The Impact of Free and Added Sugars on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Data
2.2. Search Strategy
2.3. Eligibility Criteria
2.4. Data Extraction
2.5. Quality Assessment
2.6. Analysis of Cognitive Function
3. Results
3.1. Study Selection
3.2. Study Characteristics
3.3. Assessment of Study Quality
3.4. Cognitive Outcomes and Measures
3.5. Observational—Cross-Sectional Studies
3.6. Observational—Cohort Studies
3.7. Intervention Studies
Author/s | Population | Cognitive Measures | Intervention/Control | Fasting | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adan and Serra-Grabulosa, 2010 [51] | N = 72 young adults (36 male) aged 18–25 (M = 21.07, SD = 1.70) | RAVLT, Purdue–Pegboard, Benton Judgement of Line Orientation Test (JoLO), WCST, California Computerized Assessment Package (CalCAP), digit span of WAIS |
| 8 h | Water performed worse than treatment groups (p = 0.026). Glucose performed better on Purdue pegboard assembly than placebo or caffeine (p = 0.039). No effect of treatment on reaction time, WAIS, WCST, and RAVLT. |
Allen et al., 1996 [62] | N = 28 elderly adults (6 male) aged 61–87 (M = 73) | Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, Taylor Complex Figure, dichotic listening, TMT, verbal fluency, Boston naming test, Meier visual test, grooved pegboard, figural fluency |
| 9 h | Glucose enhanced delayed recall and verbal and figure fluency (ps < 0.001). No effect of glucose on other measures. Poor glucose regulation associated with worse performance in dichotic listening (p < 0.005) and verbal fluency (p < 0.05). |
Azari, 1993 [63] | N = 18 young male adults aged 19–25 (M = 21, SD = 1.65) | Word recall |
| 4.5 h | No effect of glucose or BGL on memory. |
Benton et al., 1987 [64] | N = 60 children (30 male) aged 6 or 7 years old. | Paradigm of Shakow |
| 2–3 h | Improved reaction time after glucose (p ≤ 0.05). Glucose group reported increased quiet concentration (p < 0.001) and less likely to fidget (p < 0.04). |
Benton, 1990 [65] | T1: N = 20 male students (M age = 20.3 and 20.5 per group) T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (20 male; M age = 21.2 and 20.9) | T1: choice reaction time task T2: long arithmetic |
| 4 h | T1: Glucose associated with fewer errors (p < 0.05). T2: No effect of glucose on coordination or arithmetic. Males performed better on arithmetic (p < 0.001) and coordination (p < 0.01). |
Benton and Owens, 1993 [66] | T1: N = 153 undergraduate students (100 male, M age = 21.6, SD = 4.8; 53 female, M age = 21.8, SD = 5.2) T2: N = 53 female undergraduates (M = 21.5, SD = 5.0) | T1: word list, pattern recognition T2: word list, Weschler memory scale | T1:
| No | T1: No effect of glucose on recall or spatial memory. Increased BGL associated with more words remembered (p < 0.002). T2: No effect of glucose on Weschler or word list. Falling BGL in glucose group associated with improved memory; falling BGL in placebo group associated with worse memory. |
Benton et al., 1994 [67] | T1: N = 70 female undergraduates (M age = 21.46) T2: N = 50 male undergraduates (M age = 21.7) | T1: RIPT T2: STROOP |
| No | Glucose associated with more errors than placebo in RIPT (p < 0.031). No effect of glucose on memory, reaction time, or STROOP. |
Benton and Stevens, 2008 [68] | N = 16 children (7 male) aged 9 or 10 years | Recall of objects test, pattern recognition, paradigm of Shakow |
| No | More pictures recalled after glucose consumption (p < 0.025). No effect of glucose on spatial memory. |
Best et al., 2008 [69] | N = 45 adults (19 male) aged 40–63 (M = 52.1, SD = 5.9) | RAVLT, Self-Ordered Pointing Task (SOPT), digit span, WAIS matrix reasoning |
| 2 h | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Birnie et al., 2015 [70] | N = 16 adults (8 male) aged 18–45 (M = 23.7, SD = 5.0) | SART, Short Imaginal Processes Inventory (SIPI) |
| 2 h | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Brandt et al., 2006 [71] | T1: N = 40 undergraduate students (20 male) aged 18–25 T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (14 male) aged 18–36 (M = 22) | Word list |
| 2 h | No effect of glucose on emotional memory enhancement. BGL were negatively associated with positive items remembered (p < 0.05). Better glucoregulation associated with improved memory for negative items and fewer errors (ps < 0.05). |
Brandt et al., 2010 [72] | T1: N = 40 undergraduate students (5 male) aged 18–34 (M = 19.1) T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (27 male) aged 18–37 (M = 21) | Word list | T1:
| 2 h | Greater false alarm rate after 25 g glucose condition (p < 0.05) Recognition memory was marginally improved in the aspartame group (p = 0.05). |
Brandt et al., 2013 [73] | N = 60 undergraduate students (14 male; M age = 19.7) | STROOP |
| Overnight | Shorter reaction times in congruent and incongruent conditions after glucose consumption (ps < 0.05). Greater facilitative effect of glucose in incongruent (higher cognitive load) task. |
Brandt, 2015 [74] | N = 41 undergraduate students (9 male; M age = 19.47) | Process dissociation procedure |
| Overnight | Exclusion (high effort condition) was superior in glucose condition (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on inclusion. Aspartame scored higher in familiarity (p < 0.01) (low effort condition). No effect of treatment on recollection. |
Brody and Wolitzky, 1983 [75] | N = 59 undergraduate students (28 male) aged 16–24 (M = 18.7) | Serial sevens |
| 8 h | No effect of treatment. |
Brown and Riby, 2013 [76] | N = 35 young adults (14 male) aged 18–35 (M = 22.17, SD = 5.97) | Item recognition task, STROOP |
| 2 h | Glucose facilitation effect in more difficult task conditions, but glucose impaired performance on new items (low cognitive load) (p = 0.02). No effect of glucose on attention. |
Craft et al., 1994 [77] | N = 59 (27 younger adults aged 19–28, M = 20.8; 32 older adults aged 58–77, M = 68.5) | Paced Serial Addition Test, paragraph recall, modified CVLT, pattern recall and recognition, serial reaction time, word generation |
| Overnight | Glucose improved declarative memory in older males with good glycaemic recovery (p < 0.01). Glucose improved recall in younger men with poor glycaemic recovery (p < 0.01). Younger men with good glycaemic recovery saw memory deterioration after glucose consumption (p < 0.001). No effect of glucose on procedural memory, working memory, or verbal fluency. |
Donohoe and Benton, 1999a [78] | T1: N = 67 female undergraduate students (M age = 21.8, SD = 5.1) T2: N = 69 female undergraduates (M age = 20.2, SD = 2.1) | T1: water jar test, Finding Embedded Figures Test, Baddeley Logical Reasoning Task T2: Controlled Oral Word Association Test, WAIS—Block design subtest, Porteus maze—adults/14 year olds |
| No | T1: No effect of glucose on outcomes. T2: Improved verbal fluency after glucose consumption (p < 0.001). Faster time to solve Porteus maze for 14 year olds after glucose (p < 0.002). No effect of glucose on block design. |
Donohoe and Benton, 1999b [79] | N = 188 female undergraduate students (M age 21, SD = 4) | T1: RIPT, word list |
| No | Faster recall for glucose compared to control (p < 0.001). No effect of glucose on recall. More errors in the placebo group at 2, 4 (ps < 0.01), and 6 min (p < 0.05) (but not 8 or 10 min) of RIPT vigilance task. |
Flint and Turek, 2003 [80] | N = 67 Undergraduate students (15 male) aged 18–50 (M = 19.49, SD = 4.35) | Test of Variable of Attention (TOVA) |
| 8 h | 100 mg/kg showed impaired impulsivity (greater postcommission response time variability) (p < 0.01). No effect of treatment on any other measure. |
Ford et al., 2002 [81] | N = 20 undergraduate students aged 20–23 | Tailored version of CDR Assessment Battery |
| Overnight | No effect of glucose on memory. |
Foster et al, 1998 [52] | N = 30 female young adults aged 18–22 years (M = 19.5) | Modified CVLT, ROCF, digit span |
| 9 h | Improved delayed recall after glucose consumption (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on any other outcome. |
Giles et al., 2018 [82] | N = 105 young adults (74 female; M age = 22.5, SD = 6.6) | Immediate and delayed recall, STROOP, N-back task, continuous performance task |
| 12 h | Improved performance on sustained attention after sugar intake (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on selective attention, verbal memory, or working memory. |
