at visit 1 (cross-sectional) and (2) flavonoid intake at visit 1 and the change in TMT scores over time (longitudinal). The natural log (ln) of TMT-A and TMT-B was modeled, as both variables exhibited a strong right skew. A basic unadjusted model including only the variables needed to address the aims was fit, as well as three adjusted models that added covariates with the potential to affect test performance (e.g., age at baseline) to the basic model. The basic model included fixed effects for flavonoid intake at visit 1, time measured as years from visit 1, and the interaction between visit 1 flavonoid intake and time. It also included a random intercept with an unstructured covariance matrix to capture the correlation between repeated visits on a single subject. The fixed effects coefficient for visit 1 flavonoid intake captures the relationship between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT), addressing the first aim. In other words, it captures the effect of baseline flavonoid intake on ln(TMT) when time = 0 (i.e., at baseline visit 1) or the cross-sectional flavonoid intake–cognition relationships. The fixed effects coefficient for the interaction between visit 1 flavonoid intake and time captures the relationship between visit 1 flavonoid intake and the change in ln(TMT) over time, addressing the second aim (i.e., the longitudinal flavonoid intake–cognition relationships). The remaining three models used 15 covariates grouped as demographic, lifestyle, or clinical characteristics to adjust the basic statistical model.

2.4.2. Demographic Model

The demographic model was adjusted to the basic model for the following covariates: baseline (visit 1) age in years; age-squared (to allow for a non-linear relationship between age and cognition); sex assigned at birth (male or female); self-reported race (African American or White); income; education; and literacy. Income was indexed by poverty below or above 125% of the 2004 US Health and Human Services Poverty Guidelines (125% equals a yearly income of USD 23,563 for a family of four in 2004) [36]. Education was captured in years. Literacy was measured via the Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd edition (WRAT3) reading score, which was calculated as the sum of total correctly pronounced letters and words [37].

2.4.3. Lifestyle Model

In addition to the covariates included in the demographic model, the lifestyle model included current smoking status, current illicit drug use, and mean energy intake (kcal) based on two 24-h recalls. At the time of visit 1, illicit drugs were defined as marijuana, opiates, and cocaine. Cigarette smokers and users of drugs were categorized as current or never/former users [27].

2.4.4. Clinical Model

In addition to the covariates included in the demographic model, the clinical model included diabetes, hypertension, high serum cholesterol, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) score, and body mass index (BMI). These covariates have been associated with cognitive performance [2,12,38,39]. Diabetes mellitus was based on three measures, namely, a fasting glucose level concentration of >126 mg/dL (7.0 mmol/L), self-reports, and/or taking medication for diabetes [27]. This variable was coded as a dichotomous variable (diabetic versus not diabetic); the pre-diabetic was grouped with the not diabetic group. Hypertension was defined as having an average of seated and standing systolic blood pressure > 140 mm Hg, an average of seated and standing diastolic blood pressure > 90 mm Hg [40], a history of blood pressure medication use, and/or a self-report of hypertension. Fasting venous blood specimens were collected from participants in the morning and analyzed by Quest Diagnostics, Inc. (Chantilly, VA, USA). The serum total cholesterol (mg/dL) was assessed using a spectrophotometer (Olympus 5400, Olympus, Melville, NY, USA). High serum cholesterol was defined as having a fasting blood level >200 mg/dL, self-reports, and/or taking medication to lower serum cholesterol. Depressive symptoms were measured by the 20-item CES-D, a symptom rating scale assessing self-reported depressed mood [41]. BMI (kg/m2) was calculated from measured weight and height. A calibrated Med-weigh model 2500 digital scale was used to measure weight. Height was measured with the HANDLS study participant’s heels and back against a stadiometer (Novel Products, Inc., Rockton, IL, USA).

2.4.5. Missing Data

Several covariates had missing data: education (<1% of values), WRAT3 score (<1%), current smoking status (11%), current illicit drug use (30%), diabetes status (6%), hypertension status (16%), high cholesterol status (6%), CES-D (5%), and BMI (5%). Missing data for these variables were imputed using a discriminant function for the categorical variables and predictive mean matching for the continuous variables. Twenty imputed datasets were created. To avoid bias in estimates, all variables included in the analysis model were included in the imputation model [42]. Therefore, imputation models included ln(TMT-A), ln(TMT-B), race, time, baseline (visit 1) flavonoid intakes (total and class-specific), interactions between flavonoid intakes and time, age, age-squared, sex, poverty status, education, WRAT3 score, smoking status, illicit drug use, total energy intake, diabetes status, hypertension status, high cholesterol status, CES-D, and BMI.

2.4.6. Model Detail

All statistical analyses were performed using SAS v 9.4. All models were fit for the entire analytic study sample and stratified by race. Separate models were used for total flavonoids and each flavonoid class (anthocyanidins, flavan-3-ols, flavanones, flavones, and flavonols). Two additional analyses were performed: (1) limiting the sample to adults ≥50 years of age and (2) including subjects that were initially excluded from the analytic sample based on visit 1 MMSE. Regression estimates were reported for a 10-unit increase in flavonoid intake. Statistical significance was set at α = 0.0083 after applying a Bonferroni adjustment of α = 0.05/6 to account for the 6 different flavonoid intake values being assessed.

3. Results

3.1. Study Sample Description

At visit 1, the analytical sample of n = 1947 adults had an average age of 48 years; 42% were male and 16% had diabetes (Table 1). The mean years of education was 12.3. Thirty percent of the sample had not completed high school. Among White adults (n = 823), 31% lived at <125% poverty, compared to 50% of African American adults (n = 1124). Approximately 56% of White participants had high serum cholesterol and 38% had hypertension, compared to 44% and 49% of African American participants, respectively. Moreover, approximately 47% of the overall sample were current cigarette smokers and 18% were current users of marijuana, opiates, and/or cocaine.
The intake of total flavonoids and all flavonoid classes was highly skewed. For total flavonoids, the mean intake was 253 mg with a standard error (SE) of 11.5 mg, whereas the median was 68.1 mg with an interquartile range (IQR) of 19.7–297.5 mg (Table 2). On a weight basis, flavan-3-ols accounted for approximately 85% of intake. The mean and median intake of flavan-3-ols was 214 mg (SE = 11.1 mg) and 14.7 mg (IQR = 3.3–248.1 mg), respectively, followed by flavonols (mean 18.1 mg, SE = 0.5 mg; and median 12.9 mg, (IQR = 6.2–23.7 mg)). The median score on the TMT-A was 31 s (IQR = 25–41 s) and the median score on the TMT-B was 87 s (IQR = 62–139 s).

