Introduction
Pure vergence stimuli can be constructed in the laboratory and are useful for the study of disparity vergence eye movements. Such stimuli can be presented using a stereo pair of images moving in equal and opposite directions or by two targets placed at different depths along the midline (i.e., mid-sagittal or cyclopean axis). In the latter case, an accommodative (i.e., blurdriven) stimulus may also drive the vergence response. These “pure vergence stimuli” might be expected to produce a “pure vergence response” where vergence motor components drive the two eyes an equal amount in opposite (i.e., disjunctive) directions. Theoretically, pure vergence responses would follow along the mid-sagittal plane with no version component to the response.
Here we define version and vergence formally by the equations (These definitions apply only to movement directions and the plotting convention used here. They do not define the motor components of either version or vergence which are difficult to isolate. See
Semmlow et al. (
2008) for a discussion of terminology.):
In this paper, we define convergence as positive. Since the left eye (the positive eye in Eq. 1) moves to the right during convergence, rightward version movements are signed positive to be consistent with the vergence equation (Eq. 1). Hence divergence and leftward version movements are noted as negative.
In the response to a pure vergence stimulus, the version component should theoretically be equal zero. In normal subjects with good binocular vision, the net version component must be very small by the end of the movement, since steady fixations errors are close to zero (
Ogle, 1954). Two factors can lead to a transient version component during a symmetrical vergence response. First, vergence responses often contain saccades (
Levi et al., 1987;
van Leeuwen et al., 1998;
Zee et al., 1992;
Collewijn et al., 1995;
Semmlow et al., 2008) and even a small saccade will generate a substantial deviation for the midline. Second, even if no saccades are present, vergence movements in response to a symmetrical vergence stimulus are often asymmetrical because one eye often moves faster than the other in pure vergence responses (Hong et al., 1998).
When saccades are present, there is a facilitation of the vergence response in the form of increased velocity during the intersaccadic period. This facilitation was originally attributed by
Zee et al. (
1992) to the decrease in vergence burst neuron inhibition when omnipause neurons are silenced during saccades. The original model of this interaction, the “saccade-related vergence burst model” has been updated to include an additional influence from the saccadic system that modifies a parameter of the vergence pulse generator (
Kumar et al., 2006). This picture of saccade-vergence interaction has recently been modified by neurophysiological evidence from
King and Zhou (
2002) and Cullen’s group showing that premotor neurons encode combined conjunctive and disjunctive behavior for a given eye, the ipsilateral eye (
Van Horn & Cullen 2008; Van Horn, Sylvestre and Cullen, 2008). This and evidence from other premotor areas have led this group to conclude that saccade burst neurons by themselves “carry all the vergence drive necessary” to mediate the vergence response (Van Horn, Sylvestre and Cullen, 2008).
The neural mechanism by which eye movements are organized at the premotor level is an active research area, but the focus of this work is on the processes and strategy used to compensate for the midline deviation produced by saccades during responses to pure vergence stimuli. These corrective processes represent a high level control strategy and are only peripherally related to the interactions between version and vergence described above.
Semmlow et al. (
2008) found that most responses to a pure vergence stimulus do contain saccades. For more than half of their subjects, every response contained at least one saccade. A similar finding has been reported by
Coubard and Kapoula (
2008) who found either horizontal or vertical saccades in 84% of all vergence responses.
Semmlow et al. (
2008) also found that the initial saccade in these responses to pure vergence stimuli usually increased the deviation from the midline. (Here we refer to midline deviations as simply “error” and the saccades that produce these deviations, “error-inducing saccades.”) Although most initial saccades produced error, all subjects had some responses where the initial saccade reduced the midline deviation by compensating for a vergence-induced midline deviations.
Semmlow et al. (
2008) present evidence that most error-inducing initial saccades were the result of a monocular distraction produced by the transient diplopia of the vergence stimulus along with ocular preference. A similar suggestion has been made by
van Leeuwen et al. (
1998) who showed that subjects without a strong monocular preference were much more likely to produce saccadefree vergence responses to pure vergence stimuli. In a few subjects, initial error-inducing saccades brought the preferred eye closer to the target, which could lead to faster recognition at the expense of delayed binocular vision.
