1. Introduction
The main objective of this paper is to demonstrate that the inferential statistics method and the Systems Dynamics method can provide similar results when studying the short-term evolution of individual happiness because of a single drug dose. To achieve this, an application case, where drugs are alcohol and caffeine, is analyzed through a single-case experimental design with replication with two participants.
There is a scientific debate about whether happiness is a trait or a state (see [
1] for a review). However, some authors go beyond this controversy by offering an inclusive proposal about the happiness nature: it is both a trait and a state [
2]. We adopt this inclusive approach in this study.
If happiness has a state nature as well as a trait one, then we must be able to study its short-term and long-term dynamics, even because of a single eliciting stimulus in a single session, as a drug intake is. For instance, it has been demonstrated that, after a single dose intake of alcohol or caffeine, both stimuli can increase happiness and other feelings such as euphoria in the short term [
3,
4,
5,
6,
7,
8].
The relationship between alcohol and happiness depends on doses and lifestyle. In fact, moderate drinkers tend to be happier than abstainers and heavy drinkers [
9,
10], and also tend to reduce their depression and stress [
11]. In addition, happiness and team cohesion are significant predictors of alcohol consumption in university sports people [
12]. In a longitudinal study with a Russian sample, a hump-shaped relationship among men but not among women was obtained [
13]. In addition, there exists a nonlinear relationship between alcohol and happiness showing that abstainers and occasional drinkers, as well as heavy and problem drinkers, are at risk of reaching high anxiety and depression levels [
14].
On the other hand, the existence of individual differences inside the acute effects of both drugs (alcohol and caffeine) has been proved [
15,
16,
17], as well as the existence of an interaction between personality, time of the day and caffeine, in such a way that in the morning of the first day of the experiment, low impulsive persons were hindered and high impulsive ones were helped by caffeine. This pattern reverses in the evening of this first day, and it reverses again in the evening of the second day [
18]. Furthermore, previous findings of studies using a large single dose, such as increased alertness and anxiety, improved performance on simple and choice reactive tasks, or a cognitive vigilance, may be applicable to normal patterns of the effects of caffeine consumption [
19].
Long-term dynamical effects of alcohol consumption have also been studied [
20]: the result of a meta-analysis show that the positive effect intensity of alcohol consumption decreases over time. In fact, the study [
20] proposes the construction of a mathematical model to study accurately this dynamical response. However, even the corresponding short-term dynamics have not been well described yet. Therefore, a mathematical dynamical model, the stimulus response model (SRM), is here presented to fill this gap. The SRM is capable of predicting and describing how the whole personality (the General Factor of Personality or GFP) changes during a single session in response to a single dose of caffeine or alcohol, and also how its responses vary between individuals [
21,
22]. However, it has not been applied to the study of happiness dynamics yet.
Then, this study applies the SRM to predict the evolution of a subject’s happiness in response to a single dose of alcohol or caffeine. In fact, the advantages of the SRM are as follows: (a) it is capable of reproducing happiness dynamically as a consequence of a single dose of a stimulus such as alcohol or caffeine; (b) it points out how much the obtained happiness response is due to the stimulus; and (c) it provides the changes in the happiness trait at short term. Therefore, it is possible with the SRM to go beyond the results of the inferential statistics methods traditionally used by the behavioral sciences, as demonstrated here.
In addition, the SRM is presented within the System Dynamics approach, proposed by Jay W. Forrester about the half twentieth century [
23]. The System Dynamics approach includes, between other features, a diagram like the hydrodynamic ones as a help to analyze, understand and program the model equations that can be written as a system of coupled differential or finite difference ones (see
Section 4 for details). In fact, the “hydrodynamic diagram” that System Dynamics uses is much more intuitive for a behavioral researcher than an abstract set of coupled differential equations (see
Section 5 for the comparison with other approaches frequently used in behavioral sciences). Nevertheless, and only for pure mathematicians’ interest, the SRM can be described as a time function equation for the stimulus plus an integrodifferential equation for happiness.
Moreover, it is here demonstrated that the SRM reproduces accurately the happiness’ measures of the experimental design below presented. In this experimental design happiness is measured with two instruments: the Euphoria Scale (ES) and the Smiley Face Scale (SFS). Both scales are shown in this work to be a very good instrument to measure happiness in both trait and state aspects.