3.2. Flavonoid-TMT A and B Associations

Among all study subjects, the basic model demonstrated a significant association between visit 1 anthocyanidin intake and visit 1ln(TMT-A) scores. For a 10-unit (mg) increase in anthocyanidin intake, the ln(TMT-A) would be expected to decrease by 0.013 units at visit 1 (seconds; p = 0.006; Table 3). The association is depicted in Figure 2, where at time = 0 (i.e., visit 1), the predicted value for ln(TMT-B) is lower (i.e., predicted cognition is higher) for larger values of visit 1 anthocyanidin intake. This association was not significant after covariate adjustment in the demographic, lifestyle, or clinical models. There was no significant relationship between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-A) over time. An example of this lack of association over time is shown in Figure 2. The parallel lines indicate that differing levels of visit 1 anthocyanidin intake are not associated with differences in the rate of change in cognition over time. In addition, there were no significant associations between visit 1 flavonoid intake and either visit 1 ln(TMT-A) or change in ln(TMT-A) over time among White adults (Table S1) or African American adults (Table S2) when stratified by race.
There were several significant associations between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B) among all study subjects using the basic model. A significant decrease in ln(TMT-B) was observed for a 10-unit increase in intake of total flavonoids and all flavonoid classes except flavanones (Table 4). The significant associations included intake of total flavonoids ( β ^ = −0.001, p = 0.001), flavones ( β ^ = −0.887, p < 0.001), flavonols ( β ^ = −0.030, p < 0.001), flavan-3-ols ( β ^ = −0.001, p = 0.002), and anthocyanidins ( β ^ = −0.031, p < 0.001). However, there were no significant associations between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-B) over time. In addition, the significant associations did not persist after adjusting for covariates in the demographic, lifestyle, or clinical models.
Among White adults, significant associations between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B) exist for flavone intake ( β ^ = −0.745, p-value < 0.001) and anthocyanidin intake ( β ^ = −0.044, p-value < 0.001) in the basic model (Table 5). The only association to persist with further adjustment was that between anthocyanidin intake and ln(TMT-B) score and it was found in the demographic model only (   ^ = −0.022, p-value = 0.006). No significant associations were observed among African American adults (Table 6).
An additional analysis performed limited the study sample to adults ≥50 years of age (n = 846). No significant associations were seen between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-A) or the change in ln(TMT-A) over time in any of the four models among all adults (Table S3), among White adults (Table S4), or among African American adults (Table S5). Using the basic model, visit 1 ln(TMT-B) exhibited expected decreases of 0.786 units for a 10-unit increase in visit 1 flavone intake (p-value = 0.003) and 0.037 units for a 10-unit increase in visit 1 anthocyanidin intake (p-value = 0.001) among all study subjects (Table S6). However, the associations did not persist after adjustment and no significant associations were observed between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-B) over time. The relationship between visit 1 anthocyanidin intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B) using the basic model was also observed among White adults ( β ^ = −0.038, p-value = 0.001; Table S7) but not among African American adults (Table S8). No other significant associations were observed for ln(TMT-B) among White or African American adults aged 50 years and older.
In a second additional analysis (retaining individuals originally excluded because their visit 1 MMSE was <24), significant associations were observed with the basic model only between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-A) among all adults for flavone intake ( β ^ = −0.309, p = 0.002), flavonols intake ( β ^ = −0.015, p = 0.002), and anthocyanidin intake ( β ^ = −0.017, p = 0.001) and among White adults for anthocyanidin intake ( β ^ = −0.017, p = 0.003) (Tables S9–S11). The findings between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B) were similar to those seen in the original study sample, with significant associations in the basic model only for total flavonoids and all flavonoid classes except flavanones among all adults (Table S12) and flavones and anthocyanidins among White adults (Tables S13 and S14).