Ginieis et al., 2018 [83] | N = 49 young adults (26 fasted (15 female, M age = 22.6, SD = 4.2) or 23 nonfasted (13 female, M age = 24.3, SD = 4.9)) | Simple response time task, arithmetic task, STROOP |
| 10 h | Slower reaction time after glucose consumption in the fasting group for simple response task and arithmetic task (p < 0.05). STROOP response time was impaired in the glucose and sucrose conditions, independent of fasting (p < 0.001). |
Gonder-Frederick et al., 1987 [84] | N = 11 elderly adults aged 58–76 (M = 67.4, SD = 5.7) | WAIS memory subscales |
| 9 h | Glucose group had improved performance on narrative memory (p = 0.024) and total Weschler scale (p = 0.009) (one-tailed). BGL at 30 and 60 min after beverage consumption was negatively associated with narrative memory, visual memory, and total Weschler scale (ps < 0.05). |
Hope et al., 2013 [85] | T1: N = 12 young adults (6 male; M age = 25.1, SD = 2.1). T2: N = 24 young adults (3 male; M age = 20.1, SD = 0.7) | (1) Flanker task, simple version (2) Flanker task, demanding version |
| No | T1: Slower reaction time after glucose consumption (p = 0.03) only when glucose administered in session 1 and placebo in session 2. T2: Slower reaction time after glucose consumption (p = 0.045). No effect of glucose on error rates. |
Jones et al., 2012 [32] | N = 18 young adults (5 male) aged 18–37 (M age = 19) | Tailored version of CDR Assessment Battery |
| 12 h | Enhancements in attention (p < 0.01) and speed (p < 0.05) 15 min after glucose ingestion. Impairments in working memory (p < 0.05) 60 min after glucose ingestion. Speed enhanced 15 min after fat consumption (p < 0.05). Working memory enhanced 15 min following protein ingestion (p < 0.05). Episodic memory and memory quality enhanced 60 min following protein ingestion (p < 0.01). |
Kaplan et al., 2000 [31] | N = 20 older adults (10 male) aged 60–82 | Tailored version of RAVLT, TMT, attention task (television recall) |
| 10–12 h | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. Significant predictors of declarative memory were glycaemic regulation, BMI, and beta-cell function. |
Kaplan et al., 2001 [33] | N = 22 older adults (11 male) aged 61–79 (M = 71.2, SD = 1.3) | Tailored version of RAVLT, paragraph recall, TMT, attention task (television recall) |
| 10–12 h | Protein, glucose, and fat (ps < 0.001) improved delayed recall at 15 min compared to placebo. Protein (p = 0.04), glucose (p = 0.02), and fat (p = 0.008) improved immediate recall at 15 min compared to placebo. No effect of treatment on recall or TMT at 60 min. Fat ingestion improved attention at 60 min. |
Kennedy and Scholey, 2000 [86] | N = 20 young adults (6 male) aged 19–30 (M = 20.4) | Serial threes and sevens, word retrieval |
| 9 h | Improved performance in serial sevens after glucose ingestion (p < 0.01). Performance in serial threes and sevens positively associated with fall in BGL during task (ps < 0.05). No significant effect of glucose on word retrieval. |
Maben and Smith, 1996 [49] | N = 48 young adults (24 male) aged 18–32 | Word list, Baddeley Logical Reasoning Task, Semantic memory task (not described) |
| 9 h | Sugar and aspartame conditions performed more accurately on logical reasoning than control (p < 0.05) but more slowly (p < 0.001). No effect of sugar or aspartame on free recall, semantic memory, or recognition memory. |
Macpherson et al., 2015 [87] | N = 48 (24 young adults aged 18–23 (M = 20.6, SD = 1.4) and 24 older adults aged 65–85 (M = 72.5, SD = 5.1)) | Auditory word recognition, target tracking task |
| 12 h | Tracking precision improved in older adults after glucose ingestion (p = 0.05) after controlling for BMI, IQ, and glucose regulation. No effect of glucose in younger adults. |
Mantantzis et al., 2018 [88] | N = 112 (54 undergraduate students aged 18–27 and 58 older adults aged 65–82) | Choice reaction time task |
| 2 h | Glucose improved speed (p = 0.001) and accuracy (p = 0.007) in the older adult group only. |
Martin and Benton, 1999 [89] | N = 80 female undergraduates (M age = 22.6) | Consonant trigrams |
| Overnight fast vs. no fast | Performance improved over time for fasting plus glucose (p < 0.001), breakfast plus glucose (p < 0.03), and breakfast without glucose (p < 0.001). Falling BGL during the task was associated with better recall (p < 0.001). |
Meikle et al., 2004 [53] | N = 25 adults (17 female) aged 18–52 (M = 28.4, SD = 9.3). Younger group (N = 14, M age = 21.8, SD = 3.3) and middle-aged group (N = 11, M age = 38.4, SD = 6.7) | Choice reaction time task, TMT, letter cancelation test, word retrieval, word list |
| 9 h | Older adults saw improvement in reaction time for high memory load tasks after 25 g (p < 0.05) or 50 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. All participants had improved delayed free recall after 25 g (p < 0.05) and 50 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. |
Meikle et al., 2005 [54] | T1: 37 young adults (29 female aged 17–48 (M = 28.3) T2: N = 24 young adults (20 female) aged 18–20 (M = 18.9) | Word list | T1:
| 9 h | T1: No main effect of treatment post hoc; for higher difficulty task, placebo forgot more words than glucose prelearning (p < 0.01) and postlearning groups (p < 0.05) T2: Glucose performed better on high memory load tasks (p < 0.01) but not on high cognitive demand tasks. |
Messier et al., 1998 [90] | N = 100 female undergraduate students aged 17–48 (M = 21.3, SD = 4.6) | Word list |
| 12 h | Increased primacy word recall after 10, 300, 500, 800, and 1000 mg/kg compared to one or both controls. Increased recency word performance after 500 mg/kg glucose. Impaired performance on recency word recall after 10 and 300 mg/kg glucose. |
Miller et al., 2013 [91] | N = 36 adults (11 male; M age = 23.3, SD = 7.0) | Anagram problem solving |
| 3 h | Fructose (p = 0.01) and glucose (p < 0.01) solved more problems than placebo. |
Mohanty and Flint, 2001 [92] | N = 70 undergraduate students (22 male; M age = 20.6, SD = 4.3) | Recall of object location task (pattern recognition) |
| 8 h | In the emotional condition, more errors were made following glucose ingestion (no p values given). In the neutral condition, fewer errors were made following 100 mg/kg glucose compared to placebo. Response time was slower following 50 g glucose. More errors were made in spatial memory retention following glucose ingestion. |
Owen et al., 2010 [55] | N = 90 undergraduate students (29 male) aged 18–30 (M = 21) | Word list, face recognition test |
| 12 h | Improvement in implicit memory following 60 g glucose (p < 0.01). More false alarms after 25 g glucose compared to placebo or 60 g glucose (ps = 0.03). |
Owen et al., 2012 [56] | N = 30 adults aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Word list, serial threes and sevens, Corsi block-tapping, STROOP, simple response time task |
| 2 h vs. 12 h | 60 g glucose increased working memory (speed of recognition (p < 0.05) and serial threes (p < 0.01)) after fasting. Reaction time impaired with 25 g glucose after fasting. |
Owen et al., 2013 [57] | N = 24 young adults aged 18–30 (M = 20) | Word list, serial threes and sevens, Corsi block-tapping |
| 12 h | Improved working memory and declarative memory after 25 g and 60 g glucose (ps < 0.05). Improved spatial working memory and word recognition after 25 g (ps < 0.05) and 60 g (ps < 0.01) glucose. No effect of glucose on accuracy. Participants with poor glycaemic control performed better on recall after 25 g glucose (p < 0.05). Improved response time after 25 g (p < 0.05) and 60 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. |
Parker and Benton, 1995 [93] | N = 100 females (M age = 20.15) | Dichotic listening task, word list, choice reaction time task, auditory word recognition |
| No | No effect of glucose on recognition Glucose drinkers recalled more when they nominated their right ear than left (p < 0.005), whereas placebo drinkers did not. Recall from attended (p < 0.013) or unattended (p < 0.034) right ear was better when BGL falling rather than rising. |
Peters et al., 2020a [30] | N = 32 adults (16 younger adults aged 21–30 (8 female, M age = 25.8, SD = 3.2) and 16 older adults aged 55–78 (8 female, M age = 68.6, SD = 6.5)) | Serial sevens, Virtual Morris Water Maze |
