Next Article in Journal
Methodology for Modernizing Local Gas-Fired District Heating Systems into a Central District Heating System Using Gas-Fired Cogeneration Engines—A Case Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Impact of Problem-Based Learning on the Perception, Understanding, and Application of Statistical Concepts in Business Administration and Management Students
Previous Article in Journal
Sustainable Practices and Performance of Resource-Based Companies: The Role of Internal Control
Previous Article in Special Issue
Investigating the Effects of Career Education Programs on High School Students’ Career Development Competencies in Korea
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Towards Sustainable Early Education Practices: A Quasi-Experimental Study on the Effects of Kindergarten Physical Education Programs on Fundamental Movement Skills and Self-Regulation in Haikou City, China

1
Faculty of Associated Medical Sciences, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen 40002, Thailand
2
Faculty of Physical Education, Hainan Normal University, Haikou 571158, China
3
BNOJHP Research Center, PT Division of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Associated Medical Sciences, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen 40002, Thailand
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2024, 16(4), 1400; https://doi.org/10.3390/su16041400
Submission received: 12 December 2023 / Revised: 26 January 2024 / Accepted: 5 February 2024 / Published: 7 February 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sustainable Development of Teaching Methods and Education System)

Abstract

:
Acquiring fundamental movement skills (FMS) in early childhood is linked to long-term engagement in physical activity, promoting lifelong health. Concurrently, the development of self-regulation contributes to fostering socially responsible and empathetic individuals. This study aims to contribute to the sustainable development goals of Good Health and Well-being (Goal 3) and Quality Education (Goal 4) by promoting early education practices that enhance children’s physical and socio-emotional development. This quasi-experimental study, employing a pre/post-test control design, examined the influence of different kindergarten physical education programs on FMS and self-regulation. Participants from a sports-themed kindergarten, ordinary public kindergarten, and non-profit private kindergarten were equally divided into three groups. The “Hello Sunshine” (HS) group followed a ball game-based program; the ordinary physical education (OPE) group’s curriculum had multi-themed physical activities; and the free-play (FP) group as a comparison group (free play) spent their activity time freely playing. Data from a total of 239 children were analyzed, all of whom received identical assessments of fundamental movement skills and self-regulation by the Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3) and the head–toes–knees–shoulders (HTKS) test at baseline and after 10 weeks of the PE curriculum. There was a significant effect of grouping on TGMD-3 composite scores after controlling for the pre-test score (F(2, 235) = 65.232, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.357). The composite score of the HS group was clearly higher than that of the OPE (95% CI:10.72~16.45) and FP (95% CI: 4.16~9.98) groups. A significant group time effect was observed for self-regulation (F(2, 236) = 4.588, p = 0.011, Partial η2 = 0.037). After 10 weeks, the HS group displayed a more significant increase in self-regulation (14.8%) than that in the OPE (9.7%) and FP (14.6%) groups. The ball game-based program exhibited more advantages in promoting fundamental movement skills and self-regulation among the kindergarteners. Overall, this study’s findings highlight the potential benefits of kindergarten physical education programs and underscore the importance of early childhood development, emphasizing its potential to contribute to holistic child development and align with sustainability goals.