4. Discussion

This study sought to determine if dietary flavonoid intake based on multiple 24-h dietary recalls was associated with cognitive function as assessed by the TMT-A and TMT-B scores. The associations evaluated included flavonoid intake and cognitive scores at visit 1 (cross-sectional) as well as flavonoid intake at visit 1 and cognitive scores over three study visits (longitudinal). To our knowledge, this is the first time that these relationships have been investigated in a sample of exclusively urban African American and White adults who present with multiple risk factors for diet-related disease, including cognitive impairment.
The basic model revealed several significant inverse associations between dietary intake of flavonoids at visit 1 and TMT-B scores at visit 1 for the overall sample and for White adults. In contrast, TMT-A scores were only inversely associated with anthocyanidin intake at visit 1 and only for the overall sample. Only the association between TMT-B and anthocyanidin intake at visit 1 among White adults was significant after covariate adjustment and only in the demographic model. Further adjustment of clinical and lifestyle characteristics mitigated the association. No other significant associations were found between flavonoid intake and TMT-A or TMT-B scores after covariate adjustments, either cross-sectionally or over time, for the overall sample, or among African American adults or White adults.
The null associations were not unanticipated. Results from research studies of flavonoid-cognitive associations have been inconsistent and even contradictory [43,44,45,46,47,48,49]. Those epidemiological and intervention studies that did find positive associations between the increased consumption of flavonoid-rich foods and cognitive function cite only weak effects [44,48]. For instance, Desideri and colleagues [45] and Mastroiacovo and colleagues [46] both noted slight improvements in TMT-A and TMT-B occurring with flavan-3-ol intakes of 520 or 993 mg, respectively, compared to 48 mg over 8 weeks while Brickman and colleagues [47] found an improvement only in TMT-B with flavan-3-ol intakes of 609 vs. 13 mg over 30 days. These findings suggest that cognitive change over time may not be detectable in both TMT-A and -B and that it may be dose-dependent. Other research has suggested such a dose-response relationship [50]. The total mean flavonoid intake of the HANDLS study participants was only 253 mg (median 68 mg, IQR 20–297 mg), which is well below the intakes cited in these previous studies showing positive associations. Additionally, the current recommendation for flavan-3-ols intake to reduce the risk of other chronic diseases, namely, cardiovascular disease and diabetes, is 400–600 mg/d [51] and the mean intake among HANDLS participants was only 214 mg (median 15 mg, IQR 3–248 mg). Furthermore, previous research reported that intakes of anthocyanidins and flavones were lower in the HANDLS study sample compared to the US population with similar demographic characteristics examined in What We Eat in America-National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (WWEIA-NHANES) [32]. In HANDLS, it has been shown that diet quality as assessed by the dietary inflammatory index improved minimally over time [52], implying that the intake of flavonoids is also stable. It is plausible that flavonoid intakes in the HANDLS study were too low to detect any associations with cognitive measures.
The mechanisms by which flavonoids may improve cognitive function are not fully understood. The ability of flavonoids to improve cognition over short time frames may be mediated by peripheral vascular changes facilitating more efficient cerebral blood flow [43,44]. For example, anthocyanidins and their metabolites have neuroprotective actions, including decreasing neuroinflammation, preventing excitotoxicity, preventing aggregation of proteins, and activating pro-survival pathways while inhibiting pro-apoptotic pathways and improving axonal health [50]. They have been shown to enter the brain and their concentrations are correlated with cognitive performance [53,54], a possible explanation for the inverse associations seen between anthocyanidin intake and TMT-A and TMT-B in the basic models.
There is evidence that TMT performance declines in normal aging, with TMT-B performance declining more rapidly than TMT-A performance in older adults [55,56]. TMT-B is considered a measure of fluid intelligence [57]. Many fluid cognitive abilities, especially psychomotor ability and processing speed, peak in the third decade of life and then decline at an estimated rate of −0.02 standard deviations per year [58]. Evidence suggests that the effects of flavonoids on cognition are mediated by age and are dose-dependent [50]. The age of the sample at visit 1 ranged from 30 to 64 years with a mean follow-up of 12.1 years among those subjects that completed visit 5, suggesting many participants were at or close to their cognitive peak at baseline. Consequently, it is expected that any flavonoid–cognition associations observed would only concern cognitive measures of more complex abilities, such as those reflected in the TMT-B [50]. The observed minimally adjusted associations between flavonoid intake and TMT-B might reflect the potential for improvements in the complex abilities that comprise the executive function cognitive domain.
It is well-established that healthy dietary patterns are associated with more favorable cognitive outcomes [58]. Overall, diet quality was low in HANDLS. The mean score for diet quality evaluated by the Healthy Eating Index-2010 score of the HANDLS study participants at visit 1 was 42.6 out of a possible 100 points with a range of 42–49 [52], compared to a range of mean scores between 57.4 and 61.6 for US individuals of similar ages [59]. In addition, and perhaps at least as important, is the impact of other lifestyle behaviors on cognitive outcomes that could attenuate any dietary influences on the brain [56]. The HANDLS study sample has an extremely high prevalence of unhealthy lifestyle behaviors, as evidenced by about 47% of the sample being classified as current smokers, as compared to about 21% nationally in 2004 [60]. Smoking is widely associated with cognitive decline [61,62,63]. The prevalence of chronic diseases among HANDLS study participants is also high, often higher than the US national population. For example, at the baseline visit of the HANDLS study, the prevalence of obesity was 42.1% [64]; diabetes, 16.5% [65]; and hypertension, 45.2% [66]. In comparison, national estimates among adults for a similar time frame were 32.2% for obesity [67], 10.3% for diabetes [68], and 29.3% for hypertension [69]. The benefits of flavonoids may be muted not only by their low intake but also by the high prevalence of other lifestyle behaviors and health conditions that accelerate cognitive decline.
This study has a few limitations. First, the 24-h dietary recalls were self-reported and could have been affected by memory, aspects of cognitive reserve like education and income, and social desirability bias [70,71]. However, the dietary data collection method, the automated multiple-pass method, has been validated using the doubly labeled water technique and has been shown to reduce bias in the collection of energy intakes [72]. Second, nutritional supplement intake was not obtained at visit 1 but could impact flavonoid intake and thus flavonoid–cognition associations. Lastly, due to data availability, only one cognitive measure was used in the analyses.
This study has several strengths. First, the flavonoid database is comprehensive, containing values for total flavonoids in six classes for over 7000 foods and beverages [33]. Second, the stratified design of the HANDLS study with the relatively large number of African American participants allowed the investigation of associations by race [26]. Moreover, with the added dimensions of income (low to moderate) and residence (exclusively urban), the HANDLS study participants represent a socioeconomically disadvantaged group understudied in the literature. Third, the availability of TMT-A and B over three time points allowed longitudinal, as well as cross-sectional, analyses. Finally, a variety of confounders not related to diet were accounted for in the analyses.

5. Conclusions

Minimally adjusted analyses suggest beneficial associations between dietary intake of flavonoids and cognitive scores, as assessed via the TMT-A and TMT-B scores. However, accounting for confounders attenuated these relationships. The network of brain regions used to successfully perform the TMT-A and TMT-B and the potential impact of flavonoids on these regions has not yet been studied in sufficient detail and warrants further investigation. In order to advance the field and make conclusive statements based on evidence, it is important to continue the study of the relationship between flavonoid intake and cognition in samples with a wide range of flavonoid intake, especially individuals with intakes with high intakes.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu16091360/s1. Table S1: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for White study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S2: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for African American study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S3: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for all study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S4: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for White study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S5: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for African American study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S6: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for all study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; TableS7: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for White study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S8: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for African American study participants 50+ years, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S9: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for all study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S10: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for White study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S11: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for African American study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S12: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for all study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020; TableS13: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for White study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020; Table S14: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for African American study participants without MMSE exclusion, HANDLS 2004–2020.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.F.K. and R.S.S.; methodology, M.F.K., S.B.C., M.A.B. and J.D.G.; formal analysis, S.B.C. and M.A.B.; resources, A.J.M. and M.K.E.; writing—original draft preparation, M.F.K., S.B.C. and R.S.S.; writing—review and editing, M.F.K., S.B.C., R.S.S., J.D.G., A.J.M., M.K.E. and A.B.Z.; supervision, M.F.K. and A.J.M.; project administration, M.K.E. and A.B.Z.; funding acquisition, M.K.E. and A.B.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work is supported by the Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Aging, and National Institutes of Health, grant Z01-AG000513.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the National Institutes of Health Institutional Review Board (09AGN248). The study was originally approved in 2004 and the most recent continuing review was approved 22 October 2023.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon request to researchers with valid proposals who agree to the confidentiality agreement as required by our Institutional Review Board. We publicize our policies on our website https://handls.nih.gov (accessed 12 February 2024). Requests for data access may be sent to Alan Zonderman (co-author) or the study manager, Jennifer Norbeck, at [email protected].