| 12 h | Older adults performed worse than younger in placebo condition (p = 0.02) but not glucose condition. Older adults had poorer glucose regulation (p = 0.002) and a greater response to glucose (p = 0.006) than younger adults. |
Riby et al., 2004 [94] | N = 20 older adults (M age = 68.75, SD = 6.0) | Verbal Paired Associates, digit symbol substitution, digit span |
| Unclear | Greater recall in glucose compared to placebo group (p < 0.05); immediate recall most sensitive to glucose (p < 0.01). |
Riby et al., 2008 [95] | N = 33 middle-aged adults (19 females) aged 35–55) | Word list, national adult reading test, digit symbol substitution, letter cancellation, TMT, digit span, category fluency |
| 2 h | 50 g glucose scored higher on word recall accuracy than 25 g or placebo (p < 0.001). Good glucose regulators performed better on trail making, except after 50 g glucose (p = 0.05). Consumption of “sugar, calories, sweets and drinks” were related to poor glucose control (p < 0.05). |
Riby et al., 2011 [96] | N = 56 adults (25 male) aged 17—80 (M = 34.4, SD = 17.0) | SART |
| 3 h | Increased speed of response after glucose (p < 0.05). No effect of glucose on accuracy or sustained attention. Good glucose regulators had quicker responses compared to poor regulators (p < 0.05). |
Scholey et al., 2001 [97] | N = 30 adults (11 male) aged 20–30 (M = 27.7) | Serial sevens, word retrieval, word list |
| 9 h | Glucose ingestion led to more responses in serial sevens (p < 0.05). BGL fell more during serial sevens (high cognitive load task) regardless of treatment (p = 0.009). No effect of glucose on verbal fluency or memory. |
Scholey et al., 2009 [98] | N = 120 adults (77 female; M age = 21.6, SD = 4.9) | Word recognition (auditory), tracking task |
| 11 h | Improved tracking after glucose ingestion (p = 0.045). No effect of glucose on recognition accuracy or reaction time. |
Scholey et al., 2014 [58] | N = 160 adults aged 18–55 | Arithmetic task, STROOP, memory search task, target tracker |
| 12 h | No difference between glucose groups and placebo. Improved scores due to caffeine + glucose. |
Serra-Grabulosa et al., 2010 [59] | N = 40 students (20 male) aged 18–25 (M = 19.6, SD = 1.7) | Continuous Performance Test—Identical Pairs (CPT-IP) |
| 8 h | No effect of treatment on performance. |
Smith and Foster, 2008 [99] | N = 32 adolescents (12 male) aged 14–17 (mean = 15.6, SD = 0.9) N = 10 in glucose second test condition | Modified CVLT-II |
| 9.5–10.5 h | No effect of glucose on recall. Treatment x treatment order effect—improved performance for glucose ingestion only when glucose ingested in second session after placebo trial. |
Smith et al., 2011b [100] | N = 58 adolescent males aged 14–17 (M = 15.5, SD = 1.0) | Modified CVLT-II |
| 9.5–11 h | More items recalled after glucose ingestion on 4th (p < 0.05) and 5th (p < 0.01) trial. Glucose only improved memory in those reporting higher trait anxiety (p < 0.05). |
Spiers et al., 1998 [101] | N = 48 adults (24 male) aged 18–35 | Word list, digit span, Corsi block test, TMT, Go-No-Go, Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), STROOP |
| No | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. |
Stollery and Christian, 2013 [102] | M = 93 adults (35 male) aged 18–35 (M = 20.7) | Word list recall, spatial location recognition, category verification |
| Unclear | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Stollery and Christian, 2015 [103] | N = 80 adults (26 male) aged 18–51 (M = 22.4) | PAL | T1:
| 9.5 h | T1: No effect of glucose on performance. T2: Glucose ingestion at retrieval led to improved retention (p = 0.016) and lower omission errors (p = 0.008). |
Stollery and Christian, 2016 [104] | N = 31 adults (9 male; glucose M age 22.5, SD = 1.5; placebo M age = 26.5, SD = 4.0) | Object-location binding task |
| 9.5 h | Improved location memory (p = 0.029) and object-location binding memory (p = 0.006) after glucose ingestion. No effect of glucose on object memory, retrieval time, or errors. Higher BGL associated with better location memory (p = 0.027) and binding memory (p = 0.012). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2001 [105] | N = 60 adults aged 18–28 (M age = 21) | CVLT, Rey–Osterrieth complex figure drawing modified digit Span |
| 9 h or 2 h | Superior performance of glucose on immediate recall interference list (p < 0.01), no effect of fasting condition. Short-delay free recall improved after glucose (p < 0.001) and after breakfast compared to lunch and 9 h fasted (ps < 0.001). Short-delay cued recall, long-delay free recall, and long-delay recognition improved after glucose (ps < 0.001); no effect of fasting condition. Long-delay cued recall improved after glucose (p < 0.001), and breakfast outperformed lunch (p < 0.05). Rey–Osterrieth performance superior after glucose (p < 0.005), no effect of condition. No effect of drink or condition on digit span. |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2002a [106] | N = 80 (18 male) adults aged 18–29 (M age = 20) | Modified CVLT, modified Rey–Osterrieth, complex figure drawing, modified serial sevens |
| 2 h | Superior performance of glucose on immediate recall interference list and long-delay recognition (p < 0.01). Aspartame improved long-delay cued recall in conditions with no interference (low cognitive load) (p < 0.05). Glucose facilitation effect in other short- and long-delay cued and free recall seen only in interference conditions (high cognitive load). Rey–Osterrieth performance superior after glucose (p < 0.05). Serial sevens performance superior after glucose (p < 0.005). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2002b [50] | N = 60 adults (26 male) aged 19–34 (M = 21) | Modified CVLT, serial sevens, ROCF (Delivered at baseline, 15 min, and 24 h after treatment) |
| 2 h | Improved performance on delayed free recall after glucose ingestion (p < 0.001). Improved 24 h delayed free recall (p < 0.001) and recognition (p = 0.007) after glucose ingestion. After 30 min, glucose performed better on delayed reproduction of figure (p = 0.03). No effect of glucose on immediate free recall or serial sevens performance. |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2008 [107] | N = 56 young adults aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Word list |
| 2 h | Increased recognition responses (recalling words associated with memories or experiences) following glucose ingestion (p = 0.04). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2011 [108] | N = 30 young adults (6 male) aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Serial threes and sevens, word list |
| 12 h | Improved spatial working memory (Corsi block task) after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.02). Improved immediate free recall after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.01). Improved recognition performance after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.05). No effect of glucose on numeric working memory (serial threes and sevens) or delayed free recall. Good glycaemic control associated with improved performance after 60 g glucose. Poor glycaemic control associated with improved performance after 15 g glucose. |
van der Zwaluw and et al., 2014 [61] | N = 43 older adults (16 male; M age = 77.7, SD = 5.6) | RAVLT, PAL, story recall, verbal fluency, digit span, STROOP, Test for Attentional Performance (TAP) |
| 10–12 h | Improved attention, working memory, and information processing after sucrose compared to placebo (p = 0.04). Improved tap flexibility. |
Walk et al., 2017 [60] | N = 113 children aged 9–10 | Erikson flanker task |
| 10 h | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. When glycaemic regulation was adjusted for, only Maltodextrin had improved reaction time (p = 0.044). |
Winder and Borrill, 1998 [109] | 104 adults (52 male) aged 18–55 (mean = 29.2, SD = 9.23) | Name–face association task, selective reminding task |
| No | No effect of glucose on performance. |
3.8. Blood Glucose Regulation
3.9. Meta-Analysis for Word List Recall Studies
3.10. Heterogeneity of Reported Outcomes and Study Bias
3.11. Sensitivity Analysis
3.12. Strengths and Limitations
4. Discussion
4.1. Observational Research
4.2. Experimental Research
4.3. Sugar Types