On the other hand, and as announced above, both inferential statistics and the SRM are used here to integrate the mathematical tools currently used in humanity sciences (inferential statistics) and other ones frequently used in natural sciences (dynamical systems). In addition, to set the significance of the individual differences in the alcohol/caffeine dynamical response, both the SRM outcomes and SRM parameters are obtained with uncertainty, i.e., with their corresponding confidence intervals and confidence levels. Therefore, the here called
overall power parameter (
opp), defined as a combination of four SRM parameters, provides those differences with the possibility to determine whether they are significant or not. The conclusion of the above-mentioned integration is that the analysis of the
opp confidence intervals leads to the same conclusions about those differences as the classical inferential statistics (see
Section 5 for details).
About the contents of the next sections,
Section 2 is devoted to the experimental design methodology.
Section 3 presents the results of the inferential statistics application to the experimental scores.
Section 4 presents the SRM by using the methodology of Systems Dynamics, with the hydrodynamic diagram as the central qualitative tool to help understanding. In
Section 5, the SRM is used to reproduce the dynamics present in the scores obtained in
Section 4 by the experimental design presented in
Section 2, as well as its main predictions.
Section 5 presents the integration of the results from inferential statistics and those from the SRM.
Section 6 introduces the conclusions of this paper, stressing the advantages of the System Dynamics approach over other methods used in behavioral sciences such as structural equations or time series analysis. Annex 1 ending this paper is a practical support to better understand the construction of a dynamical model such as the SRM.
3. Inferential Statistics Results
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of the results of the experiment, mean and standard deviation, corresponding to the two participants in all the three previously mentioned phases. Participant 1 (P1) score is lower in the base line (BL), for alcohol and caffeine in both ES and SFS than Participant 2 (P2). Moreover, the statistical dispersion measure in all conditions and scales is higher in P1 than in P2.
Two-way repeated measures ANOVA were performed to determine whether there are significant differences in mean scores in ES between the three conditions (BL, alcohol and caffeine) for each participant. See
Table 2. We have used these parametric statistical tests for the score on the ES because the score on that scale is the sum of the scores of the 4 items considered in the Likert scale for measuring EUPHORIA (cheerful, elated, exhilarated, and lively), assuming that it is an approximately continuous variable. Although Likert scales are technically ordinal because they consist of a series of ordered categories, several authors have found consistent support for using them as approximately continuous. Then, ordinal variables with five or more categories can often be used as continuous without any harm to the analysis performed on them. This fact may result in several categories much higher than the ordinal Likert scales they are calculated from. Therefore, the fact of taking the sum or mean of two or more ordinal variables to create an approximately continuous variable is something that many researchers encounter when using surveys [
27,
28,
29,
30].
For the SFS analyses, as it is an ordinal single-item scale, we used the Friedman test for repeated measures and the Wilcoxon test of related samples to compare the three conditions (BL, alcohol and caffeine). The results are shown in the text (last paragraph of
Section 3).
The total number of registers was 19 over the course of 3 h. A factor (experimental condition) was considered with three levels, corresponding to the three phases of the experiment: A = baseline; B = alcohol; C = caffeine, for the analysis of variance of these repeated measures. The 19 registers from the two scales in each phase of the experiment were considered as the dependent variable. Thus, the 19 registers from Phase A were compared with those of Phase B and Phase C, with 19 registers each one as well.
There exist significant effects of both alcohol and caffeine in ES for both participants. For example, for P1, there exist a significant effect of both drugs in ES (F(2, 36) = 27.771, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.607, β = 1.000). In general, a medium effect size has been obtained for Cohen’s value, but the power is maximum (1) in both participants. The numbers in parentheses are the degrees of freedom. In the case of F, the first number (2) corresponds to the degrees of freedom of the model and the second number (36) to the degrees of freedom of the residuals.
Pairwise multiple comparisons were performed with the Bonferroni adjust for mean differences and
t-test between all conditions and for both participants. See
Table 3.
In general, the effects of both drugs are higher for the ES scores than for the BL ones, but the effect of alcohol is higher in P1 than in P2 on ES, and the contrary occurs for the caffeine effect.
For example, in P1 alcohol increases significantly the Euphoria (ES), where the pre-scores (
Table 1) (BL, M = 3.84, SD = 1.068) were lower than the post-scores (M = 13.63, SD = 6.457) t(32) = −6.886,
p < 0.001, CI95% (−2.252, −0.889), β = 1.000, while regarding caffeine, the pre-scores were also lower than the post-scores (M = 10.16, SD = 5.069) t(32) = −5.712,
p < 0.001, CI95% (−1.919, −0.682), β = 1.000, but, as we can see with CI 95%, there is no significant difference between both conditions when we compare the effects of caffeine and alcohol.