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the HANDLS clinical staff and HANDLS participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Hale, J.M.; Schneider, D.C.; Mehta, N.K.; Myrskylä, M. Cognitive impairment in the U.S.: Lifetime risk, age at onset, and years impaired. SSM Popul. Health 2020, 11, 100577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Cognitive Impairment: A Call for Action, Now! Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/aging/pdf/cognitive_impairment/cogimp_poilicy_final.pdf (accessed on 6 March 2024).
  3. Jennings, A.; Steves, C.J.; Macgregor, A.; Spector, T.; Cassidy, A. Increased habitual flavonoid intake predicts attenuation of cognitive ageing in twins. BMC Med. 2021, 19, 185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bakoyiannis, I.; Daskalopoulou, A.; Pergialiotis, V.; Perrea, D. Phytochemicals and cognitive health: Are flavonoids doing the trick? Biomed. Pharmacother. 2019, 109, 1488–1497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Cheng, N.; Bell, L.; Lamport, D.J.; Williams, C.M. Dietary Flavonoids and Human Cognition: A Meta-Analysis. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2022, 66, e2100976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Yeh, T.S.; Yuan, C.; Ascherio, A.; Rosner, B.A.; Willett, W.C.; Blacker, D. Long-term Dietary Flavonoid Intake and Subjective Cognitive Decline in US Men and Women. Neurology 2021, 97, e1041–e1056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gardener, S.L.; Rainey-Smith, S.R.; Weinborn, M.; Bondonno, C.P.; Martins, R.N. Intake of Products Containing Anthocyanins, Flavanols, and Flavanones, and Cognitive Function: A Narrative Review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2021, 13, 640381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Cheatham, C.L.; Nieman, D.C.; Neilson, A.P.; Lila, M.A. Enhancing the Cognitive Effects of Flavonoids With Physical Activity: Is There a Case for the Gut Microbiome? Front. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 833202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Melrose, J. The Potential of Flavonoids and Flavonoid Metabolites in the Treatment of Neurodegenerative Pathology in Disorders of Cognitive Decline. Antioxidants 2023, 12, 663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chu, Z.; Han, S.; Luo, Y.; Zhou, Y.; Zhu, L.; Luo, F. Targeting gut-brain axis by dietary flavonoids ameliorate aging-related cognition decline: Evidences and mechanisms. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2023, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Godos, J.; Caraci, F.; Castellano, S.; Currenti, W.; Galvano, F.; Ferri, R.; Grosso, G. Association Between Dietary Flavonoids Intake and Cognitive Function in an Italian Cohort. Biomolecules 2020, 10, 1300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Shishtar, E.; Rogers, G.T.; Blumberg, J.B.; Au, R.; Jacques, P.F. Long-term dietary flavonoid intake and change in cognitive function in the Framingham Offspring cohort. Public Health Nutr. 2020, 23, 1576–1588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Moreira, A.; Diógenes, M.J.; de Mendonça, A.; Lunet, N.; Barros, H. Chocolate Consumption is Associated with a Lower Risk of Cognitive Decline. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2016, 53, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Nooyens, A.C.; Milder, I.E.; van Gelder, B.M.; Bueno-de-Mesquita, H.B.; van Boxtel, M.P.; Verschuren, W.M. Diet and cognitive decline at middle age: The role of antioxidants. Br. J. Nutr. 2015, 113, 1410–1417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Root, M.; Ravine, E.; Harper, A. Flavonol Intake and Cognitive Decline in Middle-Aged Adults. J. Med. Food 2015, 18, 1327–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Letenneur, L.; Proust-Lima, C.; Le Gouge, A.; Dartigues, J.F.; Barberger-Gateau, P. Flavonoid intake and cognitive decline over a 10-year period. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 165, 1364–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Crichton, G.E.; Bryan, J.; Murphy, K.J. Dietary antioxidants, cognitive function and dementia--a systematic review. Plant Foods Hum. Nutr. 2013, 68, 279–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. National Institute of Mental Health. Major Depresssion. Available online: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/major-depression (accessed on 6 March 2024).
  19. Miller, K.; Feucht, W.; Schmid, M. Bioactive Compounds of Strawberry and Blueberry and Their Potential Health Effects Based on Human Intervention Studies: A Brief Overview. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Nicolucci, C.; Padovani, M.; Rodrigues, F.d.C.; Fritsch, L.N.; Santos, A.C.; Priolli, D.G.; Sciani, J.M. Flavonoids: The use in mental health and related diseases. Nat. Prod. Res. 2023, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Calderaro, A.; Patanè, G.T.; Tellone, E.; Barreca, D.; Ficarra, S.; Misiti, F.; Laganà, G. The Neuroprotective Potentiality of Flavonoids on Alzheimer’s Disease. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23, 14835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Khan, H.; Ullah, H.; Aschner, M.; Cheang, W.S.; Akkol, E.K. Neuroprotective Effects of Quercetin in Alzheimer’s Disease. Biomolecules 2019, 10, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ullah, A.; Munir, S.; Badshah, S.L.; Khan, N.; Ghani, L.; Poulson, B.G.; Emwas, A.H.; Jaremko, M. Important Flavonoids and Their Role as a Therapeutic Agent. Molecules 2020, 25, 5243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bennett, D.A.; Schneider, J.A.; Buchman, A.S.; Barnes, L.L.; Boyle, P.A.; Wilson, R.S. Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2012, 9, 646–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fanelli Kuczmarski, M.; Sebastian, R.S.; Goldman, J.D.; Murayi, T.; Steinfeldt, L.C.; Eosso, J.R.; Moshfegh, A.J.; Zonderman, A.B.; Evans, M.K. Dietary Flavonoid Intakes Are Associated with Race but Not Income in an Urban Population. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. National Institiute on Aging. Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span Study Design. Available online: https://handls.nih.gov/01Design.htm (accessed on 15 November 2023).
  27. National Institutes of Health; National Institute on Aging; Intramural Research Program. Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span. Available online: https://handls.nih.gov/02Protocol.htm (accessed on 6 March 2024).
  28. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Mini-Mental State Examination. Available online: https://www.physio-pedia.com/Mini-Mental_State_Examination (accessed on 6 March 2024).