1. Background

From the very beginning of life, early childhood care and education serve as the cornerstone for the sustained development, well-being, and health of children. Early childhood is a crucial stage in the development of children’s physical, motor, emotional, and social abilities [1]. Sustainable Development Goal 4 within Education 2030 emphasizes the need for universal access to quality early childhood development, care, and pre-primary education, aiming at fostering holistic children’s development, and ensuring that all children acquire the knowledge and skills needed to promote sustainable development [2]. However, most preschool children currently face at least three problems that pose a negative, long-term impact on their development: (1) physical inactivity [3], (2) poor fundamental movement skills (FMS) [4,5], and (3) inadequate preparation for school [6]. Meanwhile, childhood obesity and its associated health issues have garnered international attention. The World Health Organization (WHO) reported that an estimated 38.2 million children under five years of age were overweight or obese in 2019, especially in developing countries, and the prevalence rate is increasing rapidly [7]. In particular, the COVID-19 pandemic has seen forced closures of schools and parks, people isolated at home for weeks and months, reductions in physical activity, and increases in sedentary behavior, with studies reporting limitations in children’s physical fitness and motor skill development [8,9], and these changes may have even had long-lasting consequences [10,11]. Therefore, these problems urgently need to be improved in the post-pandemic era.
FMSs consist of basic movements that form the foundation for more complex and specialized physical activities and comprise three components: locomotor skills, object control skills, and stability skills [12,13]. One review synthesizes the relationship between FMS competency and eight potential benefits (i.e., global self-concept, perceived physical competence, and reduced sedentary behavior) in children and adults [14]. FMS at preschool age is a predictor of adolescent and adult physical activity (PA) [15,16]. However, the systematic review found that global levels of FMS in children only correspond to “below average” to “average” TGMD-2 normative data [17]. Therefore, the crucial implementation of fundamental movement skills (FMS) interventions in early education environments aligns with Sustainable Development Goal 4 (Quality Education) by ensuring that children develop holistically, acquiring the physical and cognitive skills necessary for a sustainable and prosperous future. Moreover, the cultivation of FMS in early childhood correlates with Sustainable Development Goal 3 (Good Health and Well-being), fostering a lasting dedication to physical activity and overall well-being [18].
The preschool years are critical to the development of children’s FMS, as supported by motor development theory [19]. However, certain children’s FMS, such as specific throwing or kicking techniques, are not learned naturally through free play and may require intentional practice and instruction [20]. Per Zeng’s [21] systematic review, of the 10 studies assessing the effects of PA on motor skills, 8 report significant improvements in FMS, and 1 observed mixed findings. Moreover, one failed to promote any beneficial outcomes.
Further, the development of motor and cognitive functions is deeply interconnected to infancy and early childhood. There is increasing evidence of a positive association between FMS, PA, and self-regulation [21,22,23]. Self-regulation is a multi-dimensional concept, which is a higher-order cognitive process that usually refers to the ability to control or direct one’s attention, thoughts, emotions, and actions [24]. Effective self-regulation, including but not limited to concentrating on tasks, relying on the working memory function to extract established behavioral norms, utilizing inhibitory control to restrain instinctual reactions, and accessing existing behavioral norms to take final actions, is highly relevant to positive classroom behavior and academic achievement [25,26,27]. In the Head Start Early Learning Outcomes Framework, self-regulation is acknowledged as a pivotal component in the early development of children [28]. Furthermore, in the context of sustainability, individuals with refined self-regulation skills are well positioned to engage in socially responsible behaviors, contributing to the development of a sustainable and conscientious global community [29]. Studies find that self-regulation ability develops rapidly in early childhood and is affected by many ecological factors, such as individual development and traits, parenting, and early childhood education [30,31,32]. Many studies focus on designing classroom or academically relevant interventions for socioeconomically disadvantaged children, aiming to improve their behavioral problems and enhance school readiness, such as the Head Start REDI program [33], Chicago School Readiness Project [34], and Tools [35], but there is limited research on intervening self-regulation through exercise.
However, evidence indicates that PA contributes to improved cognitive abilities [36]. van der Fels et al. find a specific association between physical activity and certain aspects of cognitive performance and suggest complex motor intervention programs to promote both motor and higher-order cognitive skills in pre-adolescent children [37]. This provides a basis for using PA interventions to promote changes in self-regulation. A recent study by Robinson et al. finds that the Children’s Health Activity Motor Program (CHAMP) can enhance self-regulation [22]. Miller et al. show that CHAMP significantly improved behavioral self-regulation and had no effect on cognitive self-regulation [38]. In another study, games and exercise improved partial self-regulation [39], and other relevant studies using comprehensive exercise interventions report mixed results [40,41,42,43]. These inconsistent results may be related to differences in study design and the self-regulation measurement. Our study utilized the head–toes–knees–shoulders (HTKS) task to test behavior self-regulation, which has been demonstrated to have high reliability and ecological validity. The task evaluates inhibitory control and the ability to follow specific instructions and inhibit impulsive responses. It requires children to listen to instructions and respond with large muscle movements, as it reflects control and directive behavior in the classroom [27].
In China, the vast majority of children spend more than eight hours each day in daycare, where kindergarten PA policy, outdoor activity time, and teacher PA strategy influence their PA [44,45]. Kindergartens can provide a wide range of public health benefits by serving as a key intervention environment for PA. For example, providing organized and structured teaching activities to promote fitness and skill development in children, outdoor activities, including engaging in activities and playing in outdoor environments, and nature-based education, offer benefits such as enhanced physical health, cognitive development, and emotional well-being [46]. However, challenges like safety concerns, weather dependency, and resource constraints must be addressed to ensure successful and sustainable implementation, fostering a holistic learning environment for children. In recent years, the Chinese government has proposed many policies and initiatives aimed at improving physical education (PE) for young children. However, no comparative study has been conducted on the effects of mainstream PE curricula in Chinese kindergartens.
This study aimed to compare the effects of two common groups of PE programs with a control group (free-play activities) in Chinese kindergartens, including a ball-themed program, regular PE courses, and free play, on children’s FMS and self-regulation. We hypothesized that the FMS scores of both two groups of preschoolers have significantly improved and that the HS group would have the greatest improvement. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the two groups of children would significantly improve self-regulation, especially in the HS group.

2. Materials and Methods

This quasi-experimental study was conducted in three kindergartens (sports-themed public kindergarten, ordinary public kindergarten, and non-profit private kindergarten) in the center of Haikou City, China. The trial was approved by the Khon Kaen University Ethics Committee for Human Research (HE642100), registered with the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (registration number: ChiCTR2000035414) on 8 October 2020, and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.1. Participants

This study’s sample size was determined using WinPepi software (Version 11.65) based on Westendorp’s research [47], which indicated that 83 participants in each kindergarten would be sufficient for a significance level of 5%, a power level of 80%, and a confidence level of 95%, inflated to compensate for a 5% drop-out rate to test the primary outcome (e.g., FMS). A total of 249 children (5.48 ± 0.59 years old; 46% girls; no ethnic minorities) were enrolled in this study. Each kindergarten class was considered an experimental unit, with children from two classes each of grades K2 and K3 selected. Only healthy and typically developing children, aged 4–6 years, whose primary caregivers provided informed consent, were eligible to participate. Children who (1) had communication difficulties or unexplained health problems, (2) had contraindications to exercise, such as congenital heart disease or asthma, and (3) had additional training from a sports club were excluded.

2.2. Procedures

The kindergarten students were recruited at the beginning of the school term (March 2022). The teacher placed an informed consent form in the students’ backpacks for parents to consider. Researchers and teachers screened the children who returned the consent form. Baseline data, including height, weight, FMS, and self-regulation, of the enrolled children were collected in the first week. The FMS test was conducted in a sports field, while the self-regulation test was held in a separate classroom. With the cooperation of the teachers and based on the time and space arrangements of each kindergarten, all data were collected within two weeks. If a child was absent on the day of the test, data were collected on another day to minimize missing data. Each child received a sticker or snack as a reward after completing the test. After 10 weeks of PE curricula, the post-test data were collected. The study procedure is shown in Figure 1.

2.3. Intervention

Kindergartens operate on a five-day school week. Children in the sports-themed kindergarten received the ball game-based program “Hello Sunshine” (HS group). The ordinary public kindergarten followed a theme-based program for daily life (OPE group), while the non-profit private kindergarten allowed children free play during activity time (FP group). Apart from the PE classes and outdoor activities, the daily activities in each kindergarten remained the same. In the HS and OPE groups, PE classes in grade K2 were held once a week, lasting 25 min each session. In grade K3, PE classes were held twice a week, with each session lasting 30 min. Outdoor activities were scheduled for 60 min, five times a week. The FP group had one hour of outdoor activity every day. All three groups participated in a 10-week PE intervention. The PE program for the HS and OPE groups totaled 54 h in K2 and 60 h in K3, while the FP group’s PE totaled 50 h. Additional details are described below.