Irrespective of the motivation for error-inducing saccades, some compensation must occur by the end of the movement. While larger errors, particularly congurgate errors, may be tolerated under certain conditions, normal subjects can achieve highly accurate binocular fixation with errors of minutes of arc (
Ogle, 1954). If the initial saccade in the vergence response moves the eyes away from the midline, then either a subsequent compensatory saccade, an offsetting vergence asymmetry (or slow version), or both is required to bring the eyes back to the midline. This study will isolate those vergence responses containing error-inducing initial saccades and determine the corrective mechanism used to bring the eyes back to binocular fixation.
Results
The goal of this paper is to describe the mechanism used for returning the eyes to midline binocular fixation after an initial saccade has produced a midline error. There are two possible mechanisms to correct the saccadic-induced error: saccadic compensation and/or an asymmetrical vergence response. In the discussion, we note the latter could be a true asymmetric (i.e., monocular) response or the combination of symmetric slow vergence and symmetric slow version responses We use the term ‘asymmetric vergence’ as a generic description of the behavior with no implications about the neural nature of the response. We use the term ‘vergence asymmetry’ as the measure of the version component of the an asymmetric vergence. Both saccades and asymmetrical vergence were used to compensate saccadic-induced midline errors in our subjects. An example of saccadic compensation is seen in
Figure 2A. A large 2.5 deg initial leftward saccade occurs near the beginning of the vergence movement and is compensated approximately 200 msec later by a rightward saccade. There is still some residual error at the end of the second saccade (approximately 0.3 deg) and it is eventually corrected by a combination of asymmetrical vergence and other saccades,
Figure 2B and
Figure 2C, respectively.
An example of the second compensatory mechanism, an offsetting asymmetrical vergence, is shown in
Figure 3. In
Figure 3A, an approximately 1.4 deg saccade to the right visual field is quickly offset by a larger vergence movement in the right eye (blue curve) than in the left (red curve). The combined vergence signal using Eq. 1 and version signal using Eq. 2 is shown in
Figure 3B and
Figure 3C respectively.
It is also possible that both mechanisms contribute approximately equally to compensate for an initial errorinducing saccade. This is illustrated in
Figure 4 where a large initial rightward saccade is compensated about equally by a leftward saccade and a vergence assymmetry.
Error-Inducing Saccades – Saccadic Correction
If the midline error produced by an initial errorinducing saccade is compensated by a secondary saccade, we would expect that the amplitude of the second saccade would correlate strongly (and negatively) with the amplitude of the initial saccade.
Figure 5 shows the correlation between an initial error-inducing saccade and the subsequent saccade for three subjects. These subjects were chosen because they represent the range of behaviors seen in our subjects. If all of the error induced by the initial saccade were compensated by the second saccade, points would lie along the 1-to-1 line passing through zero (solid black line). The slope of the regression line would be -1. When a point lies above or below this line, the difference between the point and that line represents the compensatory contribution of the other mechanism, in this case asymmetrical vergence (or slow version). The dashed black line shows the linear regression of second saccade amplitude against initial saccade amplitude when the initial saccade was errorinducing. (Only responses that had an initial error– inducing saccade were included in the data of
Figure 6.) When the error-inducing saccade was the only saccade in the response, the amplitude of this saccade was plotted as a square point and these responses were not included in the regression analysis.
Figure 5A shows a highly scattered set of data with a regression line (dashed line) having a slope = -0.18 and a low correlation coefficient (
r2 = 0.1). This non-significant correlation indicates that the second saccade did not systematically compensate for the midline deviation produced by the initial error-inducing saccade. A figure below shows that vergence asymmetry is primarily responsible for compensating the saccade-induced midline errors in this subject.
In
Figure 5B, a steeper regression line (slope = -0.74) is seen with a significant correlation (
r2 = 0.74;
p < 0.01) indicating that some, but not all of the error compensation is provided by the second saccade. In
Figure 5C, a large negative slope is found (slope = -0.86) with high correlation (
r2 = 0.88,
p < 0.01). It is clear that in this subject the second saccade is compensating for much of the midline error produced by the initial saccade. The fact that the regression line is shifted to the right of the 1-to1 line indicates that the secondary saccades are slightly larger than that necessary for full compensation.
The information provided in
Figure 5 is summarized by graphing the regression slopes for all our subjects in
Figure 6 (gold bars). Compensatory correlations are negative: a compensating secondary saccade has an amplitude that is the negative (i.e., oppositely directed) of the initial saccade. For better visualization, the negative values are plotted upward. The higher bars show greater correlation between the error-inducing saccade and the corrective saccade. The purple bars represent the regression slope between the initial error-inducing saccade versus all successive saccades. Again, only responses that have an initial error-inducing saccade are used to determine the regression slopes. (The bar graph shows subject numbers on the bottom which have been ordered so that the regression slope values are generally increasing.)
Responses often contain more than two saccades and it is possible that these additional saccades could also be compensating for the midline error induced by the initial saccade. The purple bars in
Figure 6 show the correlation slopes when the net amplitude of all saccades following the initial error-inducing saccade is used. For most subjects, there is no increase in the regression slope, indicating that saccades that occur after the second saccade do not serve in a compensatory role. In Sub. 12, a large increase in slope value is seen, but saccadic compensation is actually reduced because the slope is now further from the ideal -1.0 value than it was before the additional saccades were included. Hence, we conclude that in all subjects that use saccades to reduce the midline error, only the second saccade is involved in compensation.
If the initial error-inducing saccade is not fully compensated by subsequent saccades, the difference must be accounted for by vergence asymmetry. Compensation through asymmetical vergence is quantified in the next section.
Figure 7 shows data from the same three subjects as
Figure 5, but in this case vergence asymmetry is plotted along the vertical axis. Vergence asymmetry is taken as the difference in saccade-free left and right eye movements at the end of the response. This is also equal to the total vergence asymmetry during the response. As with
Figure 5, these plots include only responses that have an initial error-inducing saccade. Since the vergence compensation is measured at the end of the response while the saccadic-induced error occurs early (sometimes even before the vergence response begins), a strong correlation suggests a causal relationship, one in which the vergence asymmetry is in response to the error saccade.
Error-Inducing Saccades ~ Vergence Correction
If the midline error produced by the initial saccade is compensated by asymmetrical vergence, the vergence asymmetry should have a strong negative correlation with the amplitude of the initial saccade.
In
Figure 7A, a strong negative correlation is observed between vergence asymmetry and the amplitude of the initial error-inducing saccade. The regression line has a slope of -1.1 (
r2 = 0.67;
p < 0.01) indicating that most of the initial error is compensated by asymmetrical vergence (The fact that the slope is greater than 1.0 suggests that vergence asymmetry is compensating for asymmetries induced by later saccades as well as the initial saccade.). This complements the findings in
Figure 5A that show saccades have no role in compensating for the saccade-induced midline error in this subject’s responses. In
Figure 7B, the slope is reduced to -0.46, but the correlation remains significant (
r2 = 0.55;
p < 0.01) demonstrating that some of the error compensation comes from asymmetrical vergence. Again this complements the data in
Figure 7B which shows that some saccadic compensation was also used for compensation in this subject. The positive regression slope for the Sub. 12 in
Figure 7C shows that vergence asymmetry plays no role in compensating for the error induced by the initial saccade. Vergence asymmetry may even be increasing the saccade-induced error although the correlation was not significant.
Figure 5C indicated that the second saccade was the primary compensatory mechanism in this subject.
The role of asymmetrical vergence can be summarized for our 12 subjects using a bar graph of regression slopes as in
Figure 6. Again, negative values are plotted upward so that higher bars indicate greater compensation from vergence asymmetry.
Figure 8 plots the regression slopes of the relationship between vergence asymmetry and initial saccadic amplitude (purple bars) and also repeats the slopes of the relationship between the amplitudes of the error-inducing and corrective secondary saccade (gold bars in
Figure 6). Since the inverse values are plotted, a negative value means that the regression slope was actually positive; that is opposite to the slope of a compensatory mechanism. As expected, the slopes that indicate the use of asymmetrical vergence as a compensatory mechanism (purple bars) are approximately complementary to the slopes indicative of saccadic compensation.
A comparison of the regression slopes in
Figure 8 for saccadic and vergence correction of initial error shows a reciprocal relationship. This relationship is emphasized in the plot of saccade compensation slopes versus vergence compensation slopes shown in
Figure 9. The inverse 1-to-1 line is shown as a solid black line and the correlation of these values as a dashed red line. The regression line has slope of -0.89 showing a highly correlated inverse relationship between these two corrective mechanisms (
r2 = 0.61;
p < 0.01). The 12 subjects have been grouped into three classes based on the dominant compensatory mechanism. The boundaries were first identified using unsupervised cluster analysis (routine “clusterdata” from the MATLAB Statistical Toolbox), but are also apparent from the data.
Discussion
Horizontal saccades are found in almost every response to a pure vergence stimulus in our untrained subjects. While we did not measure vertical saccades, their only influence on our data would be to enhance vergence dynamics and that is not of interest in this study. Here, we concentrate on the processes that return the eyes to binocular fixation in those responses where the initial saccade produces a midline error. The three regions shown in
Figure 9 were selected by cluster analysis and represent the primary mechanism for correction based on the amplitude of the corrective movement. The reason a subject selects a given strategy is not currently known. The groups do not correspond to any of the groups found in the previous study (
Semmlow et al., 2008) that were based on saccadic behaviors. The type of strategy used is also not determined by properties of the vergence system such as peak velocities or response latencies. However, based on the strong negative correlation between the two compensatory processes, the role of each of the compensatory processes appears to be well-established, on average, within any given subject.
The regression slopes indicate an average behavior observed over all responses. Even in subjects that rely primarily on one mechanism, either or both mechanisms may be utilized in any given movement. In other words, for any given response, the two processes trade-off compensating each other’s errors. An example of this shown in
Figure 10 which plots the relationship between the second and initial saccade amplitude,
Figure 10A, and vergence asymmetry and initial saccade amplitude,
Figure 10B, for Sub. 10. This subject relies heavily on saccadic compensation as seen in
Figure 10A (regression slope = -0.72); however, in the two specific responses plotted as yellow diamonds, the saccadic compensation is unusually weak.
Figure 10B shows that in these two particular responses (also plotted as yellow diamonds), the vergence asymmetry is much larger than normal for this subject and it is vergence compensation that is primarily responsible for correcting the initial error in these responses.
Despite this variability, the eyes always arrive at the correct final position using saccadic and/or vergence compensation. Since there are two potential compensatory mechanisms, some process must exist to achieve balance between the two processes.
Krommenhoek and Van Gisbergen (
1994) have shown that both saccades and vergence components use internal (i.e., extra-retinal) feedback loops in their control. These internal feedback mechanisms may provide a possible mechanism for balancing between the two compensatory processes. Considering the accuracy of binocular fixation, a visually driven feedback path is probably involved in compensating for any residual binocular error. The factors that determine the dominant compensatory strategy and the mechanism that ultimately attains the highly accurate binocular fixation are the two most important questions to emerge from this work.
We have been referring to all slow version movements as vergence asymmetry primarily as a matter of verbal convenience. It is likely that early slow midline deviations are attributable to vergence asymmetry as the two eye movements rarely show identical vergence dynamics (
Horng et al., 1998). If one eye is faster, or has a shorter latency, the inequality will create a deviation from the midline. However, toward the end of the movement when dynamic vergence has decayed is it possible that the vergence asymmetry is due to some type of slow conjunctive movement: either smooth pursuit or another unknown slow corrective process? Recent evidence regarding the premotor organization cited above indicates that both vergence and saccades, including slow vergence are driven by neurons preferential to a specific eye. This suggests that the observed vergence asymmetry is due to monocular control components that drive the two eyes asymmetrically to achieve the final required binocular fixation. An intriguing final question is whether or not these motor signals are the result of monocular or binocular stimulus signals.