  30. Steinfeldt, L.; Anand, J.; Murayi, T. Food reporting patterns in the USDA Automated Multiple-Pass Method. Procedia Food Sci. 2013, 2, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. US Department of Agriculture. Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies. Available online: https://www.ars.usda.gov/northeast-area/beltsville-md-bhnrc/beltsville-human-nutrition-research-center/food-surveys-research-group/docs/fndds-download-databases/ (accessed on 3 March 2024).
  32. Sebastian, R.S.; Fanelli Kuczmarski, M.; Wilkinson Enns, C.; Goldman, J.D.; Murayi, T.; Moshfegh, A.J.; Zonderman, A.B.; Evans, M.K. Application of the Database of Flavonoid Values for USDA Food Codes 2007–2010 in Assessing Intake Differences between the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study and What We Eat in America (WWEIA), NHANES. J Food Compost Anal 2021, 104, 104124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Sebastian, R.S.; Wilkinson Enns, C.; Goldman, J.D.; Martin, C.L.; Steinfeldt, L.C.; Murayi, T.; Moshfegh, A.J. A New Database Facilitates Characterization of Flavonoid Intake, Sources, and Positive Associations with Diet Quality among US Adults. J. Nutr. 2015, 145, 1239–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Reitan, R.M. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1958, 8, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Corrigan, J.D.; Hinkeldey, N.S. Relationships between parts A and B of the Trail Making Test. J. Clin. Psychol. 1987, 43, 402–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. US Department of Health and Human Services. The 2004 HHS Poverty Guidelines. Available online: https://aspe.hhs.gov/2004-hhs-poverty-guidelines (accessed on 3 March 2024).
  37. Wilkinson, G.G. The Wide Range Achievement Test: Manual, 3rd ed.; Wide Range: Wilmington, DE, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kim, M.; Park, J.M. Factors affecting cognitive function according to gender in community-dwelling elderly individuals. Epidemiol. Health 2017, 39, e2017054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gil-Peinado, M.; Alacreu, M.; Ramos, H.; Sendra-Lillo, J.; García, C.; García-Lluch, G.; Lopez de Coca, T.; Sala, M.; Moreno, L. The A-to-Z factors associated with cognitive impairment. Results of the DeCo study. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1152527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Chobanian, A.V.; Bakris, G.L.; Black, H.R.; Cushman, W.C.; Green, L.A.; Izzo, J.L., Jr.; Jones, D.W.; Materson, B.J.; Oparil, S.; Wright, J.T., Jr.; et al. Seventh report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. Hypertension 2003, 42, 1206–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Austin, P.C.; White, I.R.; Lee, D.S.; van Buuren, S. Missing Data in Clinical Research: A Tutorial on Multiple Imputation. Can. J. Cardiol. 2021, 37, 1322–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Lamport, D.J.; Saunders, C.; Butler, L.T.; Spencer, J.P. Fruits, vegetables, 100% juices, and cognitive function. Nutr. Rev. 2014, 72, 774–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Socci, V.; Tempesta, D.; Desideri, G.; De Gennaro, L.; Ferrara, M. Enhancing Human Cognition with Cocoa Flavonoids. Front. Nutr. 2017, 4, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Desideri, G.; Kwik-Uribe, C.; Grassi, D.; Necozione, S.; Ghiadoni, L.; Mastroiacovo, D.; Raffaele, A.; Ferri, L.; Bocale, R.; Lechiara, M.C.; et al. Benefits in cognitive function, blood pressure, and insulin resistance through cocoa flavanol consumption in elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment: The Cocoa, Cognition, and Aging (CoCoA) study. Hypertension 2012, 60, 794–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Mastroiacovo, D.; Kwik-Uribe, C.; Grassi, D.; Necozione, S.; Raffaele, A.; Pistacchio, L.; Righetti, R.; Bocale, R.; Lechiara, M.C.; Marini, C.; et al. Cocoa flavanol consumption improves cognitive function, blood pressure control, and metabolic profile in elderly subjects: The Cocoa, Cognition, and Aging (CoCoA) Study—A randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015, 101, 538–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Brickman, A.M.; Khan, U.A.; Provenzano, F.A.; Yeung, L.K.; Suzuki, W.; Schroeter, H.; Wall, M.; Sloan, R.P.; Small, S.A. Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults. Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 1798–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lamport, D.J.; Williams, C.M. Polyphenols and Cognition In Humans: An Overview of Current Evidence from Recent Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses. Brain Plast. 2021, 6, 139–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kesse-Guyot, E.; Fezeu, L.; Andreeva, V.A.; Touvier, M.; Scalbert, A.; Hercberg, S.; Galan, P. Total and specific polyphenol intakes in midlife are associated with cognitive function measured 13 years later. J. Nutr. 2012, 142, 76–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Bell, L.; Lamport, D.J.; Butler, L.T.; Williams, C.M. A Review of the Cognitive Effects Observed in Humans Following Acute Supplementation with Flavonoids, and Their Associated Mechanisms of Action. Nutrients 2015, 7, 10290–10306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Crowe-White, K.M.; Evans, L.W.; Kuhnle, G.G.C.; Milenkovic, D.; Stote, K.; Wallace, T.; Handu, D.; Senkus, K.E. Flavan-3-ols and Cardiometabolic Health: First Ever Dietary Bioactive Guideline. Adv. Nutr. 2022, 13, 2070–2083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Fanelli Kuczmarski, M.; Beydoun, M.A.; Georgescu, M.F.; Noren Hooten, N.; Mode, N.A.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Diet Quality Trajectories over Adulthood in a Biracial Urban Sample from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span Study. Nutrients 2023, 15, 3099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Spencer, S.J.; Korosi, A.; Layé, S.; Shukitt-Hale, B.; Barrientos, R.M. Food for thought: How nutrition impacts cognition and emotion. NPJ Sci. Food 2017, 1, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Ahles, S.; Joris, P.J.; Plat, J. Effects of Berry Anthocyanins on Cognitive Performance, Vascular Function and Cardiometabolic Risk Markers: A Systematic Review of Randomized Placebo-Controlled Intervention Studies in Humans. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2021, 22, 6482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Salthouse, T.A. What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance? Intelligence 2011, 39, 222–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. MacPherson, S.E.; Cox, S.R.; Dickie, D.A.; Karama, S.; Starr, J.M.; Evans, A.C.; Bastin, M.E.; Wardlaw, J.M.; Deary, I.J. Processing speed and the relationship between Trail Making Test-B performance, cortical thinning and white matter microstructure in older adults. Cortex 2017, 95, 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Salthouse, T.A. Cognitive correlates of cross-sectional differences and longitudinal changes in trail making performance. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2011, 33, 242–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Harada, C.N.; Natelson Love, M.C.; Triebel, K.L. Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2013, 29, 737–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee. Scientific Report of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee: Advisory Report to the Secretary of Health and Human Services and the Secretary of Agriculture; U.S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service: Washington, DC, USA, 2015.