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Attuquayefio, T.; Stevenson, R.J.; Oaten, M.J.; Francis, H.M. A four-day Western-style dietary intervention causes reductions in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory and interoceptive sensitivity. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0172645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kendig, M.D.; Leigh, S.J.; Morris, M.J. Unravelling the impacts of western-style diets on brain, gut microbiota and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 128, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nguyen, J.C.D.; Killcross, A.S.; Jenkins, T.A. Obesity and cognitive decline: Role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oikonomou, E.; Psaltopoulou, T.; Georgiopoulos, G.; Siasos, G.; Kokkou, E.; Antonopoulos, A.; Vogiatzi, G.; Tsalamandris, S.; Gennimata, V.; Papanikolaou, A.; et al. Western Dietary Pattern Is Associated with Severe Coronary Artery Disease. Angiology 2018, 69, 339–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kopp, W. How Western Diet And Lifestyle Drive The Pandemic Of Obesity And Civilization Diseases. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Obes. 2019, 12, 2221–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dove, A.; Shang, Y.; Xu, W.; Grande, G.; Laukka, E.J.; Fratiglioni, L.; Marseglia, A. The impact of diabetes on cognitive impairment and its progression to dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2021, 17, 1769–1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Taboada, I.; González-Pardo, H.; Conejo, N.M. Western Diet: Implications for Brain Function and Behavior. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 564413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muhammad, T.; Meher, T. Association of late-life depression with cognitive impairment: Evidence from a cross-sectional study among older adults in India. BMC Geriatr. 2021, 21, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiNicolantonio, J.J.; Lucan, S.C.; O’Keefe, J.H. The Evidence for Saturated Fat and for Sugar Related to Coronary Heart Disease. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2016, 58, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillespie, K.M.; Kemps, E.; White, M.J.; Bartlett, S.E. The Impact of Free Sugar on Human Health-A Narrative Review. Nutrients 2023, 15, 889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiNicolantonio, J.J.; Bhutan, J.; O’Keefe, J.H. Added sugars drive chronic kidney disease and its consequences: A comprehensive review. J. Insul. Resist. 2016, 1, a3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beecher, K.; Alvarez Cooper, I.; Wang, J.; Walters, S.B.; Chehrehasa, F.; Bartlett, S.E.; Belmer, A. Long-Term Overconsumption of Sugar Starting at Adolescence Produces Persistent Hyperactivity and Neurocognitive Deficits in Adulthood. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 670430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yu, B.; He, H.; Zhang, Q.; Wu, H.; Du, H.; Liu, L.; Wang, C.; Shi, H.; Xia, Y.; Guo, X.; et al. Soft drink consumption is associated with depressive symptoms among adults in China. J. Affect Disord. 2015, 172, 422–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chong, C.P.; Shahar, S.; Haron, H.; Din, N.C. Habitual sugar intake and cognitive impairment among multi-ethnic Malaysian older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 2019, 14, 1331–1342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amoutzopoulos, B.; Steer, T.; Roberts, C.; Collins, D.; Page, P. Free and Added Sugar Consumption and Adherence to Guidelines: The UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey (2014/15–2015/16). Nutrients 2020, 12, 393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- World Health Organisation. Guideline: Sugars Intake for Adults and Children; World Health Organisation: Geneva, Switzerland, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, Nutrition, and Allergies (NDA). Scientific Opinion on Dietary Reference Values for carbohydrates and dietary fibre. EFSA J. 2010, 8, 1462. [Google Scholar]
- Watts, M.E.; Pocock, R.; Claudianos, C. Brain Energy and Oxygen Metabolism: Emerging Role in Normal Function and Disease. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2018, 11, 216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mergenthaler, P.; Lindauer, U.; Dienel, G.A.; Meisel, A. Sugar for the brain: The role of glucose in physiological and pathological brain function. Trends Neurosci. 2013, 36, 587–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCrimmon, R.J. Consequences of recurrent hypoglycaemia on brain function in diabetes. Diabetologia 2021, 64, 971–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Backeström, A.; Papadopoulos, K.; Eriksson, S.; Olsson, T.; Andersson, M.; Blennow, K.; Zetterberg, H.; Nyberg, L.; Rolandsson, O. Acute hyperglycaemia leads to altered frontal lobe brain activity and reduced working memory in type 2 diabetes. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cacciatore, M.; Grasso, E.A.; Tripodi, R.; Chiarelli, F. Impact of glucose metabolism on the developing brain. Front. Endocrinol. 2022, 13, 1047545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Balendra, V.; Esposto, J.C.; Reich, N. Bitter brain: Hypoglycemia and the pathology of neurodegeneration and dementia. Alzheimers. Dement. 2021, 17, e052401. [Google Scholar]
- Yu, J.H.; Kim, R.E.Y.; Park, S.Y.; Lee, D.Y.; Cho, H.J.; Kim, N.H.; Yoo, H.J.; Seo, J.A.; Kim, S.G.; Choi, K.M.; et al. Association of long-term hyperglycaemia and insulin resistance with brain atrophy and cognitive decline: A longitudinal cohort study. Diabetes Obes. Metab. 2023, 25, 1091–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hamelin, H.; Poizat, G.; Florian, C.; Kursa, M.B.; Pittaras, E.; Callebert, J.; Rampon, C.; Taouis, M.; Hamed, A.; Granon, S. Prolonged Consumption of Sweetened Beverages Lastingly Deteriorates Cognitive Functions and Reward Processing in Mice. Cereb. Cortex. 2021, 32, 1365–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beilharz, J.E.; Maniam, J.; Morris, M.J. Short-term exposure to a diet high in fat and sugar, or liquid sugar, selectively impairs hippocampal-dependent memory, with differential impacts on inflammation. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 306, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen, C.; Lu, Z.; Wang, X.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, D.; Li, S. Sugar-sweetened beverages consumption is associated with worse cognitive functions in older adults: From the national health and nutrition examination survey and food patterns equivalents database. Nutr. Neurosci. 2022, 26, 1011–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gomes Gonçalves, N.; Suemoto, C.K.; Ferreira, N.V. Different Sources of Sugar Consumption and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2011–2014. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2023, 78, 620–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, M.A.; Riby, L.M.; Eekelen, J.A.; Foster, J.K. Glucose enhancement of human memory: A comprehensive research review of the glucose memory facilitation effect. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 770–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, R.; White, D.J.; Cornwell, B.R.; Scholey, A. Functional Connectivity of the Anterior and Posterior Hippocampus: Differential Effects of Glucose in Younger and Older Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2020, 12, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaplan, R.J.; Greenwood, C.E.; Winocur, G.; Wolever, T.M. Cognitive performance is associated with glucose regulation in healthy elderly persons and can be enhanced with glucose and dietary carbohydrates. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 72, 825–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, E.K.; Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Wesnes, K.A. Acute ingestion of different macronutrients differentially enhances aspects of memory and attention in healthy young adults. Biol. Psychol. 2012, 89, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaplan, R.J.; Greenwood, C.E.; Winocur, G.; Wolever, T.M. Dietary protein, carbohydrate, and fat enhance memory performance in the healthy elderly. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2001, 74, 687–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- García, C.R.; Piernas, C.; Martínez-Rodríguez, A.; Hernández-Morante, J.J. Effect of glucose and sucrose on cognition in healthy humans: A systematic review and meta-analysis of interventional studies. Nutr. Rev. 2021, 79, 171–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sun, Q.; Yang, Y.; Wang, X.; Yang, R.; Li, X. The Association between Sugar-Sweetened Beverages and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged and Older People: A Meta-Analysis. J. Prev. Alzheimers Dis. 2022, 9, 323–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. PLoS Med. 2021, 18, e1003583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lester, M.L.; Thatcher, R.W.; Monroe-Lord, L. Refined carbohydrate intake, hair cadmium levels, and cognitive functioning in children. Nutr. Behav. 1982, 1, 3–13. [Google Scholar]