As for SFS, we applied a Friedman test, as mentioned above. For P1, the result is significant (χ2 = 7.01, p < 0.05), as it is the case for P2 (χ2 = 7.01, p < 0.05). But the Wilcoxon paired samples test only shows significant results for the effect of alcohol on BL in P1 (Z = −2949, p < 0.05), and for the effect of caffeine on BL in P2 (Z = −2.941, p < 0.05). Thus, the results from the ES and SFS are similar for both participants.
4. The Stimulus Response Model (SRM)
Following Forrester’s methodology [
23] for Systems Dynamics, the model to be built (the SRM) can be represented qualitatively by the hydrodynamic diagram (HD) that is showed in
Figure 1. It summarizes the factors and patterns of the problem dynamics studied in the specialized literature or obtained from the scholars’ knowledge about the problem. In the case here studied, the problem dynamics is given by the individual happiness response to a stimulus (drug), whose knowledge can be taken from [
17,
21,
22].
However, before presenting the HD and deducing the equations from it, the variables considered, their symbols and their dimensions and units are presented in
Table 4 for the input variables or parameters, and in
Table 5 for the output variables. Note that the input variables or parameters are those not influenced causally for any other model variables, and, in general, can depend on time. In the model presented, they are constant, and their values are calibrated from real data with a genetic algorithm, i.e., an informatics program that finds the set of parameter values that provides the SRM dynamical prediction closest to the experimental scores, with their respective confidence intervals and corresponding confidence levels. The last two rows of
Table 4 represent (for ES and for SFS) the initial happiness value and the initial drug in blood value, respectively. These values are not computed by the genetic algorithm, because the first one is the score of the participant before consumption (see below:
y0 in Equation (12) or Equation (18)), and the second one is the amount of drug in blood before consumption, being always
s0 = 0 because neither alcohol nor caffeine have been consumed since a long time ago for both participants.
The parameter that measures the global effect of the drug on an individual by the SRM is defined as a combination of the three power parameters and the inhibitor effect delay of
Table 4, and it is here called as the
overall power parameter:
. Note that the three terms of
have the same dimensions (T
−1) and units (min
−1).
In addition, the output variables of
Table 5 depend causally on other model variables through the corresponding equations: every model output variable corresponds to a particular model equation deduced from the HD and the hypotheses stated about its variables.
Following Forrester’s methodology [
23] for Systems Dynamics, the model to be built (the SRM) can be represented qualitatively by the hydrodynamic diagram (HD), as shown in
Figure 1. It summarizes the factors and patterns of the problem dynamics studied in the specialized literature or obtained from the scholars’ knowledge about the problem. In the case here studied, the problem dynamics is given by the individual happiness response to a stimulus (drug), whose knowledge can be taken from [
17,
21,
22].
Let us deduce the model equations from this diagram. Take into account for this deduction the variable dimensions and units presented in
Table 4 and
Table 5. First, to compute the drug in blood,
, and the drug in digestive organs,
, the following balance equations provided by the HD must be considered in finite differences form (
is a finite time increment):
However, Equations (1) and (2) can be written in the infinitesimal limit form (
):
From now onwards, the balance equations for the level variables of the HD will be written in the infinitesimal limit form. Note from the HD the causal influences on the flows
and
. The hypothesis about how to compute them is provided by the drug chemical kinetics [
21,
22]:
Then, from (5) in (3) and (4), their initial values are added in the initial time
:
Equations (6) and (7) provide the drug dynamics in organism, being
α and
β two parameters to be calibrated for both participants and phases. Both parameters depend on the individual personality and on the kind of drug. For alcohol in Phase B, they are calibrated in the intervals
α [0.00118, 0.02050] and
β [0.00462, 0.00533] following the literature about alcohol kinetics [
22], and for caffeine in Phase C the corresponding intervals are
α [0.00137, 0.091138] and
β [0.00144, 0.00304] following the literature about caffeine kinetics [
21].
Considering the balance equation in the infinitesimal limit form for the happiness level variable,
in the HD:
Taking the literature about the subject as a base, the hypotheses about the mathematical patterns of the three flows considered in (8) are explained in the following paragraphs.
About the homeostatic control,
fr(t), it is a control around the tonic or basal happiness
b. It is assumed that the happiness state,
y(t), tends to return to the tonic happiness when it is modified by a stimulus:
Parameter b in (9) represents the value to which the y(t) variable would tend asymptotically under the ideal case that no other stimulus would influence the individual happiness. The restriction to be considered in the calibration of parameter b is that its value must belong to the interval of the corresponding happiness scale: b 0, 20] for the ES and b (0, 7] for the SFS. Parameter a represents the strength or power of the above-mentioned asymptotic dynamics and its only restriction is that its value must be greater or equal to zero. Both parameter values also depend on the kind of drug and on the individual characteristics.