  60. CDC. Cigarette Smoking Among Adults—United States, 2004. In MMWR Weekly; CDC: Atlanta, GA USA, 2005; Volume 54, pp. 1121–1124. [Google Scholar]
  61. Ott, A.; Andersen, K.; Dewey, M.E.; Letenneur, L.; Brayne, C.; Copeland, J.R.; Dartigues, J.F.; Kragh-Sorensen, P.; Lobo, A.; Martinez-Lage, J.M.; et al. Effect of smoking on global cognitive function in nondemented elderly. Neurology 2004, 62, 920–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Singh, L.; Chandra, R.; Pai, M.; Singh, A.; Mazumdar, S.; Balhara, Y.P.S.; Singh, P.K.; Singh, S. How Does Tobacco Use Affect the Cognition of Older Adults? A Propensity Score Matching Analysis based on a Large-Scale Survey. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2023, 26, 342–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Durazzo, T.C.; Meyerhoff, D.J.; Nixon, S.J. A comprehensive assessment of neurocognition in middle-aged chronic cigarette smokers. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2012, 122, 105–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Beydoun, M.A.; Fanelli-Kuczmarski, M.T.; Beydoun, H.A.; Dore, G.A.; Canas, J.A.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Dairy product consumption and its association with metabolic disturbance in a prospective study of urban adults. Br. J. Nutr. 2018, 119, 706–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Wright, R.S.; Waldstein, S.R.; Kuczmarski, M.F.; Pohlig, R.T.; Gerassimakis, C.S.; Gaynor, B.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Diet quality and cognitive function in an urban sample: Findings from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. Public Health Nutr. 2017, 20, 92–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cotugna, N.; Fanelli-Kuczmarski, M.; Clymer, J.; Hotchkiss, L.; Zonderman, A.B.; Evans, M.K. Sodium intake of special populations in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity Across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. Prev. Med. 2013, 57, 334–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ogden, C.L.; Carroll, M.D.; Curtin, L.R.; McDowell, M.A.; Tabak, C.J.; Flegal, K.M. Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999–2004. JAMA 2006, 295, 1549–1555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Palmer, M.K.; Toth, P.P. Trends in Lipids, Obesity, Metabolic Syndrome, and Diabetes Mellitus in the United States: An NHANES Analysis (2003–2004 to 2013–2014). Obes. (Silver Spring) 2019, 27, 309–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ong, K.L.; Cheung, B.M.; Man, Y.B.; Lau, C.P.; Lam, K.S. Prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension among United States adults 1999–2004. Hypertension 2007, 49, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Poslusna, K.; Ruprich, J.; de Vries, J.H.; Jakubikova, M.; van’t Veer, P. Misreporting of energy and micronutrient intake estimated by food records and 24 h recalls, control and adjustment methods in practice. Br. J. Nutr. 2009, 101 (Suppl. S2), S73–S85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Scarmeas, N.; Anastasiou, C.A.; Yannakoulia, M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2018, 17, 1006–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Moshfegh, A.J.; Rhodes, D.G.; Baer, D.J.; Murayi, T.; Clemens, J.C.; Rumpler, W.V.; Paul, D.R.; Sebastian, R.S.; Kuczynski, K.J.; Ingwersen, L.A.; et al. The US Department of Agriculture Automated Multiple-Pass Method reduces bias in the collection of energy intakes. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 88, 324–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow diagram for study participation selection from Wave 1, HANDLS 2004–2009. Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test, and MMSE, Mini-Mental State Exam. a The primary analytic sample consisted of the 1947 adults shown here. However, an additional analysis used the 2140 adults available prior to the exclusion for MMSE, while a second additional analysis used the 846 adults that remained after excluding 1101 individuals <50 years of age at visit 1.
Figure 1. Flow diagram for study participation selection from Wave 1, HANDLS 2004–2009. Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test, and MMSE, Mini-Mental State Exam. a The primary analytic sample consisted of the 1947 adults shown here. However, an additional analysis used the 2140 adults available prior to the exclusion for MMSE, while a second additional analysis used the 846 adults that remained after excluding 1101 individuals <50 years of age at visit 1.
Nutrients 16 01360 g001
Figure 2. Linear prediction of ln(TMT-A), by time and visit 1 anthocyanidin levels, among all study subjects using the basic model, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Figure 2. Linear prediction of ln(TMT-A), by time and visit 1 anthocyanidin levels, among all study subjects using the basic model, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Nutrients 16 01360 g002
Table 1. Description of visit 1 characteristics, overall analytical sample, and race, HANDLS 2004–2009.
Table 1. Description of visit 1 characteristics, overall analytical sample, and race, HANDLS 2004–2009.
CharacteristicAll Adults
(n = 1947)
White Adults
(n = 823)
African American Adults
(n = 1124)
X ¯ ± S E X ¯ ± S E X ¯ ± S E
Energy Intake (kcal/d)2015 ± 222036 ± 321999 ± 30
Age, years48.06 ± 0.2148.28 ± 0.3347.90 ± 0.28
WRAT3 score43.09 ± 0.1645.89 ± 0.2341.05 ± 0.21
CES-D14.58 ± 0.2514.80 ± 0.4014.41 ± 0.32
Body Mass Index29.98 ± 0.1830.05 ± 0.2629.93 ± 0.23
Education (yrs)12.31 ± 0.0612.56 ± 0.1012.13 ± 0.07
% ± SE% ± SE% ± SE
Male Sex41.60 ± 1.1240.58 ± 1.7142.35 ± 1.47
Poverty < 125% a42.01 ± 1.1231.11 ± 1.6150.00 ± 1.49
High Cholesterol49.25 ± 1.1856.37 ± 1.8144.03 ± 1.56