- Hassevoort, K.M.; Lin, A.S.; Khan, N.A.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J. Added sugar and dietary fiber consumption are associated with creativity in preadolescent children. Nutr. Neurosci. 2020, 23, 791–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baym, C.L.; Khan, N.A.; Monti, J.M.; Raine, L.B.; Drollette, E.S.; Moore, R.D.; Scudder, M.R.; Kramer, A.F.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J. Dietary lipids are differentially associated with hippocampal-dependent relational memory in prepubescent children. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 99, 1026–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gui, Z.; Huang, S.; Chen, Y.; Zhao, Y.; Jiang, N.; Zhang, S.; Chen, Y. Association between Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Consumption and Executive Function in Children. Nutrients 2021, 13, 4563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, F.; Yin, X.; Liu, Y.; Li, M.; Gui, X.; Bi, C. Association Between Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Consumption and Executive Function Among Chinese Tibetan Adolescents at High Altitude. Front. Nutr. 2022, 9, 939256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Naveed, S.; Venäläinen, T.; Eloranta, A.M.; Erkkilä, A.T.; Jalkanen, H.; Lindi, V.; Lakka, T.A.; Haapala, E.A. Associations of dietary carbohydrate and fatty acid intakes with cognition among children. Public Health Nutr. 2020, 23, 1657–1663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Al-Sabah, R.; Al-Taiar, A.; Rahman, A.; Shaban, L.; Al-Harbi, A.; Mojiminiyi, O. Season of birth and sugary beverages are predictors of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices Scores in adolescents. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 6145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Øverby, N.C.; Lüdemann, E.; Høigaard, R. Self-reported learning difficulties and dietary intake in Norwegian adolescents. Scand. J. Public Health 2013, 41, 754–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berger, P.K.; Plows, J.F.; Jones, R.B.; Alderete, T.L.; Rios, C.; Pickering, T.A.; Fields, D.A.; Bode, L.; Peterson, B.S.; Goran, M.I. Associations of maternal fructose and sugar-sweetened beverage and juice intake during lactation with infant neurodevelopmental outcomes at 24 months. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2020, 112, 1516–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cohen, J.F.W.; Rifas-Shiman, S.L.; Young, J.; Oken, E. Associations of Prenatal and Child Sugar Intake with Child Cognition. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2018, 54, 727–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ye, X.; Gao, X.; Scott, T.; Tucker, K.L. Habitual sugar intake and cognitive function among middle-aged and older Puerto Ricans without diabetes. Br. J. Nutr. 2011, 106, 1423–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maben, A.; Smith, A. Sugar, aspartame and mental performance: A preliminary investigation. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 1996, 11, 21–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Foster, J.K.; Durlach, P.; Perez, C. The effect of retrograde and anterograde glucose administration on memory performance in healthy young adults. Behav. Brain Res. 2002, 134, 505–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adan, A.; Serra-Grabulosa, J.M. Effects of caffeine and glucose, alone and combined, on cognitive performance. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2010, 25, 310–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foster, J.K.; Lidder, P.G.; Sünram, S.I. Glucose and memory: Fractionation of enhancement effects? Psychopharmacology 1998, 137, 259–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meikle, A.; Riby, L.M.; Stollery, B. The impact of glucose ingestion and gluco-regulatory control on cognitive performance: A comparison of younger and middle aged adults. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2004, 19, 523–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meikle, A.; Riby, L.M.; Stollery, B. Memory processing and the glucose facilitation effect: The effects of stimulus difficulty and memory load. Nutr. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 227–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owen, L.; Finnegan, Y.; Hu, H.; Scholey, A.B.; Sünram-Lea, S.I. Glucose effects on long-term memory performance: Duration and domain specificity. Psychopharmacology 2010, 211, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owen, L.; Scholey, A.B.; Finnegan, Y.; Hu, H.; Sünram-Lea, S.I. The effect of glucose dose and fasting interval on cognitive function: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, six-way crossover study. Psychopharmacology 2012, 220, 577–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owen, L.; Scholey, A.; Finnegan, Y.; Sünram-Lea, S.I. Response variability to glucose facilitation of cognitive enhancement. Br. J. Nutr. 2013, 110, 1873–1884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholey, A.; Savage, K.; O’Neill, B.V.; Owen, L.; Stough, C.; Priestley, C.; Wetherell, M. Effects of two doses of glucose and a caffeine-glucose combination on cognitive performance and mood during multi-tasking. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2014, 29, 434–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serra-Grabulosa, J.M.; Adan, A.; Falcón, C.; Bargalló, N. Glucose and caffeine effects on sustained attention: An exploratory fMRI study. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2010, 25, 543–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walk, A.M.; Raine, L.B.; Kramer, A.F.; Cohen, N.J.; Khan, N.A.; Hillman, C.H. Differential Effects of Carbohydrates on Behavioral and Neuroelectric Indices of Selective Attention in Preadolescent Children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Zwaluw, N.L.; van de Rest, O.; Kessels, R.P.; de Groot, L.C. Short-term effects of glucose and sucrose on cognitive performance and mood in elderly people. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2014, 36, 517–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, J.B.; Gross, A.M.; Aloia, M.S.; Billingsley, C. The effects of glucose on nonmemory cognitive functioning in the elderly. Neuropsychologia 1996, 34, 459–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Azari, N.P. Effects of glucose on memory processes in young adults. Psychopharmacology 1991, 105, 521–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benton, D.; Brett, V.; Brain, P.F. Glucose improves attention and reaction to frustration in children. Biol. Psychol. 1987, 24, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benton, D. The impact of increasing blood glucose on psychological functioning. Biol. Psychol. 1990, 30, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benton, D.; Owens, D.S. Blood glucose and human memory. Psychopharmacology 1993, 113, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benton, D.; Owens, D.S.; Parker, P.Y. Blood glucose influences memory and attention in young adults. Neuropsychologia 1994, 32, 595–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benton, D.; Stevens, M.K. The influence of a glucose containing drink on the behavior of children in school. Biol. Psychol. 2008, 78, 242–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Best, T.; Bryan, J.; Burns, N. An investigation of the effects of saccharides on the memory performance of middle-aged adults. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2008, 12, 657–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birnie, L.H.; Smallwood, J.; Reay, J.; Riby, L.M. Glucose and the wandering mind: Not paying attention or simply out of fuel? Psychopharmacology 2015, 232, 2903–2910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brandt, K.R.; Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Qualtrough, K. The effect of glucose administration on the emotional enhancement effect in recognition memory. Biol. Psychol. 2006, 73, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brandt, K.R.; Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Jenkinson, P.M.; Jones, E. The effects of glucose dose and dual-task performance on memory for emotional material. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 211, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brandt, K.R.; Gibson, E.L.; Rackie, J.M. Differential facilitative effects of glucose administration on Stroop task conditions. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 127, 932–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brandt, K.R. Effects of glucose administration on category exclusion recognition. J. Psychopharmacol. 2015, 29, 777–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brody, S.; Wolitzky, D.L. Lack of mood changes following sucrose loading. Psychosomatics 1983, 24, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, L.A.; Riby, L.M. Glucose enhancement of event-related potentials associated with episodic memory and attention. Food Funct. 2013, 4, 770–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craft, S.; Murphy, C.; Wemstrom, J. Glucose effects on complex memory and nonmemory tasks: The influence of age, sex, and glucoregulatory response. Psychobiology 1994, 22, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donohoe, R.T.; Benton, D. Cognitive functioning is susceptible to the level of blood glucose. Psychopharmacology 1999, 145, 378–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donohoe, R.T.; Benton, D. Declining Blood Glucose Levels after a Cognitively Demanding Task Predict Subsequent Memory. Nutr. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 413–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flint, R.W., Jr.; Turek, C. Glucose effects on a continuous performance test of attention in adults. Behav. Brain Res. 2003, 142, 217–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ford, C.E.; Scholey, A.B.; Ayre, G.; Wesnes, K. The effect of glucose administration and the emotional content of words on heart rate and memory. J. Psychopharmacol. 2002, 16, 241–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giles, G.E.; Avanzato, B.F.; Mora, B.; Jurdak, N.A.; Kanarek, R.B. Sugar intake and expectation effects on cognition and mood. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2018, 26, 302–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ginieis, R.; Franz, E.A.; Oey, I.; Peng, M. The “sweet” effect: Comparative assessments of dietary sugars on cognitive performance. Physiol. Behav. 2018, 184, 242–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gonder-Frederick, L.; Hall, J.L.; Vogt, J.; Cox, D.J.; Green, J.; Gold, P.E. Memory enhancement in elderly humans: Effects of glucose ingestion. Physiol. Behav. 1987, 41, 503–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hope, C.; Seiss, E.; Dean, P.J.; Williams, K.E.; Sterr, A. Consumption of glucose drinks slows sensorimotor processing: Double-blind placebo-controlled studies with the Eriksen flanker task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kennedy, D.O.; Scholey, A.B. Glucose administration, heart rate and cognitive performance: Effects of increasing mental effort. Psychopharmacology 2000, 149, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Macpherson, H.; Roberstson, B.; Sünram-Lea, S.; Stough, C.; Kennedy, D.; Scholey, A. Glucose administration and cognitive function: Differential effects of age and effort during a dual task paradigm in younger and older adults. Psychopharmacology 2015, 232, 1135–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mantantzis, K.; Maylor, E.A.; Schlaghecken, F. Gain without pain: Glucose promotes cognitive engagement and protects positive affect in older adults. Psychol. Aging 2018, 33, 789–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, P.Y.; Benton, D. The Influence of a Glucose Drink on a Demanding Working Memory Task. Physiol. Behav. 1999, 67, 69–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Messier, C.; Pierre, J.; Desrochers, A.; Gravel, M. Dose-dependent action of glucose on memory processes in women: Effect on serial position and recall priority. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 1998, 7, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, H.C.; Bourrasseau, C.; Blampain, J. Can you enhance executive control without glucose? The effects of fructose on problem solving. J. Psychopharmacol. 2013, 27, 645–650. [Google Scholar]
- Mohanty, A.; Flint, R.W. Differential effects of glucose on modulation of emotional and nonemotional spatial memory tasks. CABN 2001, 1, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parker, P.Y.; Benton, D. Blood glucose levels selectively influence memory for word lists dichotically presented to the right ear. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 843–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riby, L.M.; Meikle, A.; Glover, C. The effects of age, glucose ingestion and gluco-regulatory control on episodic memory. Age Ageing 2004, 33, 483–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riby, L.M.; McLaughlin, J.; Riby, D.M.; Graham, C. Lifestyle, glucose regulation and the cognitive effects of glucose load in middle-aged adults. Br. J. Nutr. 2008, 100, 1128–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riby, L.M.; Law, A.S.; McLaughlin, J.; Murray, J. Preliminary evidence that glucose ingestion facilitates prospective memory performance. Nutr. Res. 2011, 31, 370–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholey, A.B.; Harper, S.; Kennedy, D.O. Cognitive demand and blood glucose. Physiol. Behav. 2001, 73, 585–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholey, A.B.; Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Greer, J.; Elliott, J.; Kennedy, D.O. Glucose administration prior to a divided attention task improves tracking performance but not word recognition: Evidence against differential memory enhancement? Psychopharmacology 2009, 202, 549–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, M.A.; Foster, J.K. Glucoregulatory and order effects on verbal episodic memory in healthy adolescents after oral glucose administration. Biol. Psychol. 2008, 79, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, M.A.; Hii, H.L.; Foster, J.K.; van Eekelen, J.A. Glucose enhancement of memory is modulated by trait anxiety in healthy adolescent males. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 25, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spiers, P.A.; Sabounjian, L.; Reiner, A.; Myers, D.K.; Wurtman, J.; Schomer, D.L. Aspartame: Neuropsychologic and neurophysiologic evaluation of acute and chronic effects. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1998, 68, 531–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stollery, B.; Christian, L. Glucose and memory: The influence of drink, expectancy, and beliefs. Psychopharmacology 2013, 228, 685–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stollery, B.; Christian, L. Glucose, relational memory, and the hippocampus. Psychopharmacology 2015, 232, 2113–2125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stollery, B.; Christian, L. Glucose improves object-location binding in visual-spatial working memory. Psychopharmacology 2016, 233, 529–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Foster, J.K.; Durlach, P.; Perez, C. Glucose facilitation of cognitive performance in healthy young adults: Examination of the influence of fast-duration, time of day and pre-consumption plasma glucose levels. Psychopharmacology 2001, 157, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Foster, J.K.; Durlach, P.; Perez, C. Investigation into the significance of task difficulty and divided allocation of resources on the glucose memory facilitation effect. Psychopharmacology 2002, 160, 387–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Dewhurst, S.A.; Foster, J.K. The effect of glucose administration on the recollection and familiarity components of recognition memory. Biol. Psychol. 