2.3.1. HS Group

‘Hello Sunshine’ is an upgraded version of this kindergarten ball-based characteristic curriculum, which is based on the 3–6 Years Age Early Learning and Development Guideline of our country [48] and the comprehensive Seefeldt’s motor development theory [49]. It has been meticulously designed by a team of PE teachers led by the principal, taking into consideration the age and psychological characteristics of young children. The implementation of children’s ball activities is approached from the perspective of the children, adhering to the principles of playfulness and appropriateness. The ball game-based program incorporated an array of balls like football, basketball, badminton, ping-pong, table tennis, baseball, golf, handball, and bowling. The program focused on developing various ball skills, including kicking, dribbling, hitting, et al. Considering program design, Newell’s constraints model [50] served as the theoretical foundation. First, the ecological task analysis method was applied to consider two or three critical individual constraints related to the motor tasks. Second, environmental or task constraints that could be manipulated were identified to control the difficulty of the motor tasks. Finally, the range of task difficulty was determined based on the developmental level of the children’s motor skills [51]. For example, when learning the badminton forehand stroke, hand-eye coordination and strength are individual constraints. Adjustable task constraints include hitting a stationary ball (by tying and suspending the ball with a string) and hitting a moving ball (self-tossed). The height and speed of hitting represent another task constraint, which is related to strength. Environmental constraint adjustment involves using a short-handled plastic badminton racket, replacing feathers with balloons, and using storytelling to stimulate children’s enthusiasm for hitting. Finally, teachers adjust the difficulty level based on the individual abilities of the children. The structure of each class includes a 5 min warm-up period, a 20–25 min exercise period, and a 5 min cool-down activity. There was a high degree of autonomy during outdoor time. In addition to self-selected ball skill activities once (K2) or twice (K3) each week, participants could choose their area of exercise, such as the equipment zone or bike zone, during the remainder of outdoor time (Table 1).

2.3.2. OPE Group

PE classes were taught by a PE teacher who referred to a specified kindergarten textbook for various monthly themes, including knowledge and activities related to health and PA. In these 10 weeks, themes included “Transportation Exhibition” and “My Friends” in grade K2 and “Change” and “I love …” in K3. Basic skills, such as walking, running, rolling, crawling, jumping, and throwing, were taught through the themes. Outdoor time was the same as that of the HS group, but children were allowed to freely play in different areas.

2.3.3. FP Group

Children are given the autonomy to choose playmates and activities based on their personal interests, with an emphasis on child-led activities. The playgrounds, which are equipped with swings, slides, sandboxes, and open spaces, serve as venues for physical activities. Despite lacking a PE background, teachers focus on supervision to ensure a safe environment. They refrain from actively guiding or directing children’s play unless safety concerns arise.

2.4. Outcome Measures

2.4.1. Demographic and Anthropometric Data

The demographic information of the children (such as date of birth and sex) was provided by the teachers, and their height and weight were measured using an electronic scale (height and weight tester, HW-900B, Zhengzhou, China) on the morning of the assessment conducted by the research assistant.

2.4.2. FMS

The Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3), which is an effective tool for measuring the gross motor skills of children aged 3–10 years, was used for FMS assessment [52,53]. The TGMD-3 has demonstrated good internal consistency reliability, test–retest reliability, and construct validity [52,54]. TGMD-3 is divided into two subscales: locomotor skills (run, gallop, hop, leap, jump, and slide) and ball skills (two-hand strike, one-hand strike, dribble, kick, catch, overhand throw, and underhand throw). The TGMD-3 was conducted with groups of 4–5 participants, with each group session lasting approximately 30 min. A trained administrator demonstrated each skill’s correct movements. Then, each participant performed one practice trial followed by two formal tests. An experienced scorer reviewed the recorded videos of these tests. According to the testing criteria, each skill was assessed on the basis of three to five performance criteria. If a child demonstrated the correct performance criteria, the child received a score of one for each criterion; otherwise, a score of zero was given. Raw scores were calculated by adding the scores of the two formal tests.

2.4.3. Self-Regulation

The head–toe–knees–shoulders (HTKS) test evaluates children’s behavioral self-regulation, has strong inter-rater reliability [25,26], and is a good assessment tool for behavioral self-regulation in 4–8-year-old children regardless of their cultural background [24]. The children were individually tested by professional psychologists and their assistants in separate rooms within the kindergarten. The test is divided into two tasks. The first is the head-to-toe task, in which the child was asked to respond in the opposite way to the examiner’s two verbal instructions. The second task involved mixed knee and shoulder commands, in which the child should also respond in the opposite manner to the commands [55]. The HTKS test consists of 26 irregular commands, and the child received two points for a correct response, zero points for an incorrect response, and one point if an incorrect response was immediately corrected. The maximum score is 52 [56]. This test has been used in research on Chinese preschool children [57].

2.5. Data Analysis

Statistical analysis data were analyzed using SPSS (version 25.0; IBM, Armonk, NJ, USA). The normality of the data was verified using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and further descriptive statistics were performed. Intra-group comparisons were conducted using paired t-tests or Wilcoxon paired-sample tests depending on the distribution of the data. The baselines were used as a co-variable and a covariance analysis was performed in comparison to the TGMD-3 results. The 3 (HS, OPE, and FP) × 2 (baseline and after 10 weeks) mixed-model ANOVA comparison was used in the comparison of the HTKS results. The effect size was expressed as partial η2 (ŋp2) and was defined as small (0.01), medium (0.06), and large (0.14).

3. Results

Ten preschool children had missing or outlier data for one or more outcome variables either at baseline or after 10 weeks, resulting in their exclusion from the analysis. The final sample consisted of 239 kindergarten children (mean age: 5.49 ± 0.60 years; 54% boys). Detailed demographic and anthropometric information are provided in Table 2.

3.1. FMS

When comparing the baseline TGMD-3 scores of the three groups, we found significant differences in both the locomotor (F(2, 236) = 8.651, p < 0.001) and composite scores (F(2, 236) = 4.115, p = 0.018). However, ball skill scores were similar across the groups (F(2, 236) = 2.735, p = 0.067).
After 10 weeks, the TGMD-3 scores in all three groups improved compared to those in the pre-test. When the baseline was controlled, the independent variable (grouping) showed a significant effect; the difference in the TGMD-3 subset and composite score between the groups is presented in Table 3. The results of the pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 4.