Diabetes15.83 ± 0.8315.80 ± 1.2915.85 ± 1.10
Hypertension44.58 ± 1.1438.13 ± 1.7249.31 ± 1.50
Current smoker47.24 ± 1.1643.78 ± 1.8049.78 ± 1.54
Current drug user b17.84 ± 0.8912.29 ± 1.1821.90 ± 1.27
Abbreviations: SE, standard error; WRAT3, Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd ed; CES-D, Center of Epidemiological Studies-Depression; kcal/d, kilocalories/day; yrs, years. a Poverty < 125% of 2004 DHHS levels [36]. b Drug use is defined as illicit drugs, specifically, marijuana, cocaine, and opiates.
Table 2. Visit 1 flavonoid intake and cognitive function, HANDLS 2004–2009.
Table 2. Visit 1 flavonoid intake and cognitive function, HANDLS 2004–2009.
Characteristic X ¯ ± S E MedianIQR (Q1–Q3)
Total253.31 ± 11.5468.11(19.66–297.47)
Flavonols18.14 ± 0.4612.85(6.18–23.69)
Flavones0.62 ± 0.020.32(0.11–0.76)
Flavanones12.44 ± 0.620.38(0.00–11.82)
Flavan-3-ols214.43 ± 11.1314.73(3.30–248.11)
Anthocyanins6.61 ± 0.450.36(0.00–3.39)
TMT-A37.88 ± 1.0631.00(25.00–41.00)
TMT-B144.58 ± 3.5387.00(62.00–139.00)
Abbreviations: SE, standard error; IQR, interquartile range; Q, quartile; mg, milligrams; sec, seconds; TMT, Trails Making Test.
Table 3. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for all study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Table 3. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-A) for all study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Basic Model bDemographic Model cLifestyle Model dClinical Model e
β ^   ± SEp-Value β ^   ± SEp-Value β ^   ± SEp-Value β ^ ± SEp-Value
Flavonoid Main Effect: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-A)
 Total Flavonoids−0.00034 ± 0.000190.0650.00011 ± 0.000170.5230.00014 ± 0.000170.4010.00005 ± 0.000170.759
 Flavones−0.20026 ± 0.096130.0370.08503 ± 0.088170.3350.13137 ± 0.088620.1380.08991 ± 0.087620.305
 Flavonols−0.01021 ± 0.004680.0290.00131 ± 0.004260.7590.00491 ± 0.004330.2560.00067 ± 0.004240.874
 Flavonones−0.00209 ± 0.003500.549−0.00461 ± 0.003160.144−0.00308 ± 0.003180.332−0.00399 ± 0.003140.205
 Flavan-3-ols−0.00032 ± 0.000190.0960.00013 ± 0.000180.4530.00015 ± 0.000180.3810.00007 ± 0.000170.697
 Anthocyanidins−0.01301 ± 0.004730.006−0.00395 ± 0.004320.361−0.00199 ± 0.004340.647−0.00281 ± 0.004310.514
Flavonoid*Time Interaction: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-A) over time
 Total Flavonoids−0.00001 ± 0.000020.823−0.00001 ± 0.000020.618−0.00001 ± 0.000020.634−0.00001 ± 0.000020.631
 Flavones−0.02099 ± 0.012070.082−0.02334 ± 0.011950.051−0.02449 ± 0.011960.041−0.02318 ± 0.011950.052
 Flavonols0.00003 ± 0.000590.964−0.00012 ± 0.000590.838−0.00010 ± 0.000590.863−0.00015 ± 0.000590.798
 Flavonones−0.00017 ± 0.000430.690−0.00027 ± 0.000430.526−0.00028 ± 0.000430.506−0.00024 ± 0.000430.569
 Flavan-3-ols0.00000 ± 0.000020.864−0.00001 ± 0.000020.673−0.00001 ± 0.000020.688−0.00001 ± 0.000020.682
 Anthocyanidins−0.00003 ± 0.000500.953−0.00018 ± 0.000490.719−0.00018 ± 0.000490.717−0.00014 ± 0.000490.770
Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; SE, standard error. a Associations are reported for a 10-unit increment in visit 1 flavonoid intake. b Basic model includes fixed effects for visit 1 flavonoid intake, time, and visit 1 flavonoid intake*time. c Demographic model is the basic model adjusted for visit 1 age in years, age-squared, sex, race, poverty status, education in years, and Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) scores. d Lifestyle model is the demographic model adjusted for current smoking status, current drug use, and total energy intake at visit 1. e Clinical model is the demographic model adjusted for diabetes, hypertension, high cholesterol, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), and body mass index (BMI).
Table 4. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for all study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Table 4. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for all study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Basic Model bDemographic Model cLifestyle Model dClinical Model e
β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value
Flavonoid Main Effect: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B)
 Total Flavonoids−0.00105 ± 0.000310.001−0.00015 ± 0.000270.586−0.00011 ± 0.000270.684−0.00022 ± 0.000260.410
 Flavones−0.88659 ± 0.16112<0.001−0.15912 ± 0.139360.254−0.10337 ± 0.140330.461−0.13482 ± 0.137540.327
 Flavonols−0.03028 ± 0.00787<0.001−0.00363 ± 0.006730.5900.00078 ± 0.006850.910−0.00422 ± 0.006650.526
 Flavonones0.00860 ± 0.005890.1440.00591 ± 0.004990.2370.00813 ± 0.005020.1060.00738 ± 0.004930.134
 Flavan-3-ols−0.00103 ± 0.000330.002−0.00016 ± 0.000280.569−0.00014 ± 0.000280.621−0.00025 ± 0.000270.372
 Anthocyanidins−0.03067 ± 0.00799<0.001−0.00381 ± 0.006860.578−0.00120 ± 0.006890.862−0.00115 ± 0.006780.865
Flavonoid*Time Interaction: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-B) over time
 Total Flavonoids0.00007 ± 0.000030.0330.00006 ± 0.000030.0700.00006 ± 0.000030.0670.00005 ± 0.000030.091
 Flavones0.01514 ± 0.016440.3580.01254 ± 0.016300.4420.01169 ± 0.016330.4740.01189 ± 0.016290.466
 Flavonols0.00177 ± 0.000820.0310.00152 ± 0.000810.0590.00154 ± 0.000810.0570.00131 ± 0.000810.105
 Flavonones−0.00038 ± 0.000590.521−0.00052 ± 0.000590.370−0.00054 ± 0.000590.360−0.00055 ± 0.000590.345
 Flavan-3-ols0.00007 ± 0.000030.0330.00006 ± 0.000030.0670.00006 ± 0.000030.0640.00006 ± 0.000030.085
 Anthocyanidins0.00044 ± 0.000680.5200.00027 ± 0.000670.6840.00026 ± 0.000670.6970.00025 ± 0.000670.714
Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; SE, standard error. a Associations are reported for a 10-unit increment in visit 1 flavonoid intake. b Basic model includes fixed effects for visit 1 flavonoid intake, time, and visit 1 flavonoid intake*time. c Demographic model is the basic model adjusted for visit 1 age in years, age-squared, sex, race, poverty status, education in years, and Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) scores. d Lifestyle model is the demographic model adjusted for current smoking status, current drug use, and total energy intake at visit 1. e Clinical model is the demographic model adjusted for diabetes, hypertension, high cholesterol, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), and body mass index (BMI).
Table 5. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for White study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Table 5. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for White study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Basic Model bDemographic Model cLifestyle Model dClinical Model e
β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value