2008, 77, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sünram-Lea, S.I.; Owen, L.; Finnegan, Y.; Hu, H. Dose-response investigation into glucose facilitation of memory performance and mood in healthy young adults. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 25, 1076–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winder, R.; Borrill, J. Fuels for memory: The role of oxygen and glucose in memory enhancement. Psychopharmacology 1998, 136, 349–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, R.; White, D.; Cleeland, C.; Scholey, A. Fuel for Thought? A Systematic Review of Neuroimaging Studies into Glucose Enhancement of Cognitive Performance. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020, 30, 234–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, D.; Law, M.; Rücker, G.; Schwarzer, G. The Hartung-Knapp modification for random-effects meta-analysis: A useful refinement but are there any residual concerns? Stat. Med. 2017, 36, 3923–3934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindseth, G.N.; Coolahan, S.E.; Petros, T.V.; Lindseth, P.D. Neurobehavioral effects of aspartame consumption. Res. Nurs. Health 2014, 37, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Erbaş, O.; Erdoğan, M.A.; Khalilnezhad, A.; Solmaz, V.; Gürkan, F.T.; Yiğittürk, G.; Eroglu, H.A.; Taskiran, D. Evaluation of long-term effects of artificial sweeteners on rat brain: A biochemical, behavioral, and histological study. J. Biochem. Mol. Toxicol. 2018, 32, e22053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pase, M.P.; Himali, J.J.; Beiser, A.S.; Aparicio, H.J.; Satizabal, C.L.; Vasan, R.S.; Seshadri, S.; Jacques, P.F. Sugar- and Artificially Sweetened Beverages and the Risks of Incident Stroke and Dementia: A Prospective Cohort Study. Stroke 2017, 48, 1139–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsan, L.; Chometton, S.; Zuo, Y.; Sun, S.; Hayes, A.M.R.; Bridi, L.; Lan, R.; Fodor, A.A.; Noble, E.E.; Yang, X.; et al. Lasting effects of low-calorie sweeteners on glucose regulation, sugar intake, and memory. bioRxiv 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ediga, M.G.; Annapureddy, S.; Salikineedy, K.; Nimgampalle, M. Aspartame consumption causes cognitive impairment in streptozotocin-induced diabetic Wistar rats. Biologia 2023, 78, 2393–2407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cong, W.N.; Wang, R.; Cai, H.; Daimon, C.M.; Scheibye-Knudsen, M.; Bohr, V.A.; Turkin, R.; Wood, W.H., 3rd; Becker, K.G.; Moaddel, R.; et al. Long-term artificial sweetener acesulfame potassium treatment alters neurometabolic functions in C57BL/6J mice. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e70257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, S.K.; McCarthy, D.M.; Vied, C.; Stanwood, G.D.; Schatschneider, C.; Bhide, P.G. Transgenerational transmission of aspartame-induced anxiety and changes in glutamate-GABA signaling and gene expression in the amygdala. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2022, 119, e2213120119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomova, G.D.; Arnold, K.F.; Gilthorpe, M.S.; Tennant, P.W.G. Adjustment for energy intake in nutritional research: A causal inference perspective. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2022, 115, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Altermann Torre, V.; Machado, A.G.; de Sá Couto-Pereira, N.; Mar Arcego, D.; Dos Santos Vieira, A.; Salerno, P.S.V.; Dos Santos Garcia, E.; Lazzaretti, C.; Toniazzo, A.P.; Nedel, F.; et al. Consumption of a palatable diet rich in simple sugars during development impairs memory of different degrees of emotionality and changes hippocampal plasticity according to the age of the rats. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2020, 80, 354–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lemos, C.; Rial, D.; Gonçalves, F.Q.; Pires, J.; Silva, H.B.; Matheus, F.C.; da Silva, A.C.; Marques, J.M.; Rodrigues, R.J.; Jarak, I.; et al. High sucrose consumption induces memory impairment in rats associated with electrophysiological modifications but not with metabolic changes in the hippocampus. Neuroscience 2016, 315, 196–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuang, H.; Sun, M.; Lv, J.; Li, J.; Wu, C.; Chen, N.; Bo, L.; Wei, X.; Gu, X.; Liu, Z.; et al. Hippocampal apoptosis involved in learning deficits in the offspring exposed to maternal high sucrose diets. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2014, 25, 985–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- He, A.; Zhang, Y.; Yang, Y.; Li, L.; Feng, X.; Wei, B.; Zhu, D.; Liu, Y.; Wu, L.; Zhang, L.; et al. Prenatal high sucrose intake affected learning and memory of aged rat offspring with abnormal oxidative stress and NMDARs/Wnt signaling in the hippocampus. Brain Res. 2017, 1669, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yamazaki, M.; Yamada, H.; Munetsuna, E.; Ishikawa, H.; Mizuno, G.; Mukuda, T.; Mouri, A.; Nabeshima, T.; Saito, K.; Suzuki, K.; et al. Excess maternal fructose consumption impairs hippocampal function in offspring via epigenetic modification of BDNF promoter. FASEB J. 2018, 32, 2549–2562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, G.; Yu, Q.; Tan, B.; Ke, X.; Zhang, C.; Li, H.; Zhang, T.; Lu, Y. Gut dysbiosis impairs hippocampal plasticity and behaviors by remodeling serum metabolome. Gut. Microbes 2022, 14, 2104089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- An, R.; Liu, G.; Khan, N.; Yan, H.; Wang, Y. Dietary Habits and Cognitive Impairment Risk Among Oldest-Old Chinese. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2019, 74, 474–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Feng, T.; Feng, Z.; Jiang, L.; Yu, Q.; Liu, K. Associations of health behaviors, food preferences, and obesity patterns with the incidence of mild cognitive impairment in the middle-aged and elderly population: An 18-year cohort study. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 275, 180–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muñoz-García, M.I.; Martínez-González, M.A.; Martín-Moreno, J.M.; Razquin, C.; Cervantes, S.; Guillén-Grima, F.; Toledo, E. Sugar-sweetened and artificially-sweetened beverages and changes in cognitive function in the SUN project. Nutr. Neurosci. 2020, 23, 946–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pase, M.P.; Himali, J.J.; Jacques, P.F.; DeCarli, C.; Satizabal, C.L.; Aparicio, H.; Vasan, R.S.; Beiser, A.S.; Seshadri, S. Sugary beverage intake and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease in the community. Alzheimers Dement. 2017, 13, 955–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsai, Z.T.; Chen, C.L.; Chiou, H.; Chang, C.J.; Chen, C.Y.; Pei-Hsuan Wu, K.; Chung, C.Y.; Chen, P.H. Differential Longitudinal Effects of Frequent Sweetened Food Consumption at Different Exposure Ages on Child Cognitive, Language, and Motor Development. Biomed. J. 2023, 100608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, M.; Ke, P.; Wang, C.; Xia, W.; Meng, X.; Di, H.; Gan, Y.; He, Y.; Tian, Q.; Jiang, H.; et al. Association of food groups with the risk of cognitive impairment in Chinese older adults. J. Affect. Disord. 2022, 309, 266–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yan, X.; Xu, Y.; Huang, J.; Li, Y.; Li, Q.; Zheng, J.; Chen, Q.; Yang, W. Association of consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages with cognitive function among the adolescents aged 12–16 years in US, NHANES III, 1988–1994. Front. Nutr. 2022, 9, 939820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Author/s | Population | Cognitive Measures | Dietary Measures | Sugar Type | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Al-Sabah et al., 2020 a [44] | N = 1370 adolescents aged 11–16 | Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) | FFQ | Sugary drinks (frequency of consumption) | Inverse relationship between sugary drink consumption and cognitive performance (p < 0.001). Sugary drink consumption a main predictor of cognitive function (p < 0.001). |
Baym et al., 2014 a [40] | N = 52 children aged 7–9 | Memory tasks (paired associated images) | Youth–adolescent FFQ (YAQ) | Total sugar, added sugar (normalised by total daily kcal consumption) | Total and added sugars had no relationship to memory. Performance impacted negatively by saturated fatty acids and positively by omega-3 fatty acids (ps < 0.05). |
Berger et al., 2020 b [46] | N = 88 mother–infant pairs (mother M age = 68.1, SD = 6.7) | Bayley-III scales of infant development | FFQ at 1 and 6 months postnatal | Fructose, SSB (incl. juice), total sugar, added sugar (adjusted for kcal per day) | Maternal fructose (p < 0.01) and SSB (p = 0.02) consumption at 1 month postnatal negatively associated with infant cognitive development at 24 months. Infant cognition lower in infants of obese mothers (p ≤ 0.001). No effect of maternal intake at 6 months. |
Chong et al., 2019 a [14] | N = 1209 older adults aged 60 years and over (M = 68.1, SD = 5.6) | RAVLT, MMSE, MoCA, digit symbol (processing speed test, not described), VR | Dietary history questionnaire (1 week recall) | Fructose, glucose, total sugar, added sugar, SSB, sugar from cakes and deserts, adjusted for daily calorie intake) | Mild cognitive impairment had higher consumption of fructose (p = 0.004) and glucose (p = 0.032). MMSE scores lower in the higher percentile of total and free sugar intake (ps < 0.001). Risk of cognitive impairment increased 3.3-, 3.3-, and 3.6-fold in highest percentile of sucrose, total sugar, and free sugar, respectively (ps < 0.001). Risk of cognitive impairment increased by 3.7- and 1.8-fold for SSB and sugar from cakes and deserts, respectively (ps < 0.001). Reduction in risk of cognitive impairment by 35% in highest percentile of fruit consumption (p < 0.05). |
Cohen et al., 2018 b [47] | N = 1234, mother–child pairs (tested during pregnancy and early childhood) | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), WRAVMA, KBIT-II, WRAML | FFQ | Maternal (prenatal) SSB consumption, child SSB consumption (not adjusted for energy intake) | Maternal sucrose ingestion inversely associated with nonverbal KBIT-II (p = 0.03) and visual memory (p = 0.01) in mid-childhood. Maternal SSB ingestion inversely associated with nonverbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p = 0.03). Maternal diet soda consumption associated with lower WRAVMA in early childhood (p = 0.03) and verbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p < 0.001). Early childhood SSB consumption inversely associated with verbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p = 0.01). Fructose (p = 0.005) and fruit (p = 0.03) positively associated with PPVT-II in early childhood. |
Gui et al., 2021 a [41] | N = 6387 children (3410 male) aged 6–12 (M = 8.6, SD = 1.5) | BRIEF | FFQ | HFCS from SSB (frequency of consumption) | Associated with poor performance on executive function and high risk of executive dysfunction (ps < 0.0001). |
Hassevoort et al., 2020 a [39] | N = 54 children (31 female) aged 8–12 (M = 9.1, SD = 0.8) | TTCT—Verbal form A | 3-day FFQ | Added sugar (normalised to intake per 1000 kcal) | Inversely associated with fluency, originality, and overall TTCT score (p < 0.01). |
Lester et al., 1982 a [38] | N = 184 children (100 male) aged 5–16 | WISC-R, WIPPSI, WRAT | FFQ—24-h recall | Refined carbohydrates (adjusted for total calories) | Negative relationship with all aspects of cognition (full-scale IQ (p = 0.001), performance IQ (p = 0.025), verbal IQ (p = 0.005), math (p = 0.005), and reading (p = 0.025)) other than spelling. Ratio of refined carbohydrates to total food calories negatively correlated with full-scale IQ (p < 0.015). |
Naveed et al., 2020 a [43] | N = 487 children (250 male) aged 6–8 | Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM) | FFQ | Fructose, sucrose, glucose (adjusted for daily energy intake) | Increased fructose intake associated with higher fluid intelligence scores in all children (p = 0.002) and boys (p ≤ 0.001) but not girls alone; effect disappeared in all children and reduced in boys when fruits and berries were accounted for. No effect of glucose or sucrose |
Øverby et al., 2013 a [45] | N = 482 students (236 male, M age = 14.6) | Self-reported schooling difficulties (maths, reading, and writing) | FFQ | SSB, junk food (frequency of consumption) | Higher intake of SSB (p = 0.04) and junk food (p ≤ 0.001) associated with increased odds of self-reported math difficulties. Fruit intake associated with lower odds of math difficulties. No relationship with reading and writing difficulties. |
Ye et al., 2011 c [48] | N = 737 adults aged 45–75 | MMSE, word list learning, digit span, clock drawing and figure copying (visual–spatial), STROOP, verbal fluency test | FFQ (12-month estimate) | Total sugar, added sugar, SSB (adjusted for total energy intake) | Increased sucrose (p = 0.014), glucose (p = 0.032), SSB (p = 0.005), and added fructose (p = 0.028), but not natural fructose, associated with lower MMSE. Total sugars inversely correlated with letter fluency (p < 0.05), recognition and recall (ps < 0.05), memory (p = 0.01), and MMSE (p = 0.02). Added sugars inversely associated with letter fluency (p < 0.05), long-term recall (p < 0.05), and MMSE (p = 0.005). |
Zhang et al., 2022 a [42] | N = 1231 adolescents aged 13–18 (M = 15.77, SD = 1.7) | Modified Erikson flanker task, 1-back and 2-back tasks, more-odd shifting task | FFQ | SSB (frequency of consumption) | Drinking SSBs ≥2 times per week had worse performance for inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility than no SSBs (ps < 0.001). |
Cognitive Domain | Study Type | Enhanced N (%) | Impaired N (%) | Mixed (Enhanced and Impaired) N (%) | No Effect/Not Reported |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Global cognitive function | Cross-sectional | 0 (0.0) | 7 (77.8) | 0 (0.0) | 2 (22.2) |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 7 (100.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |
Memory | Intervention | 24 (42.1) | 2 (3.5) | 6 (10.5) | 25 (43.9) |
cross-sectional | 0 (0.0) | 2 (28.6) | 0 (0.0) | 5 (71.4) | |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 2 (33.3) | 0 (0.0) | 4 (66.7) | |
Coordination | Intervention | 4 (50.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 4 (50.0) |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 2 (100.0) | |
Attention | Intervention | 14 (29.2) | 4 (8.3) | 1 (2.1) | 29 (60.4) |
cross-sectional | 0 (0.0) | 1 (20.0) | 0 (0.0) | 4 (80.0) | |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 4 (100.0) | |
Perception | Intervention | 6 (30.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 14 (70.0) |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 6 (100.0) | |
Processing speed | Intervention | 15 (29.4) | 5 (9.8) | 1 (2.0) | 30 (58.8) |
cross-sectional | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 5 (100.0) | |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 4 (100.0) | |
Executive function | Intervention | 6 (25.0) | 0 (0.0) | 1 (4.2) | 17 (70.8) |
cross-sectional | 0 (0.0) | 3 (100.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | |
cohort | 0 (0.0) | 1 (25.0) | 0 (0.0) | 3 (75.0) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gillespie, K.M.; White, M.J.; Kemps, E.; Moore, H.; Dymond, A.; Bartlett, S.E. The Impact of Free and Added Sugars on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Nutrients 2024, 16, 75. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010075
Gillespie KM, White MJ, Kemps E, Moore H, Dymond A, Bartlett SE. The Impact of Free and Added Sugars on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Nutrients. 2024; 16(1):75. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010075
Chicago/Turabian StyleGillespie, Kerri M., Melanie J. White, Eva Kemps, Halim Moore, Alexander Dymond, and Selena E. Bartlett. 2024. "The Impact of Free and Added Sugars on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Nutrients 16, no. 1: 75. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010075
APA StyleGillespie, K. M., White, M. J., Kemps, E., Moore, H., Dymond, A., & Bartlett, S. E. (2024). The Impact of Free and Added Sugars on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Nutrients, 16(1), 75. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010075