About the excitation effect,
fe(t), it tends to increase happiness proportionally to the stimulus,
s(t), and the present happiness,
y(t), per unit of drug amount
M:
Parameter
p in (10) represents the strength or power of the excitation effect, and its value also depends on the kind of drug and on the individual characteristics. The only restriction for
p is that its value must be greater or equal to zero. Note that the greater the value of
p the greater the excitation effect of the considered drug on the individual happiness [
21,
22].
About the inhibitor effect,
fi(t), assuming that it actuates with some delay, it tends to decrease happiness proportionally to the happiness delay,
z(t), per unit of drug amount
M:
Parameter
q in (11) represents the strength or power of the inhibitor effect, and its value also depends on the kind of drug and on the individual characteristics. The only restriction for
q is that its value must be greater or equal to zero. Note that the greater the value of
q the greater the inhibitor effect of the considered drug on the individual happiness [
21,
22]. That is, the greater the values of
p and
q are the more intense is, respectively, the excitation effect and the inhibitor effect.
By substituting (9), (10) and (11) in (8), and adding the initial happiness value at the initial time
:
Considering the balance equation in the infinitesimal limit form for the happiness delay level variable,
, of the HD:
According to the literature about the subject [
21,
22], the hypotheses about the mathematical patterns of the two flows
fh(t) and
fl(t) that appear in (13) are the following.
About the happiness inhibitor delay,
fh(t), it is a decreasing control about the happiness delay with a time parameter
as follows:
The only restriction for parameter
τ value in (14) is that it has to be positive. About the happiness excitation delay,
fl(t), it tends to increase the happiness delay as the product of the present stimulus and happiness:
The initial value of
in
is zero due to it just starts to act when the stimulus arises from this instant. Therefore, considering this initial value and substituting (14) and (15) in (13):
Equations (6), (7), (12), and (16) define the stimulus response model (SRM) to study the short-term happiness dynamical response to a stimulus provided by a drug single dose, such as the here considered ones: alcohol and caffeine.
However, the SRM can be presented in a briefer way. On the one hand, the drug dynamics is given by (6) and (7); nevertheless, they define an independent subsystem of the SRM from which the drug in blood variable
can be analytically found:
On the other hand, Equation (16) is a linear differential equation in
; therefore, it can be integrated and substituted into (12):
Equation (18) is an integrodifferential equation where the inhibitor effect arises explicitly with a continuous delay (the integral term). Both Equations (17) and (18) also define the SRM in a much more concise way. However, the complexity of (18) forces the researcher to find its solutions numerically. Consequently, it is more advantageous to find numerical solutions for happiness by using Equations (12) and (16), while the solutions for the drug in blood variable are found directly by (17) (see following section).
5. Dynamical Results of the Stimulus Response Model (SRM)
In order to fit the SRM dynamics presented in
Section 4 to the scores obtained by the experimental design presented in
Section 3, two questions must be answered: (1) how to achieve the SRM outcomes with uncertainty; and (2) how to achieve the best outcomes (also with uncertainty) of the model parameters of
Table 4 to reproduce the dynamical patterns followed by the experimental design scores.
The answer for question (1) is that the outcomes of the stimulus response model are provided by Equation (17) for the drug in blood variable, , and by using the 4th order Runge–Kutta method for the numerical outcomes of the happiness variable, through Equations (12) and (16). However, these SRM outcomes do not present uncertainty. Therefore, to consider its uncertainty, the following steps are followed:
- (a)
Once the SRM outcomes are obtained, the corresponding residuals are computed as the differences between the experimental scores and the SRM outcomes.
- (b)
The p-value of the Anderson–Darling test is applied to the residuals to determine whether they are distributed as normal distributions of zero mean and sd standard deviation, N(0, sd), or not. Note that the H0 hypothesis is that the residuals follow such normal distribution. Subsequently, if the H0 hypothesis holds, the confidence interval for every SRM outcome is computed by the usual way, i.e., by the Student’s t distribution with a 0.05 signification level.
- (c)
All corresponding figures present the experimental scores jointly with the SRM mean value line and the two limit lines corresponding to the confidence intervals.
The answer for question (2) is that a genetic algorithm, whose algorithmic bases can be found in [
31], provides the model happiness responses with random residuals (i.e., the noise, considered as the influence of other sources different to the stimuli), as well as a sample of 40 relatively optimal sets of parameter values (those described in
Table 4) that better fit the model outcomes to the experimental scores. To be more precise, the genetic algorithm of [
31] just presents the set of values of each parameter that better fits the model outcomes. The genetic algorithm version used in this work is an advance on the [
31] version that provides a sample of the best sets of parameter values to achieve the corresponding confidence intervals.
For better understanding of how the genetic algorithm works, a brief explanation is provided here (details can be found in [
31]). The genetic algorithm tries to imitate natural selection. So, an individual is defined like a set of values for the parameters (genes) of the model and a population is defined as a set of individuals. Reproduction is defined as the creation of a new individual by choosing randomly a gene from every couple of genes one from each parent (may be with random mutation). Consanguinity is restricted to avoid too small differences between individuals. The SRM provides a fitting value for each individual of the considered population. The fitting method consists of calculating the quadratic sum (QS) of the differences between experimental scores and those provided by the SRM. The best fitting individuals (those with lesser values for QS) are chosen as reproducers to produce the next generation. Some immigrants may be considered. Summary: (1) The first population is determined by assigning randomly a value to each parameter (gene) inside a given scale. (2) Calculation of QS for each individual, ordering and selection of reproducers. (3) Random immigration. (4) Random reproduction (with consanguinity control and random mutation) up to completing population. (5) New ordering and selection. (6) Checking the end condition: if yes then exit routine else go to (3). The end condition is “no lesser QS values are observed”.
Once the samples of parameter values are obtained, the p-value of the Anderson–Darling test is applied to each sample to determine whether they are distributed as a N(m, sd), i.e., a normal distribution of m mean and sd standard deviation corresponding to each parameter. Note that the H0 hypothesis is that the samples follow a normal distribution. Subsequently, if the H0 hypothesis holds, the confidence intervals are computed in the usual way.
In addition, the results of the fitting process obtained through the genetic algorithm are evaluated by the determination coefficient values (), which measures the fitting degree between the experimental scores and the corresponding values provided by the SRM. Note that the SRM values here considered are those computed by the averages of the 40 size parameter samples, i.e., by the mean value of their confidence intervals. In addition, the measures the relation between the square covariance of both sets of data and the product of both variances, and it varies from 0 to 1, i.e., , such that the closer to the unit the better the fitting degree. The determination coefficient values are provided jointly with the p-value of the Anderson–Darling test with which the residuals are checked for being noise, i.e., a N(0, sd) or a normal distribution of zero mean and sd standard deviation between the experimental scores and the corresponding values given by the model. Note again that the H0 hypothesis is that the residuals follow a normal distribution.
It is important to emphasize the joint meaning of
and the Anderson–Darling test: the
value represents the part of scores’ dynamics explained by the SRM, and if, in addition, the residuals are demonstrated to be random by the Anderson–Darling test then it can be asserted that no other significant dynamical pattern is also assumed by the scores’ dynamics. Moreover, if a high
value is associated with a low residual dispersion (do not confuse with the statistical dispersion of
Section 3) and vice versa, then, in this case, the individual feels the drug effect accurately, and in the contrary case (low
value that is associated with a high dynamical dispersion) the individual feels the drug effect not clearly.
Moreover, the production of the SRM numerical solutions as well as the above-described genetic algorithm is stated in a C++ program. The corresponding figures and Anderson–Darling test p-values have been obtained by the software MATHEMATICA 14.0.
Figure 2 (for Participant 1) and
Figure 3 (for Participant 2) show the scores in low stimuli conditions for both participants in the experimental design in Phase A (base line), in both happiness scales. Note that both figures are far from seeming to represent noise. In fact, the Anderson–Darling test provides for both participants a
p-value lesser than 0.01, i.e., it is not an orthodox noise N(
m,
sd), being
m the average score. Therefore, either the outcomes follow a dynamical pattern produced by boring or the noise follows an unknown distribution. However, the dynamical patterns of happiness produced in Phase B and Phase C by alcohol and caffeine, respectively, are radically different, as it can be observed in the subsequent figures.
Figure 4 presents the scores with uncertainty along time of the ES and SFS for Phase B of the experimental design of Participant 1, jointly with the curves predicted by the SRM because of 26.51 mL of alcohol dose. Note that for the ES response
and for the SFS response
, being the corresponding Anderson–Darling test of the residuals, respectively, 0.71 and 0.95, which provide us a conclusion: the SRM explains almost fully the scores’ dynamics of Participant 1, in other words, the prediction of the SRM for this participant is excellent, which implies that Participant 1 feels accurately the happiness response to the alcohol stimulus.
In addition,
Table 6 shows the parameter confidence intervals of the SRM provided by the genetic algorithm for Participant 1, except for the dose and the initial values that are known and then presented with their respective single values. The corresponding
opp confidence intervals (
) obtained from
Table 6 are (6.57082, 8.16833) min
−1 for ES and (0.887418, 1.12422) min
−1 for SFS (confidence level 0.95). In
Section 6 these data are compared with those of Participant 2. In addition, note that the initial values for Participant 1 are very low for both happiness scales (
for both scales). About the initial values, according to the GFP theory [
21,
22], it is very important to emphasize that for happiness in both scales: the lower the initial value is, the more significant the dynamical responses will be, and vice versa. This is in correspondence with the high determination coefficients values obtained for both scales. Therefore, what the SRM predicts is that Participant 1 becomes happier after alcohol consumption than he usually uses to be, at least in the short term.
Figure 5 presents the scores with uncertainty of the ES and SFS for Phase B of the experimental design of Participant 2, jointly with the curves predicted by the SRM because of 26.51 mL of alcohol dose. Note that the scores’ residual dispersion is much higher than that of Participant 1. In fact, the low values of the determination coefficients,
for ES and
for SFS, announce this residual dispersion, although the Anderson–Darling test of the residuals (respectively, 0.46 and 0.70) shows us that the residuals are random. The conclusion from these data is very important: compared with Participant 1, the part of the data variability explained by the SRM for Participant 2 provides a very low happiness response to the alcohol stimulus.
In addition,
Table 7 shows the corresponding parameters’ confidence intervals of the SRM provided by the genetic algorithm for Participant 2, except for the dose and the initial values that are known and then presented with its respective single value. The corresponding
opp confidence intervals (
) obtained from
Table 7 are (4.46197, 6.20496) min
−1 for ES and (0.18140, 0.25944) min
−1 for SFS (confidence level 0.95). In
Section 6 these data are compared with those of Participant 1. In this case, the model predictions must be taken more carefully, due to the low effect of alcohol on this participant happiness. However, it can be advanced, by comparing the respective
opp intervals, that Participant 1 feels significantly more intensively the effect of alcohol consumption than Participant 2 for both happiness scales. In addition, note that the initial happiness values of Participant 2 are higher for both happiness scales than for Participant 1 (
for the ES and
for the SFS). Following again the GFP theory [
21,
22], the higher the initial value the less significant the dynamical responses and vice versa. This is in correspondence with the lower determination coefficients values obtained for both scales.
Figure 6 presents the scores with uncertainty of ES and SFS for Phase C of the experimental design for Participant 1, jointly with the corresponding curves predicted by the SRM because of 330 mg of caffeine dose. Note that the determination coefficients are again very high for Participant 1. In fact,
for ES and
for SFS, due to the residual dispersion is very low. In addition, the Anderson–Darling tests for the residuals give, respectively, 0.35 and 0.43, i.e., the residuals are random. Again, the conclusion of these data is very important: Participant 1 also feels, in a very strong way, the happiness response to the caffeine stimulus.
In addition,
Table 8 shows the parameters’ confidence intervals of the SRM provided by the genetic algorithm for Participant 1, except for the dose and the initial values that are known and then presented with its respective single value. The corresponding
opp confidence intervals (
) obtained from
Table 8 are (0.47382, 0.68607) min
−1 for ES and (0.44504, 0.64521) min
−1 for SFS (confidence level 0.95). In
Section 6 these data are compared with those of Participant 2. In addition, note that the initial values of Participant 1 are very low for both happiness scales (
for both scales). About these values, the GFP theory [
21,
22] says that the lower the initial value the more significant the dynamical responses and vice versa, which is very important to be also emphasized for happiness in both scales. This is in correspondence with the high values obtained for the determination coefficients for both scales. Therefore, again for Participant 1, what the SRM predicts is that Participant 1 becomes happier after caffeine consumption than what he usually is, at least in the short term.
Figure 7 presents the scores with uncertainty of the ES and SFS for Phase C of the experimental design of Participant 2, jointly with the curves predicted by the SRM because of 330 mg of caffeine dose. Note that the scores’ residual dispersion is again much higher than that for Participant 1, in correspondence with the lower values of the determination coefficients,
for ES and
for SFS, although the Anderson–Darling test of the residuals (respectively, 0.80 and 0.25) indicate us that the residuals are random. The conclusion from these data is very important: compared with Participant 1, the part of the data variability explained by the SRM for Participant 2 provides in a very low way his happiness response to the caffeine stimulus.
In addition,
Table 9 shows the parameters’ confidence intervals of the SRM provided by the genetic algorithm for phase C of Participant 2, except for the dose and the initial values that are known and then presented with its respective single values. The corresponding
opp confidence intervals (
) obtained from
Table 9 are (2.03852, 2.49726) min
−1 for ES and (3.13283, 5.17100) min
−1 for SFS (confidence level 0.95). In
Section 6 these data are compared with those of Participant 1. In this case, the model predictions must be taken again very carefully, due to the low effect of the caffeine stimulus for participant happiness. However, although the part of the data variability explained by the SRM for Participant 2 provides in a very low way the happiness response to the caffeine stimulus, compared with Participant 1, it can be advanced, by comparing the respective
opp intervals, that Participant 2 feels significantly more intensively the effect of caffeine consumption than Participant 1 for both happiness scales, despite the initial happiness values of Participant 2 are higher for both happiness scales than for Participant 1 (
for the ES and
for the SFS).
A general conclusion of this section is that the SRM is a mathematical tool that predicts how much a stimulus (alcohol or caffeine) affects individual happiness along time as well as the happiness dynamics pattern of this change. In addition, the SRM highlights the individual differences between the responses to both drugs that the inferential statistics approach of
Section 3 has discovered: (a) the jointly representation of the experimental scores with the SRM predictions in Phases B and C (
Figure 4,
Figure 5,
Figure 6 and
Figure 7); (b) the determination coefficients and the residual dispersion that this coefficient represents (the lower the determination coefficient the more residual dispersion); and (c) the significant differences between the overall power parameter (
opp) confidence intervals. However, the individual differences between both participants are deeply explained in
Section 6 in an integrated way with the inferential statistics results of
Section 3.
7. Discussion
Comparing the present approach with other approaches, on the one hand, Structural Equations Modeling (SEM), time series or network models (for instance) perhaps could fit the experimental data. However, we must consider that the SRM constitutes consolidated knowledge and its variables, parameters, and equations have a biological meaning. The SEM approach hardly could respect the hydrodynamic diagram of
Figure 1 with the biological sense of its variables, parameters and structure. Note, for instance, that the drug (alcohol and caffeine) pharmacokinetics fits the balance equations of the System Dynamics approach that the hydrodynamic diagram provides. Then, the subsequent SRM equations are a natural continuation from the pharmacokinetics to the general pharmacodynamics represented by the SRM. In addition, the SRM approach considers the nonlinear dynamical equations that the SEM cannot represent.
On the other hand, network models and time series are considered as input–output models and do not provide any explanation of the biological mechanisms underlying personality dynamics after drug consumption. Again, for instance, the pharmacokinetics of the drug, the homeostatic control, the immediate excitation effect or the delayed inhibition effect, which defines the general pharmacodynamics represented by the SRM, i.e., the SRM nonlinear dynamical equations that the other models cannot represent.
A single-case experimental design with replication with two participants is used in this study. This fact leads us to a controversy about how to apply the statistical analysis to a single-case experimental design. In a classical book about this topic [
32], the authors consider that ANOVA is not the most appropriate statistical analysis for the single-case experimental design. Nevertheless, being aware of the methodological limitations of this bold suggestion, this kind of analysis has been chosen because its results can illustrate very well the implicit dynamics in the effects of drugs, taking into account that the dynamical mathematical model can complement these results and facilitate both their humanistic and their natural scientific interpretations.
In fact, the results are clearly in the same vein. Starting from the two mathematical approaches, we can conclude that the difference between both participants in the experiment is evident: P1 presents more statistical dispersion (SD) than P2, but P2 presents more residual dispersion than P1, computed by the SRM predictions. In fact, this result of the SRM represents that P1 feels in a stronger way the dynamical response to both drugs than P2. However, the results provide random residuals in both cases, and so the SRM represents the deterministic predictive part of the dynamical responses.
As other studies pointed out [
21,
22], the model describes and predicts how the General Factor of Personality (GFP) changes in response to a single dose of caffeine or alcohol. This mathematical model predicts that the lower the GFP trait score is, the higher the response to both caffeine and alcohol intake will be, and better the corresponding model evolution curve will fit the data. Reading the Happiness scores, the relationship between the trait and the state evolution after a single dose intake of caffeine or alcohol is not the same as regarding the GFP in the previous studies. So, the difference between both participants is 10 and 14 points, respectively, for the Euphoria Scale, and the same score (5 points) for the Face Scale, and as we expected similar results in GFP and Happiness scores because they are closely related, this discrepancy should be studied in the future.
On the other hand, these results are consistent with the ones obtained for the GFP in the sense that the lower the initial score is the higher level will be achieved during the response to the drug intake. Likewise, the same thing happens regarding Happiness as this study reveals. So, the means of BL scores for P1 are 3.84 and 3.89 for the Euphoria Scale of Happiness and for the Face Scale, respectively, while for P2 they are 7.63 and 4.58 analogously. This result is coherent with the model prediction for the personality responses to drug intakes [
21,
22].
Finally, the individual differences between both participants observed by using the SRM parameter values can be highlighted. The overall power parameter (
opp) sees the integration of four parameters involved in the three balance terms of Equation (12) or Equation (18): the homeostatic effect power (
a), the excitation effect power (
p) and the two parameters involved in the inhibition effect power, i.e., the inhibition effect power (
q) and the inhibitor effect delay (
). Their products have the same units of
a and
p (min
−1). The addition of the four parameters as
provides the sought global effect of the drug on an individual. In addition, due to the calibration has been performed with uncertainty, this global effect can be presented as a confidence interval for both participants. Comparing those confidence intervals significant individual differences can be found out. The conclusion is showed clearly in
Table 10 by using the
overall power parameter (
opp) confidence intervals: the effect of alcohol consumption is significantly greater for Participant 1 than for Participant 2, and the effect of caffeine consumption is significantly greater for Participant 2 than for Participant 1. Moreover, as developed in
Section 5, this result is in coherence with the inferential statistics. Therefore, one of the main goals of the present study is reached: that referred to the integration of the inferential statistics method, used classically in the behavioral sciences, and that known as System Dynamics, used for the modeling and simulation of complex systems.
This study has some limitations. We use a specific dose of drug, 26.51 mL of alcohol and 330 mg of caffeine, as a replication of doses used in previous research with group experimental design [
21,
22], but it would be better to calculate an individual dose taking into account the physical conditions of both participants. In addition, we presented a single-case experimental design with replications, but to increase the number of experimental subjects, it is necessary to test some of the considered hypothesis from an intra- and inter-individual experimental design. However, some considerations must be considered in this regard:
This is not a case study but rather it is an experiment with control of variables, a single-case design with replication [
28]. Thus, the data obtained in the baseline (phase A) are contrasted with two alternative treatments (phases B and C). This experimental design has given good results, in terms of its ability to describe and predict subjective measures of the effects produced by drugs, and in terms of the adjustment of dynamic mathematical models.
The results of this study are consistent with previous studies using between-group designs that imply a larger number of participants and the same doses of both caffeine and alcohol [
21,
22]. In the first of these studies, 20 participants received 330 mg of caffeine, and in the second study, 28 participants received 26.51 g of alcohol.
Although the two previous studies used different instruments than the one used here to measure the effect of drugs, there are some positive and significant statistical relationships between all of them. Thus, in the study about caffeine [
21] the MAACL-R (Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist Revised) [
33] was used, and in the study about alcohol [
22] the GFP-FAS was used. In several studies it has been found that these scales are positively and significantly related to each and the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (OHQ) [
34]. Thus, the data obtained in this study are consistent with previous studies with large samples of participants and closely related subjective effects evaluation instruments. On the other hand, it can be argued that the results obtained in a single session cannot be extrapolated for a long-term effect, and that to increase euphoria in a session does not mean that happiness can increase and maintain itself in a stable way during life. As we pointed out above, the positive affect produced by alcohol consumption decreases along time, but it depends on the consumption pattern [
20], what means that we can promote a moderate alcohol use as a therapeutic goal [
22]. In addition, some authors [
35,
36] claim that happiness is associated with and precedes numerous successful outcomes, including marriage, friendship, income, work performance, and health. Then, the happiness–success link exists not only because success makes people happy, but also because positive affect engenders success.
Note, however, that this study is a first approach to the relationship between happiness and drug consumption. Considering that this subject can provide an upset social discussion due to somebody can understand that this paper suggests consuming drugs to reach short periods of happiness, we want to emphasize that its objective is the opposite: preventing consumers that drug consumption must be performed rationally. From this result, in future research, the objective could be to relate happiness with personality dynamics, such as it has been already performed with the General Factor of Personality dynamics [
21,
22].
The general conclusion that we can draw from this study is that inferential statistics and the SRM derived from the Systems Dynamics approach can be used in a complementary and enriching way to obtain prediction results. Particularly, it has been demonstrated that drugs such as alcohol and caffeine can increase happiness in a single session as measured by two different scales, ES and SFS.