Flavonoid Main Effect: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B)
 Total Flavonoids−0.00027 ± 0.000300.371−0.00018 ± 0.000260.505−0.00016 ± 0.000260.539−0.00024 ± 0.000260.364
 Flavones−0.74494 ± 0.17815<0.001−0.34641 ± 0.157310.028−0.31370 ± 0.158900.048−0.33389 ± 0.155900.032
 Flavonols−0.01483 ± 0.008170.070−0.00545 ± 0.007050.439−0.00389 ± 0.007130.585−0.00635 ± 0.007000.364
 Flavonones−0.01065 ± 0.009670.271−0.00173 ± 0.008450.838−0.00008 ± 0.008500.9920.00119 ± 0.008390.887
 Flavan-3-ols−0.00019 ± 0.000320.538−0.00015 ± 0.000270.588−0.00014 ± 0.000270.610−0.00022 ± 0.000270.420
 Anthocyanidins−0.04363 ± 0.00896<0.001−0.02208 ± 0.008020.006−0.02080 ± 0.008060.010−0.02031 ± 0.007970.011
Flavonoid*Time Interaction: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-B) over time
 Total Flavonoids0.00005 ± 0.000030.1180.00004 ± 0.000030.2110.00004 ± 0.000030.2040.00004 ± 0.000030.221
 Flavones0.02941 ± 0.019830.1380.02882 ± 0.019670.1430.02839 ± 0.019730.1500.03199 ± 0.019630.103
 Flavonols0.00161 ± 0.000870.0630.00133 ± 0.000860.1200.00135 ± 0.000860.1170.00125 ± 0.000860.145
 Flavonones−0.00033 ± 0.001170.778−0.00042 ± 0.001150.713−0.00046 ± 0.001160.693−0.00050 ± 0.001150.664
 Flavan-3-ols0.00005 ± 0.000030.1330.00004 ± 0.000030.2300.00004 ± 0.000030.2210.00004 ± 0.000030.237
 Anthocyanidins0.00112 ± 0.000810.1690.00091 ± 0.000800.2590.00088 ± 0.000810.2740.00085 ± 0.000800.287
Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; SE, standard error. a Associations are reported for a 10-unit increment in visit 1 flavonoid intake. b Basic model includes fixed effects for visit 1 flavonoid intake, time, and visit 1 flavonoid intake*time. c Demographic model is the basic model adjusted for visit 1 age in years, age-squared, sex, race, poverty status, education in years, and Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) scores. d Lifestyle model is the demographic model adjusted for current smoking status, current drug use, and total energy intake at visit 1. e Clinical model is the demographic model adjusted for diabetes, hypertension, high cholesterol, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), and body mass index (BMI).
Table 6. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for African American study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Table 6. Association a between visit 1 flavonoid intake and ln(TMT-B) for African American study participants, HANDLS 2004–2020.
Basic Model bDemographic Model cLifestyle Model dClinical Model e
β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value β ^   ± S E p-Value
Flavonoid Main Effect: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and visit 1 ln(TMT-B)
 Total Flavonoids−0.00044 ± 0.000780.5720.00028 ± 0.000690.6890.00041 ± 0.000690.5520.00037 ± 0.000680.584
 Flavones−0.30104 ± 0.271410.2680.12748 ± 0.244010.6010.21299 ± 0.245380.3850.15898 ± 0.240350.508
 Flavonols−0.00647 ± 0.015310.6730.00451 ± 0.013800.7440.01712 ± 0.014340.2330.00721 ± 0.013600.596
 Flavonones0.00499 ± 0.007050.4790.00868 ± 0.006290.1680.01140 ± 0.006340.0720.00915 ± 0.006200.140
 Flavan-3-ols−0.00057 ± 0.000810.4810.00008 ± 0.000720.9140.00014 ± 0.000720.8470.00015 ± 0.000710.828
 Anthocyanidins0.01909 ± 0.012830.1370.02348 ± 0.011440.0400.02879 ± 0.011530.0130.02559 ± 0.011270.023
Flavonoid*Time Interaction: Association between visit 1 flavonoid intake and change in ln(TMT-B) over time
 Total Flavonoids0.00008 ± 0.000080.2930.00008 ± 0.000080.3290.00008 ± 0.000080.3180.00006 ± 0.000080.444
 Flavones−0.01426 ± 0.025920.582−0.01687 ± 0.025780.513−0.01925 ± 0.025840.456−0.02176 ± 0.025830.399
 Flavonols0.00108 ± 0.001590.4970.00110 ± 0.001580.4860.00113 ± 0.001580.4750.00056 ± 0.001580.723
 Flavonones−0.00025 ± 0.000710.727−0.00040 ± 0.000710.577−0.00041 ± 0.000710.563−0.00041 ± 0.000710.562
 Flavan-3-ols0.00010 ± 0.000080.2410.00009 ± 0.000080.2640.00009 ± 0.000080.2550.00008 ± 0.000080.359
 Anthocyanidins−0.00108 ± 0.001090.321−0.00106 ± 0.001080.325−0.00108 ± 0.001080.317−0.00109 ± 0.001080.313
Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; SE, standard error. a Associations are reported for a 10-unit increment in visit 1 flavonoid intake. b Basic model includes fixed effects for visit 1 flavonoid intake, time, and visit 1 flavonoid intake*time. c Demographic model is the basic model adjusted for visit 1 age in years, age-squared, sex, race, poverty status, education in years, and Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) scores. d Lifestyle model is the demographic model adjusted for current smoking status, current drug use, and total energy intake at visit 1. e Clinical model is the demographic model adjusted for diabetes, hypertension, high cholesterol, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), and body mass index (BMI).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fanelli Kuczmarski, M.; Crawford, S.B.; Sebastian, R.S.; Beydoun, M.A.; Goldman, J.D.; Moshfegh, A.J.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Association between Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Executive Function among African American and White Adults in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study. Nutrients 2024, 16, 1360. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16091360

AMA Style

Fanelli Kuczmarski M, Crawford SB, Sebastian RS, Beydoun MA, Goldman JD, Moshfegh AJ, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Association between Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Executive Function among African American and White Adults in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study. Nutrients. 2024; 16(9):1360. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16091360

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fanelli Kuczmarski, Marie, Sara B. Crawford, Rhonda S. Sebastian, May A. Beydoun, Joseph D. Goldman, Alanna J. Moshfegh, Michele K. Evans, and Alan B. Zonderman. 2024. "Association between Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Executive Function among African American and White Adults in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study" Nutrients 16, no. 9: 1360. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16091360

APA Style

Fanelli Kuczmarski, M., Crawford, S. B., Sebastian, R. S., Beydoun, M. A., Goldman, J. D., Moshfegh, A. J., Evans, M. K., & Zonderman, A. B. (2024). Association between Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Executive Function among African American and White Adults in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) Study. Nutrients, 16(9), 1360. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16091360

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop