Effects of Vowel Length on Gaze Durations in Silent and Oral Reading
Abstract
:Introduction
The Present Study
Experiment 1
Method
Results
a) | ||||||||
Vowel length | Frequency | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | ||||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | |||
short | high | 201 | 8.55 | 224 | 11.13 | 136 | 26 | |
low | 230 | 8.94 | 259 | 19.96 | 148 | 20 | ||
long | high | 215 | 11.68 | 252 | 17.91 | 112 | 25 | |
low | 226 | 9.65 | 262 | 17.26 | 157 | 26 | ||
b) | ||||||||
Word | Vowel length | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | ||||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | |||
N-1 | short | 223 | 10 | 260 | 16 | 239 | 46 | |
long | 244 | 25 | 292 | 35 | 215 | 43 | ||
N+1 | short | 230 | 25 | 251 | 31 | 79 | 14 | |
long | 255 | 30 | 272 | 36 | 90 | 13 | ||
N+2 | short | 233 | 23 | 278 | 31 | 156 | 26 | |
long | 231 | 13 | 279 | 19 | 157 | 20 |
Discussion
Experiment 2
Method
Results
a) | |||||||
Vowel length | Frequency | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
short | high | 204 | 12.81 | 239 | 14.21 | 115 | 20 |
low | 219 | 14.26 | 253 | 13.15 | 120 | 16 | |
long | high | 214 | 15.02 | 245 | 16.29 | 130 | 18 |
low | 215 | 11.23 | 250 | 13.41 | 155 | 22 | |
b) | |||||||
Word | Vowel length | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
N-1 | short | 239 | 17 | 291 | 15 | 170 | 19 |
long | 230 | 16 | 284 | 15 | 180 | 24 | |
N+1 | short | 221 | 15 | 244 | 17 | 81 | 12 |
long | 229 | 19 | 261 | 26 | 100 | 15 | |
N+2 | short | 226 | 14 | 272 | 13 | 146 | 18 |
long | 229 | 15 | 289 | 14 | 144 | 19 |
Discussion
Experiment 3
Method
Results
Discussion
Experiment 4
Method
Results
Discussion
Cross-Experiment Comparisons
General Discussion
The Timeline of Vowel Length Processing
Modulation of the Vowel Length Effect Across Experiments
Comparison with Previous Vowel Length Studies
A Three-stage Framework of Phonological Processing During Reading
Acknowledgments
References
- Abramson, M., and S. Goldinger. 1997. What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: Silent reading activates inner speech. Perception & Psychophysics 59: 1059–1068. [Google Scholar]
- Ashby, J., and C. Clifton. 2005. The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition 96: 89–100. [Google Scholar]
- Ashby, J., R. Treiman, B. Kessler, and K. Rayner. 2006. Vowel processing during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 32: 416424. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen, R. H., R. Piepenbrock, and L. Gulikers. 1995. The CELEX lexical database (Release 2) [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania [Distributor. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. D., V. J. Lewis, and G. Vallar. 1984. Exploring the articulatory loop. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 36(A): 233–252. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. D., N. Thomson, and M. Buchanan. 1975. Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 14: 575–589. [Google Scholar]
- Berent, I., and C. A. Perfetti. 1995. A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonological assembly in reading English. Psychological Review 102: 146–184. [Google Scholar]
- Browman, C., and L. Goldstein. 1995. Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Edited by R. Port and T. van Gelder. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA: pp. 175–193. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, R. 1958. Words and things: An introduction to language. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N., and M. Halle. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Coltheart, M., K. Rastle, C. Perry, R. Langdon, and J. Ziegler. 2001. DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108: 204–256. [Google Scholar]
- Daneman, M., and E. M. Reingold. 1993. What eye fixations tell us about phonological recoding during reading. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 47, 153–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daneman, M., and E. M. Reingold. 2000. Do readers use phonological codes to activate word meanings? Evidence from eye movements. In Reading as a perceptual process. Edited by A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller and J. Pynte. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 447–473. [Google Scholar]
- Drieghe, D., and M. Brysbaert. 2002. Strategic effects in associative priming with words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28: 951961. [Google Scholar]
- Egger, V. 1904. La parole intérieure: Essai de psychologie descriptive. Thèse présenté à la Faculté des Lettres de Paris par Victor Egger [Inner speech: An experiment of descriptive psychology. Thesis presented by Victor Egger to the Faculté des Lettres, Paris], 2nd ed. Paris: Alcan. [Google Scholar]
- Engbert, R., A. Nuthmann, E. M. Richter, and R. Kliegl. 2005. SWIFT: a dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review 112: 777–813. [Google Scholar]
- Fodor, J. D. 1998. Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27: 285–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folk, J. R. 1999. Phonological codes are used to access the lexicon during silent reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25: 892–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fredriksen, J., and J. Kroll. 1976. Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 2: 361–379. [Google Scholar]
- Frost, R. 1998. Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin 123: 71–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heller, D. 1982. Eye movements in reading. In Cognition and eye movements. Edited by R. Groner and P. Fraisse. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp. 139–154. [Google Scholar]
- Hendriks, A. W., and H. H. J. Kolk. 1997. Strategic control in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 14: 321–366. [Google Scholar]
- Huey, E. B. 1908/1968. The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jared, D., and M. S. Seidenberg. 1990. Naming multisyllabic words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 92–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kenstowicz, M., and C. Kisserberth. 1979. Generative phonology. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Klapp, S. T. 1971. Implicit speech inferred from response latencies in same-different decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 91: 262–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landerl, K., U. Frith, and H. Wimmer. 1996. Intrusion of orthographic knowledge on phoneme awareness: Strong in normal readers, weak in dyslexic readers. Applied Psycholinguistics 17: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, Y.-A., K. S. Binder, J. O. Kim, A. Pollatsek, and K. Rayner. 1999. Activation of phonological codes during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25: 948–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levin, H., and A. Buckler-Addis. 1979. The eye-voice span. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, W., A. W. Inhoff, Y. Ye, and C. Wu. 2002. Use of parafoveally visible characters during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 12131227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lukatela, J., T. Eaton, L. Sabadini, and M. T. Turvey. 2004. Vowel duration affects visual word identification: Evidence that the mediating phonology is phonetically informed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 151162. [Google Scholar]
- Lukatela, G., and M. T. Turvey. 1994. Visual access is initially phonological: I. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123: 107–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCusker, L., M. Hillinger, and R. Bias. 1981. Phonological recoding and reading. Psychological Bulletin 89: 217–245. [Google Scholar]
- McCutchen, D., and C. Perfetti. 1982. The visual tonguetwister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21: 672–687. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, D. E., R. W. Schvaneveldt, and M. G. Ruddy. 1974. Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition 2: 309321. [Google Scholar]
- Miellet, S., and L. Sparrow. 2004. Phonological codes are assembled before word fixation: Evidence from boundary paradigm in sentence reading. Brain and Language 90: 299–310. [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti, C. A., and L. C. Bell. 1991. Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 473–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perfetti, C. A., S. Zhang, and I. Berent. 1992. Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a “universal” phonological principle. In Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning. Edited by R. Frost and L. Katz. Amsterdam: North-Holland, pp. 227–248. [Google Scholar]
- Pollatsek, A., M. F. Lesch, R. K. Morris, and K. Rayner. 1992. Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18: 148–162. [Google Scholar]
- Pollatsek, A., L. H. Tan, and K. Rayner. 2000. The role of phonological codes in integrating information across saccadic eye movements in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 607–633. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124: 372–422. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, K., and S. A. Duffy. 1986. Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory and Cognition 14: 191–201. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Rayner, K., A. Pollatsek, and K. S. Binder. 1998. Phonological codes and eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 476–497. [Google Scholar]
- Reichle, E., K. Rayner, and A. Pollatsek. 2003. The E-Z Reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26: 445–526. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Reilly, R., and R. Radach. 2006. Some empirical tests of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 34–55. [Google Scholar]
- Slowiaczek, M. L., and C. Clifton, Jr. 1980. Subvocalization and reading for meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19: 573–582. [Google Scholar]
- Spoehr, K., and E. Smith. 1971. The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 104: 21–34. [Google Scholar]
- Van Orden, G. C. 1987. A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition 15: 181198. [Google Scholar]
- Waters, G. S., M. K. Komoda, and T. Y. Arbuckle. 1985. The effects of concurrent tasks on reading: Implications for phonological recoding. Journal of Memory and Language 24: 27–45. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, M., C. Pfeil, R. Lotz, and K. Biddle. 1994. Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: the contribution of orthographic factors. In The varieties of orthographic knowledge 1: Theoretical and developmental issues. Edited by V.W. Berninger. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 137–171. [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler, J. C., L. Ferrand, A. M. Jacobs, A. Rey, and J. Grainger. 2000. Visual and phonological codes in letter and word recognition: Evidence from incremental priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 53(A): 671–692. [Google Scholar]
High frequency | Low frequency | |||
Short vowel | Long vowel | Short vowel | Long vowel | |
Word frequency (per million) | 66.00 | 67.13 | 2.46 | 2.33 |
Number of letters | 4.54 | 4.54 | 4.58 | 4.46 |
Number of syllables | 1.29 | 1.38 | 1.50 | 1.33 |
Orthographic regularity (position-dependent bigram frequency) | 2128 | 2119 | 2076 | 2066 |
Word position on line (in letter units) | 32 | 34 | 34 | 35 |
Pre-target word frequency (per million) | 426 | 505 | 277 | 272 |
Pre-target word length | 5.96 | 5.58 | 5.92 | 5.88 |
Post-target word length | 3.00 | 2.96 | 3.00 | 3.04 |
a) | |||||||
Vowel length | Frequency | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
short | high | 202 | 7.96 | 230 | 12.07 | 126 | 16 |
low | 219 | 7.66 | 251 | 11.20 | 105 | 26 | |
long | high | 198 | 7.29 | 228 | 13.05 | 100 | 17 |
low | 214 | 8.31 | 245 | 16.32 | 116 | 26 | |
b) | |||||||
Word | Vowel length | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
N-1 | short | 227 | 12 | 280 | 19 | 187 | 43 |
long | 221 | 12 | 270 | 20 | 175 | 31 | |
N+1 | short | 206 | 14 | 220 | 17 | 64 | 14 |
long | 208 | 11 | 226 | 12 | 55 | 16 | |
N+2 | short | 216 | 10 | 243 | 15 | 126 | 23 |
long | 223 | 8 | 262 | 12 | 117 | 21 |
a) | |||||||
Vowel length | Frequency | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
short | high | 178 | 8.38 | 197 | 10.02 | 130 | 26 |
low | 194 | 8.60 | 249 | 19.91 | 122 | 20 | |
long | high | 191 | 9.38 | 235 | 10.15 | 140 | 24 |
low | 198 | 11.22 | 244 | 17.16 | 167 | 27 | |
b) | |||||||
Word | Vowel length | First fixation duration | Gaze duration | Rereading time | |||
M | SE | M | SE | M | SE | ||
N-1 | short | 225 | 17 | 284 | 19 | 208 | 45 |
long | 225 | 12 | 278 | 13 | 228 | 46 | |
N+1 | short | 222 | 14 | 254 | 21 | 98 | 21 |
long | 216 | 10 | 244 | 15 | 117 | 21 | |
N+2 | short | 202 | 10 | 232 | 10 | 168 | 31 |
long | 209 | 10 | 253 | 11 | 186 | 30 |
Copyright © 2010. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Huestegge, L. Effects of Vowel Length on Gaze Durations in Silent and Oral Reading. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2009, 3, 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.5.5
Huestegge L. Effects of Vowel Length on Gaze Durations in Silent and Oral Reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2009; 3(5):1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.5.5
Chicago/Turabian StyleHuestegge, Lynn. 2009. "Effects of Vowel Length on Gaze Durations in Silent and Oral Reading" Journal of Eye Movement Research 3, no. 5: 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.5.5
APA StyleHuestegge, L. (2009). Effects of Vowel Length on Gaze Durations in Silent and Oral Reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(5), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.5.5