3.2. Self-Regulation

The Wilcoxon paired-sample test revealed that the FP group significantly increased their Part 1 (Z = −6.953, p < 0.001), Part 2 (Z =−6.213, p < 0.001), and total (Z = −7.617, p < 0.001) scores. The OPE group showed a similar upward trend in their Part 1 (Z = −6.227, p < 0.001), Part 2 (Z = −3.594, p < 0.001), and total (Z = −7.055, p < 0.001) scores. The HS group also showed significantly better Part 1 (Z = −3.757, p < 0.001), Part 2 (Z = −6.254, p < 0.001), and total (Z = −6.748, p < 0.001) scores. There was no significant difference in HTKS scores among the three groups at baseline (F(2, 236) = 1.357, p = 0.259 > 0.05). The mixed-model ANOVA was used to assess the effect of different groupings and time. While the main effect of school was not significant (F(2, 236) = 1.652, p = 0.194), the interaction between time and school was (p = 0.011) (Table 5).

4. Discussion

This quasi-experimental study explored the effects of two different kinds of PE programs with comparison (free play) on children’s motor skills and self-regulation development. All three groups demonstrated significant improvements in the FMS and self-regulation tests compared to baseline levels. In the FMS sub-test, especially for ball skills, the HS group performed better than the OPE and PE groups and exhibited a large effect size. Both the HS and FP groups revealed more significant progress in their HTKS test results than the OPE group. Overall, our results indicate that regular PA, particularly ball skill-based activity, contributes to the development of motor skills in kindergarteners and promotes the development of self-regulation abilities.

4.1. Comparison of FMS Changes in the Three Groups

Gao et al. [58] have concluded that interventions during the preschool stage are crucial for the development of FMS. They observed significant improvements in FMS following an intervention lasting only eight weeks. In terms of the impact of PE intervention on FMS, the observed results were in accordance with several previous studies, which established that structured PA intervention not only increases children’s PA level. For example, Palmer et al.’s study compared the physical activity participation of preschool children in two different physical activity opportunities: unstructured outdoor play and structured movement classes. The study found that structured movement time provides children with more opportunities to engage in physical activity compared to outdoor time alone [59]. Similarly, Tortella et al. found that, compared to the free-play group and the control group, the partially structured activity group showed significant improvements in specific tests measuring motor skills [60]. Furthermore, they discovered that this group effectively improved and maintained their motor skills better than free play [61,62].
Experts agree that FMS requires guided teaching and learning, followed by practice and reinforcement before they can be significantly improved [13,63]. Results from a meta-analysis study [63] indicate that FMS significantly improved pre- to post intervention, but the control group’s (i.e., free play) improvement was not significant. The HS program envisages learning and repeatedly practicing various basic ball skills such as dribbling, kicking, holding, and tapping the ball. These skills provide structured and guided instruction, allowing preschoolers to learn and refine their motor skills in a targeted manner. By engaging in ball-related activities, they can improve their coordination, balance, and manipulative skills, which are essential components of FMS. Our results are consistent with the study of Robinson et al. [20], which used ball skills intervention findings to suggest that providing a high-quality motor skill program in early childhood settings could potentially be a sustainable public health approach for promoting FMS and positive physical developmental trajectories for health [20]. Conversely, free play is characterized by unstructured and child-initiated activities, allowing preschoolers to explore and experiment with a wide range of movements and play scenarios. While free play may not provide explicit instruction on specific skills, it offers opportunities for spontaneous and self-directed practice of FMS. Children engage in imaginative play, create their own rules, and adapt their movements to different situations, which can contribute to the development and refinement of FMS. On the whole, our results are supported by previous studies both in theory and practice.

4.2. Comparison of HTKS Changes in the Three Groups

The results showed that PE intervention improved self-regulation in kindergartners. Among the three groups, the HS and FP groups showed a greater increase in self-regulation scores. Our findings are supported by previous related studies. As an acute intervention during classroom movement breaks, Ureña et al. [64] conducted a 15 min exercise program with different levels of difficulty for 49 children aged 4–5 years. Regardless of difficulty, the program showed a certain intervention effect on self-regulation. Using a series of music and movement games can also improve self-regulation [65,66]. Duman et al. [67] adopted embedded learning-based movement education for 5-year-old children, whose activities—group activities, cooperative games, and social interaction—required gross motor, fine motor, and visuomotor coordination skills, and they also found that the experiment group improved self-regulation. Korll et al. [68] observe a strong interaction among the program, environment, daily activities, and development of self-regulation, suggesting the need to include play in the kindergarten curriculum, in line with our ball game-based curriculum design.
Several possible mechanisms accounted for FMS, and self-regulation improved in our study. From a neuroscientific perspective, shared cerebellar and/or prefrontal processes might be involved in the relationship between motor function and cognition [47]. Research suggests that various exercises affect different neurocognitive networks [69,70]; the cerebellum is important for complex and coordinated movements, and the prefrontal cortex is critical for higher-order cognitive functioning [71]. Engaging in more complex movement patterns requires deeper information processing, leading to more consistent neural plasticity changes [72]. Furthermore, the development timeline of motor skills and executive function is similar, with accelerated development between the ages of 5 and 10.
It is important to note that the specific impact of these programs may vary depending on the design, duration, intensity, and quality of the interventions, as well as individual differences among children. Nonetheless, incorporating ball skill-based programs, theme-based PE, and free-play content into early childhood settings can offer valuable opportunities for FMS development and the cultivation of self-regulation skills in preschoolers.
The strengths of this study were two fold. First, it examined the groups of subjects, whose PA patterns represented the main forms of PE in existing Chinese kindergartens. Our results provide a theoretical basis and practical reference for the effectiveness of PE for young children, which has received scant attention in the current research. Second, our study covers the developmental evaluation of motor and cognitive domains, which highlights the important fields of children’s development.
A potential weakness of this study was that it considered only the effects of program intervention, without considering the possible effects of environmental factors such as kindergarten teachers, facilities, and playgrounds on the outcome measures. The exclusion of these environmental factors could limit the comprehensive understanding of the observed outcomes, as they play crucial roles in shaping the overall educational experience for children in a kindergarten setting. Future research should consider exploring these environmental variables to provide a more holistic assessment of the intervention’s effectiveness. Another limitation was the use of a subjective measurement instrument, including the assessment with TGMD-3, and the monitoring of physical activity intensity during interventions, which may be influenced by individual differences among observers.

5. Conclusions

This quasi-experimental study reveals that a ball game-based PE program is more effective in enhancing Fundamental Motor Skills (FMS) and self-regulation in kindergarten children compared to free play. The significance lies in emphasizing the potential benefits of structured PE interventions for early childhood development. Future research needs to explore the influence of teachers, facilities, and playgrounds on outcomes. Additionally, examining motor skill performance in diverse settings and establishing a comprehensive framework for acquisition can offer nuanced insights. These studies are essential to inform intervention guidelines and to promote sustainable development of children’s motor skills and related areas.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.W. and W.E.; Data Curation, H.R. and Y.Y.; Investigation, X.D.; Methodology, H.W. and Y.Y.; Resources, W.C.; Writing—Original Draft, H.W.; Writing—Review and Editing, W.E. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Fundamental Fund of Khon Kaen University and has received funding support from the National Science, Research and Innovation Fund (NSRF), Project No. SD-65-023, and the Hainan Province Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project (HNSK(YB)23-57).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by Khon Kaen University Ethics Committee for Human Research (HE642100, 9 September 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Libertus, K.; Hauf, P. Motor Skills and Their Foundational Role for Perceptual, Social, and Cognitive Development; Frontiers Media SA: Lausanne, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  2. UNESCO Education 2030. Incheon Declaration and Framework for Action for the Implementation of Sustainable Development Goal 4. Available online: https://bit.ly/38nV17d (accessed on 8 January 2024).
  3. Pereira, J.R.; Cliff, D.P.; Sousa-Sá, E.; Zhang, Z.; Santos, R. Prevalence of Objectively Measured Sedentary Behavior in Early Years: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2019, 29, 308–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Foulkes, J.D.; Knowles, Z.; Fairclough, S.J.; Stratton, G.; O’Dwyer, M.; Ridgers, N.D.; Foweather, L. Fundamental Movement Skills of Preschool Children in Northwest England. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2015, 121, 260–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Xia, X.; Chao, L.; Nan, C.; Yin, X.; Zheng, H.; Zhang, S. Fundamental Motor Skills of Kindergarten Children in Different Environments and Ethnic Groups in Northwest China. BMC Pediatr. 2022, 22, 423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. James, M.E.; Bedard, C.; Bremer, E.; Cairney, J. The Acceptability and Feasibility of a Preschool Intervention Targeting Motor, Social, and Emotional Development. Front. Pediatr. 2020, 8, 319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. WHO. Obesity and Overweight. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight (accessed on 20 July 2023).
  8. Carballo-Fazanes, A.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Silva, R.; Lopes, V.P.; Abelairas-Gómez, C. The Developmental Trajectory of Motor Competence of Children That Lived the Covid-19 Confinement Period: A Four-Year Follow-up Study in Portuguese Children. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2022, 7, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Abe, T.; Kitayuguchi, J.; Fukushima, N.; Kamada, M.; Okada, S.; Ueta, K.; Tanaka, C.; Mutoh, Y. Fundamental Movement Skills in Preschoolers before and during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Japan: A Serial Cross-Sectional Study. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2022, 27, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. López-Bueno, R.; López-Sánchez, G.F.; Casajús, J.A.; Calatayud, J.; Tully, M.A.; Smith, L. Potential Health-Related Behaviors for Pre-School and School-Aged Children during COVID-19 Lockdown: A Narrative Review. Prev. Med. 2021, 143, 106349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pietrobelli, A.; Pecoraro, L.; Ferruzzi, A.; Heo, M.; Faith, M.; Zoller, T.; Antoniazzi, F.; Piacentini, G.; Fearnbach, S.N.; Heymsfield, S.B. Effects of Covid-19 Lockdown on Lifestyle Behaviors in Children with Obesity Living in Verona, Italy: A Longitudinal Study. Obesity 2020, 28, 1382–1385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Goodway, J.D.; Ozmun, J.C.; Gallahue, D.L. Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults; Jones & Bartlett Learning: New York, NY, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-1-284-17494-6. [Google Scholar]
  13. Barnett, L.M.; Stodden, D.; Cohen, K.E.; Smith, J.J.; Lubans, D.R.; Lenoir, M.; Iivonen, S.; Miller, A.D.; Laukkanen, A.; Dudley, D.; et al. Fundamental Movement Skills: An Important Focus. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2016, 35, 219–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lubans, D.R.; Morgan, P.J.; Cliff, D.P.; Barnett, L.M.; Okely, A.D. Fundamental Movement Skills in Children and Adolescents. Sports Med. 2010, 40, 1019–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Williams, H.; Pfeiffer, K.; O’Neill, J.; Dowda, M.; Mciver, K.; Brown, W.; Pate, R. Motor Skill Performance and Physical Activity in Preschool Children. Obesity 2008, 16, 1421–1426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Telama, R.; Yang, X.; Leskinen, E.; Kankaanpää, A.; Hirvensalo, M.; Tammelin, T.; Viikari, J.; Raitakari, O. Tracking of Physical Activity from Early Childhood through Youth into Adulthood. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2013, 46, 955–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Bolger, L.E.; Bolger, L.A.; O’Neill, C.; Coughlan, E.; O’Brien, W.; Lacey, S.; Burns, C.; Bardid, F. Global Levels of Fundamental Motor Skills in Children: A Systematic Review. J. Sports Sci. 2021, 39, 717–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. The Sustainable Development Goals Report 2023: Special Edition; The Sustainable Development Goals Report; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2023; ISBN 978-92-1-002491-4. [Google Scholar]
  19. Seefeldt, V. Developmental Motor Patterns. Implications for Elementary School Physical Education. In Psychology of Motor Behavior and Sport; Nadeau, C.H., Halliwell, W.R., Newell, K.M., Roberts, G.C., Eds.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1980; pp. 314–323. ISBN 978-0-931250-16-3. [Google Scholar]
  20. Robinson, L.E.; Veldman, S.L.C.; Palmer, K.K.; Okely, A.D. A Ball Skills Intervention in Preschoolers: The CHAMP Randomized Controlled Trial. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2017, 49, 2234–2239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Zeng, N.; Ayyub, M.; Sun, H.; Wen, X.; Xiang, P.; Gao, Z. Effects of Physical Activity on Motor Skills and Cognitive Development in Early Childhood: A Systematic Review. BioMed Res. Int. 2017, 2017, 2760716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Robinson, L.E.; Palmer, K.K.; Bub, K.L. Effect of the Children’s Health Activity Motor Program on Motor Skills and Self-Regulation in Head Start Preschoolers: An Efficacy Trial. Front. Public. Health 2016, 4, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Ma, R.; Lin, M.; Song, H.; Pang, J.; Ma, D.; Zhang, L.; Wang, X. Correlation Between Motor Skills and Cognitive Self-Regulation Ability in 4–6 Year-Old Children. China Sport. Sci. 2019, 39, 60–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. McClelland, M.M.; Cameron, C.E. Self-Regulation in Early Childhood: Improving Conceptual Clarity and Developing Ecologically Valid Measures. Child. Dev. Perspect. 2012, 6, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Saracho, O.; Spodek, B. Contemporary Perspectives on Social Learning in Early Childhood Education; IAP: Mumbai, India, 2007; ISBN 978-1-60752-732-9. [Google Scholar]
  26. McClelland, M.M.; Cameron, C.E.; Connor, C.M.; Farris, C.L.; Jewkes, A.M.; Morrison, F.J. Links between Behavioral Regulation and Preschoolers’ Literacy, Vocabulary, and Math Skills. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 947–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. McClelland, M.M.; Gonzales, C.R.; Cameron, C.E.; Geldhof, G.J.; Bowles, R.P.; Nancarrow, A.F.; Merculief, A.; Tracy, A. The Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Revised: Links to Academic Outcomes and Measures of EF in Young Children. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 721846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Rosanbalm, K.D.; Murray, D.W. Promoting Self-Regulation in the First Five Years: A Practice Brief; Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, US. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA, 2017.
  29. Nielsen, K.S. The Role of Self-Regulation in Environmental Behavior Change. Ph.D. Thesis, Copenhagen Business School, Frederiksberg, Denmark, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  30. Huang, R.; Geng, Z.; Siraj, I. Exploring the Associations among Chinese Kindergartners between Academic Achievement and Behavioral, Cognitive and Emotional Self-Regulation. Early Educ. Dev. 2023, 34, 591–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wanless, S.B.; McClelland, M.M.; Tominey, S.L.; Acock, A.C. The Influence of Demographic Risk Factors on Children’s Behavioral Regulation in Prekindergarten and Kindergarten. Early Educ. Dev. 2011, 22, 461–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. School Readiness and Self-Regulation: A Developmental Psychobiological Approach. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 711–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bierman, K.L.; Nix, R.L.; Greenberg, M.T.; Blair, C.; Domitrovich, C.E. Executive Functions and School Readiness Intervention: Impact, Moderation, and Mediation in the Head Start REDI Program. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008, 20, 821–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Raver, C.C.; Jones, S.M.; Li-Grining, C.; Zhai, F.; Metzger, M.W.; Solomon, B. Targeting Children’s Behavior Problems in Preschool Classrooms: A Cluster-Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 77, 302–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. Closing the Achievement Gap through Modification of Neurocognitive and Neuroendocrine Function: Results from a Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial of an Innovative Approach to the Education of Children in Kindergarten. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e112393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Gunnell, K.E.; Poitras, V.J.; LeBlanc, A.; Schibli, K.; Barbeau, K.; Hedayati, N.; Ponitfex, M.B.; Goldfield, G.S.; Dunlap, C.; Lehan, E.; et al. Physical Activity and Brain Structure, Brain Function, and Cognition in Children and Youth: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 105–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. van der Fels, I.M.J.; Te Wierike, S.C.M.; Hartman, E.; Elferink-Gemser, M.T.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. The Relationship between Motor Skills and Cognitive Skills in 4-16 Year Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2015, 18, 697–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Miller, A.L.; Palmer, K.K.; Wang, L.; Wang, C.; Riley, H.O.; McClelland, M.M.; Robinson, L.E. Mastery-Oriented Motor Competence Intervention Improves Behavioral but Not Cognitive Self-Regulation in Head Start Preschoolers: Randomized Controlled Trial Results. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2023, 33, 725–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Healey, D.M.; Halperin, J.M. Enhancing Neurobehavioral Gains with the Aid of Games and Exercise (ENGAGE): Initial Open Trial of a Novel Early Intervention Fostering the Development of Preschoolers’ Self-Regulation. Child. Neuropsychol. 2015, 21, 465–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Francis, L.A.; Nix, R.L.; BeLue, R.; Keller, K.L.; Kugler, K.C.; Rollins, B.Y.; Savage, J.S. Designing a Childhood Obesity Preventive Intervention Using the Multiphase Optimization Strategy: The Healthy Bodies Project. Clin. Trials 2023, 20, 434–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rhee, K.E.; Kessl, S.; Manzano, M.A.; Strong, D.R.; Boutelle, K.N. Cluster Randomized Control Trial Promoting Child Self-Regulation around Energy-Dense Food. Appetite 2019, 133, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ray, C.; Figuereido, R.; Vepsäläinen, H.; Lehto, R.; Pajulahti, R.; Skaffari, E.; Sainio, T.; Hiltunen, P.; Lehto, E.; Korkalo, L.; et al. Effects of the Preschool-Based Family-Involving DAGIS Intervention Program on Children’s Energy Balance-Related Behaviors and Self-Regulation Skills: A Clustered Randomized Controlled Trial. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Mavilidi, M.F.; Pesce, C.; Mazzoli, E.; Bennett, S.; Paas, F.; Okely, A.D.; Howard, S.J. Effects of Cognitively Engaging Physical Activity on Preschool Children’s Cognitive Outcomes. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport. 2022, 94, 839–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Brown, W.H.; Googe, H.S.; McIver, K.L.; Rathel, J.M. Effects of Teacher-Encouraged Physical Activity on Preschool Playgrounds. J. Early Interv. 2009, 31, 126–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Chen, C.; Ahlqvist, V.H.; Henriksson, P.; Magnusson, C.; Berglind, D. Preschool Environment and Preschool Teacher’s Physical Activity and Their Association with Children’s Activity Levels at Preschool. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Becker, C.; Lauterbach, G.; Spengler, S.; Dettweiler, U.; Mess, F. Effects of Regular Classes in Outdoor Education Settings: A Systematic Review on Students’ Learning, Social and Health Dimensions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Westendorp, M.; Houwen, S.; Hartman, E.; Mombarg, R.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. Effect of a Ball Skill Intervention on Children’s Ball Skills and Cognitive Functions. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2014, 46, 414–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Chinese Ministry of Education. 3–6 Years Age Early Learning and Development Guideline. Available online: https://www.unicef.cn/reports/3-6-children-learn-and-development-guide (accessed on 14 June 2020).
  49. Seefeldt, V.; Haubenstricker, J. Patterns, Phases, or Stages: An Analytical Model for the Study of Developmental Movement. In The Development of Movement Control and Coordination; Kelso, J.A.S., Clark, J.E., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: New York, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 309–318. ISBN 978-0-471-10048-5. [Google Scholar]
  50. Newell, K.M. Constraints on the Development of Coordination. In Motor Development in Children: Aspects of Coordination and Control; Wade, M.G., Whiting, H.T.A., Eds.; Springer: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1986; ISBN 978-90-247-3389-7. [Google Scholar]
  51. Balan, C.M.; Davis, W.E. Ecological Task Analysis—An Approach to Teaching Physical Education. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Danc. 1993, 64, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dale, A. Ulrich The Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3): Administration, Scoring, & International Norms. Spor Bilim. Derg. 2013, 24, 27–33. [Google Scholar]
  53. Brian, A.; Taunton, S.; Lieberman, L.J.; Haibach-Beach, P.; Foley, J.; Santarossa, S. Psychometric Properties of the Test of Gross Motor Development-3 for Children With Visual Impairments. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 2018, 35, 145–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Li, X.; Wang, X.; Ulrich, D.A.; Xu, Q.; He, Y.; Guo, Q. Reliability and validity of TGMD-3 in fundamental move skills test for children aged 3-12 years China. J. Wuhan. Inst. Phys. Educ. 2022, 56, 86–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ponitz, C.C.; McClelland, M.M.; Matthews, J.S.; Morrison, F.J. A Structured Observation of Behavioral Self-Regulation and Its Contribution to Kindergarten Outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 605–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Meador, D.; Turner, K.A.; Lipsey, M.W.; Farran, D.C. Administering Measures from the PRI Learning-Related Cognitive Self-Regulation Study; Vanderbilt University, Peabody Research Institute: Nashville, TN, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ma, R.; Lin, M.; Song, H.; Pang, J.; Ma, D.; Zhang, L.; Wang, X. Effects of Movement Skill Development on Behavioral Self-regulation Ability of Preschool Children. China Sport. Sci. 2019, 39, 40–47. [Google Scholar]
  58. Gao, Z.; Wen, X.; Fu, Y.; Lee, J.E.; Zeng, N. Motor Skill Competence Matters in Promoting Physical Activity and Health. BioMed Res. Int. 2021, 2021, e9786368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Palmer, K.K.; Matsuyama, A.L.; Robinson, L.E. Impact of Structured Movement Time on Preschoolers’ Physical Activity Engagement. Early Child. Educ. J. 2017, 45, 201–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Tortella, P.; Haga, M.; Ingebrigtsen, J.E.; Fumagalli, G.F.; Sigmundsson, H. Comparing Free Play and Partly Structured Play in 4-5-Years-Old Children in an Outdoor Playground. Front. Public. Health 2019, 7, 197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Tortella, P.; Haga, M.; Loras, H.; Sigmundsson, H.; Fumagalli, G. Motor Skill Development in Italian Pre-School Children Induced by Structured Activities in a Specific Playground. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0160244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Palmer, K.K.; Chinn, K.M.; Robinson, L.E. The Effect of the CHAMP Intervention on Fundamental Motor Skills and Outdoor Physical Activity in Preschoolers. J. Sport. Health Sci. 2019, 8, 98–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Logan, S.W.; Robinson, L.E.; Wilson, A.E.; Lucas, W.A. Getting the Fundamentals of Movement: A Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Motor Skill Interventions in Children. Child. Care Health Dev. 2012, 38, 305–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ureña, N.; Fernández, N.; Cárdenas, D.; Madinabeitia, I.; Alarcón, F. Acute Effect of Cognitive Compromise during Physical Exercise on Self-Regulation in Early Childhood Education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Tominey, S.L.; McClelland, M.M. Red Light, Purple Light: Findings From a Randomized Trial Using Circle Time Games to Improve Behavioral Self-Regulation in Preschool. Early Educ. Dev. 2011, 22, 489–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. McClelland, M.M.; Tominey, S.L.; Schmitt, S.A.; Hatfield, B.E.; Purpura, D.J.; Gonzales, C.R.; Tracy, A.N. Red Light, Purple Light! Results of an Intervention to Promote School Readiness for Children From Low-Income Backgrounds. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ozkur, F.; Duman, G. Analyzing the Embedded Learning-Based Movement Education Program’s Effects on Preschool Children’s Visual-Motor Coordination and Self-Regulation. J. Educ. Learn. 2019, 8, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kroll, L.R. Early Childhood Curriculum Development: The Role of Play in Building Self-Regulatory Capacity in Young Children. Early Child. Dev. Care 2017, 187, 854–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Bullitt, E.; Rahman, F.N.; Smith, J.K.; Kim, E.; Zeng, D.; Katz, L.M.; Marks, B.L. The Effect of Exercise on the Cerebral Vasculature of Healthy Aged Subjects as Visualized by MR Angiography. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2009, 30, 1857–1863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Godde, B.; Staudinger, U.M. Cardiovascular and Coordination Training Differentially Improve Cognitive Performance and Neural Processing in Older Adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Diamond, A. Close Interrelation of Motor Development and Cognitive Development and of the Cerebellum and Prefrontal Cortex. Child. Dev. 2000, 71, 44–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Vazou, S.; Pesce, C.; Lakes, K.; Smiley-Oyen, A. More than One Road Leads to Rome: A Narrative Review and Meta-Analysis of Physical Activity Intervention Effects on Cognition in Youth. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2019, 17, 153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study procedure.
Figure 1. Study procedure.
Sustainability 16 01400 g001
Table 1. Example of weekly PE program content.
Table 1. Example of weekly PE program content.
IntroductionPreparationMain Part of the LessonGameFinal Part of the LessonOutdoor Activity
HS Group
K2Animal fun runningGeneral preparatory exercises with footballsExplore dribbling and ball controlRelay play obstacle runBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected ball skills with PE teacher
K3 (1st)Sliding in various directionsFancy dribbling with basketballsBasketballs are tossed and caught in pairsCatch basketballs for different tasksBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected ball skills with PE teacher
K3 (2nd)Running through obstaclesSimple toss and catch with badmintonForehand stroke, fixed ball with short-handled plastic badminton racketsForehand stroke with specific objectiveBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected zone
OPE Group
K2Imitating animalsClimbing and rolling exercisesPractice climbing and rollingTire-pushing raceBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected zone
K3 (1st)Running in a circlePractice rope swingingJumping ropeExplore ways to play with ropeBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected zone
K3 (2nd)Running in a circleBody balance exercisesPractice the combo jungle gymRelay play with crawlingBody tapping and stretching with musicSelf-selected zone
FP Group
Participants used various playground facilities (such as slides and sandboxes) to play freely
Table 2. Demographic characteristics.
Table 2. Demographic characteristics.
HS Group (n = 79)OPE Group
(n = 80)
FP Group
(n = 80)
Total
(n = 239)
Sex
Boy40 (50.63%)43 (53.75%)47 (58.75%)130 (54.39%)
Girl39 (49.37%)37 (46.25%)33 (41.25%)109 (45.61%)
Age5.38 ± 0.525.48 ± 0.675.59 ± 0.585.49 ± 0.60
Height112.9 ± 6.46117.68 ± 8.28 *112.64 ± 5.42114.41 ± 7.18
Weight19.94 ± 3.7920.59 ± 4.0719.63 ± 2.9220.05 ± 3.63
BMI (kg/m2)15.65 ± 2.2915.39 ± 2.0115.49 ± 2.2215.51 ± 2.17
* Note: p < 0.05 (Significant Difference).
Table 3. TGMD-3 scores among the groups, baseline adjusted using ANCOVA.
Table 3. TGMD-3 scores among the groups, baseline adjusted using ANCOVA.
VariableGroup
(Intervention)
BaselineAfter Intervention
(Adjusted)
p-ValueEffect Size
MeanSDMeanSD
Locomotor skills scoreHS22.994.9828.084.800.001 *0.058
OPE20.515.7227.384.92
FP23.714.5625.294.83
Ball skills scoreHS14.164.7128.605.19<0.001 **0.424
OPE13.304.6521.875.10
FP12.444.6517.865.19
TGMD composite scoreHS37.157.4756.5697.54<0.001 **0.357
OPE33.817.7849.5027.57
FP36.157.4242.9867.50
Note: * p < 0.05 (Significant Difference), ** p < 0.001 (Significant Difference). TGMD-3: Test of Gross Motor Development-3; HS: Hello Sunshine; OPE: ordinary physical education; FP: Free Play.
Table 4. Pairwise comparison of TGMD-3 scores among the groups.
Table 4. Pairwise comparison of TGMD-3 scores among the groups.
GroupLocomotor Skills ScoreBall Skills ScoreTGMD-3 Composite Score
pDifference
(95% CI)
pDifference
(95% CI)
pDifference
(95% CI)
HSFP0.001 **0.97~4.65<0.001 **8.74~12.72<0.001 **10.72~16.45
OPE1−1.18~2.57<0.001 **4.75~8.69<0.001 **4.16~9.98
OPEFP0.023 *0.22~4.00<0.001 **2.05~5.97<0.001 **3.64~9.39
HS1−2.57~1.18<0.001 **−8.69~−4.75<0.001 **−9.98~−4.16
FPOPE0.023 *−4.00~−0.22<0.001 **−5.97~−2.05<0.001 **−9.39~−3.64
HS0.001 **−4.65~−0.97<0.001 **−12.72~−8.74<0.001 **−16.45~−10.72
Note: * p < 0.05 (Significant Difference), ** p < 0.001 (Significant Difference). TGMD-3: Test of Gross Motor Development-3; HS: Hello Sunshine; OPE: ordinary physical education; FP: Free Play.
Table 5. Comparison of HTKS scores among the groups.
Table 5. Comparison of HTKS scores among the groups.
Variable A Group-by-Time
Interaction Effect Size
HTKS
Score
Pre-Test
Mean ± SD
Post-Test
Mean ± SD
% Δp-Value within Group between TimeFpPartial η2
HS34.73 ± 8.6139.86 ± 6.7414.8<0.001 **4.5880.011 **0.037
OPE34.15 ± 8.5937.46 ± 7.829.7<0.001 **
FP32.31 ± 11.5437.03 ± 11.6914.6<0.001 **
Note: ** p < 0.001 (Significant Difference). HTKS: Head–Toes–Knees–Shoulders; HS: Hello Sunshine; OPE: ordinary physical education; FP: Free Play.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wu, H.; Eungpinichpong, W.; Ruan, H.; Chen, W.; Yang, Y.; Dong, X. Towards Sustainable Early Education Practices: A Quasi-Experimental Study on the Effects of Kindergarten Physical Education Programs on Fundamental Movement Skills and Self-Regulation in Haikou City, China. Sustainability 2024, 16, 1400. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16041400

AMA Style

Wu H, Eungpinichpong W, Ruan H, Chen W, Yang Y, Dong X. Towards Sustainable Early Education Practices: A Quasi-Experimental Study on the Effects of Kindergarten Physical Education Programs on Fundamental Movement Skills and Self-Regulation in Haikou City, China. Sustainability. 2024; 16(4):1400. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16041400

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wu, Hua, Wichai Eungpinichpong, Hui Ruan, Wenke Chen, Yibei Yang, and Xiujuan Dong. 2024. "Towards Sustainable Early Education Practices: A Quasi-Experimental Study on the Effects of Kindergarten Physical Education Programs on Fundamental Movement Skills and Self-Regulation in Haikou City, China" Sustainability 16, no. 4: 1400. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16041400

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop