Influence of Time Pressure on Successive Visual Searches
Highlights
- Time pressure across two consecutive searches impairs search performance;
- First search completion consistently maintains priority, even under time pressure;
- When both search targets are found, the subsequent ones have longer fixations yet shorter saccades.
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Apparatus
2.3. Design
2.4. Materials
2.5. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Search Accuracy
3.2. Number of Fixations and Response Times
3.3. Fixation Durations
3.4. Saccade Amplitudes
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Eckstein, M.P. Visual Search: A Retrospective. J. Vis. 2011, 11, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Höfler, M.; Gilchrist, I.D.; Körner, C. Searching the Same Display Twice: Properties of Short-Term Memory in Repeated Search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2014, 76, 335–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horowitz, T.S.; Wolfe, J.M. Visual Search Has No Memory. Nature 1998, 394, 575–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Körner, C.; Gilchrist, I.D. Finding a New Target in an Old Display: Evidence for a Memory Recency Effect in Visual Search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2007, 14, 846–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawrence, D.H. Two Studies of Visual Search for Word Targets with Controlled Rates of Presentation*. Percept. Psychophys. 1971, 10, 85–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radhakrishnan, A.; Balakrishnan, M.; Behera, S.; Raghunandhan, R. Role of Reading Medium and Audio Distractors on Visual Search. J. Optom. 2022, 15, 299–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Höfler, M.; Hübel, B. Missing Targets in Multiple-Target Search. In Psychology Applications & Developments; inScience Press: Lisbon, Portugal, 2018; Volume III, pp. 247–256. [Google Scholar]
- Stankov, A.D.; Touryan, J.; Gordon, S.; Ries, A.J.; Ki, J.; Parra, L.C. During Natural Viewing, Neural Processing of Visual Targets Continues throughout Saccades. J. Vis. 2021, 21, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, H.; Zelinsky, G.J. Visual Search Is Guided to Categorically-Defined Targets. Vis. Res. 2009, 49, 2095–2103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfe, J. Visual Search: How Do We Find What We Are Looking For? Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2020, 6, 539–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfe, J.M.; Horowitz, T.S. Five Factors That Guide Attention in Visual Search. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017, 1, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bravo, M.J.; Nakayama, K. The Role of Attention in Different Visual-Search Tasks. Percept. Psychophys. 1992, 51, 465–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Franconeri, S.L.; Simons, D.J. Moving and Looming Stimuli Capture Attention. Percept. Psychophys. 2003, 65, 999–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfe, J.M.; Friedman-Hill, S.R.; Stewart, M.I.; O’Connell, K.M. The Role of Categorization in Visual Search for Orientation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1992, 18, 34–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Treisman, A.; Gormican, S. Feature Analysis in Early Vision: Evidence from Search Asymmetries. Psychol. Rev. 1988, 95, 15–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Julesz, B. Textons, the Elements of Texture Perception, and Their Interactions. Nature 1981, 290, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Treisman, A.M.; Gelade, G. A Feature-Integration Theory of Attention. Cogn. Psychol. 1980, 12, 97–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfe, J.M.; Cave, K.R.; Franzel, S.L. Guided Search: An Alternative to the Feature Integration Model for Visual Search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1989, 15, 419–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Julesz, B. A Brief Outline of the Texton Theory of Human Vision. Trends Neurosci. 1984, 7, 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fan, X.; Zhou, Q.; Xie, F.; Liu, Z. Effects of Time Pressure and Task Difficulty on Visual Search. In Proceedings of the Advances in Human Factors in Robots and Unmanned Systems; Savage-Knepshield, P., Chen, J., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 97–109. [Google Scholar]
- Yu, R.; Yang, L.; Guo, X.; Zhang, Y. Effect of Time Pressure on Dynamic Visual Search Performance. Procedia Manuf. 2015, 3, 4658–4664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, S.; Trafimow, D. Time Pressure Heuristics Can Improve Performance Due to Increased Consistency. J. Gen. Psychol. 2012, 139, 273–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Radović, T.; Rieger, T.; Manzey, D. A Global and Local Perspective of Interruption Frequency in a Visual Search Task. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 951048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCarley, J.S. Effects of Speed–Accuracy Instructions on Oculomotor Scanning and Target Recognition in a Simulated Baggage X-Ray Screening Task. Ergonomics 2009, 52, 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Herpen, E.; Trijp, H.C.M.V. Front-of-Pack Nutrition Labels. Their Effect on Attention and Choices When Consumers Have Varying Goals and Time Constraints. Appetite 2011, 57, 148–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rieger, T.; Manzey, D. Understanding the Impact of Time Pressure and Automation Support in a Visual Search Task. Hum. Factors 2022, 66, 001872082211112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cambronero-Delgadillo, A.J.; Nachtnebel, S.J.; Körner, C.; Gilchrist, I.D.; Höfler, M. Interruption in Visual Search: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1384441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lleras, A.; Rensink, R.A.; Enns, J.T. Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search: New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 684–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lleras, A.; Rensink, R.A.; Enns, J.T. Consequences of Display Changes during Interrupted Visual Search: Rapid Resumption Is Target Specific. Percept. Psychophys. 2007, 69, 980–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Zoest, W.; Lleras, A.; Kingstone, A.; Enns, J.T. In Sight, out of Mind: The Role of Eye Movements in the Rapid Resumption of Visual Search. Percept. Psychophys. 2007, 69, 1204–1217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Godwin, H.J.; Benson, V.; Drieghe, D. Using Interrupted Visual Displays to Explore the Capacity, Time Course, and Format of Fixation Plans during Visual Search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2013, 39, 1700–1712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomas, L.E.; Lleras, A. Inhibitory Tagging in an Interrupted Visual Search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 1241–1250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rieger, T.; Heilmann, L.; Manzey, D. Visual Search Behavior and Performance in Luggage Screening: Effects of Time Pressure, Automation Aid, and Target Expectancy. Cogn. Res. 2021, 6, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhou, Y.-B.; Ruan, S.-J.; Zhang, K.; Bao, Q.; Liu, H.-Z. Time Pressure Effects on Decision-Making in Intertemporal Loss Scenarios: An Eye-Tracking Study. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1451674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gerardin, P.; Nicolas, J.; Farnè, A.; Pélisson, D. Increasing Attentional Load Boosts Saccadic Adaptation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015, 56, 6304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical Power Analyses Using G*Power 3.1: Tests for Correlation and Regression Analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bouma, H. Interaction Effects in Parafoveal Letter Recognition. Nature 1970, 226, 177–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- The jamovi project Jamovi 2025.
- Cousineau, D. Confidence Intervals in Within-Subject Designs: A Simpler Solution to Loftus and Masson’s Method. TQMP 2005, 1, 42–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morey, R.D. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A Correction to Cousineau (2005). TQMP 2008, 4, 61–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bahill, A.T.; Clark, M.R.; Stark, L. The Main Sequence, a Tool for Studying Human Eye Movements. Math. Biosci. 1975, 24, 191–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lebedev, S.; Van Gelder, P.; Tsui, W.H. Square-Root Relations between Main Saccadic Parameters. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1996, 37, 2750–2758. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Pundlik, S.; Woods, R.; Luo, G. From Small to Large, All Saccades Follow the Same Timeline. J. Vis. 2015, 15, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, D.; Abel, L.A.; Dell’Osso, L.F.; Daroff, R.B. Saccadic Velocity Characteristics: Intrinsic Variability and Fatigue. Aviat. Space Env. Med. 1979, 50, 393–395. [Google Scholar]
- Bollen, E.; Bax, J.; van Dijk, J.G.; Koning, M.; Bos, J.E.; Kramer, C.G.; van der Velde, E.A. Variability of the Main Sequence. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1993, 34, 3700–3704. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Guadron, L.; Van Opstal, A.J.; Goossens, J. Speed-Accuracy Tradeoffs Influence the Main Sequence of Saccadic Eye Movements. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 5262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Di Stasi, L.L.; Renner, R.; Staehr, P.; Helmert, J.R.; Velichkovsky, B.M.; Cañas, J.J.; Catena, A.; Pannasch, S. Saccadic Peak Velocity Sensitivity to Variations in Mental Workload. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2010, 81, 413–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shapiro, K.L.; Raymond, J.E.; Arnell, K.M. The Attentional Blink. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1997, 1, 291–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dux, P.E.; Marois, R. The Attentional Blink: A Review of Data and Theory. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 1683–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adamo, S.H.; Cain, M.S.; Mitroff, S.R. Self-Induced Attentional Blink: A Cause of Errors in Multiple-Target Search. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2569–2574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sigman, M.; Gilbert, C.D. Learning to Find a Shape. Nat. Neurosci. 2000, 3, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hu, J.; Chen, Q.; Lu, D.; He, J. The Impact of Clock Timing on VDT Visual Search Performance under Time Constraint. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1369920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ernst, D.; Wolfe, J.M. How Fixation Durations Are Affected by Search Difficulty Manipulations. Vis. Cogn. 2022, 30, 339–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Mühlenen, A.; Müller, H.J.; Müller, D. Sit-and-Wait Strategies in Dynamic Visual Search. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 309–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolfe, J.M. When Do I Quit? The Search Termination Problem in Visual Search. In The Influence of Attention, Learning, and Motivation on Visual Search; Dodd, M.D., Flowers, J.H., Eds.; Nebraska Symposium on Motivation; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 183–208. ISBN 978-1-4614-4793-1. [Google Scholar]
- Meiran, N. Task Switching: Mechanisms Underlying Rigid vs. Flexible Self-Control. In Self Control in Society, Mind, and Brain; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 202–220. ISBN 978-0-19-539138-1. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe, J.; Cain, M.; Aizenman, A. Guidance and Selection History in Hybrid Foraging Visual Search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2019, 81, 637–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liston, D.B.; Stone, L.S. Effects of Prior Information and Reward on Oculomotor and Perceptual Choices. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 13866–13875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, J.; Gong, X.; Fougnie, D.; Wolfe, J.M. How Humans React to Changing Rewards during Visual Foraging. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2017, 79, 2299–2309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wagner, J.; Zurlo, A.; Rusconi, E. Individual Differences in Visual Search: A Systematic Review of the Link between Visual Search Performance and Traits or Abilities. Cortex 2024, 178, 51–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robson, S.G.; Tangen, J.M.; Searston, R.A. The Effect of Expertise, Target Usefulness and Image Structure on Visual Search. Cogn. Res. 2021, 6, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Cambronero-Delgadillo, A.J.; Körner, C.; Gilchrist, I.D.; Höfler, M. Influence of Time Pressure on Successive Visual Searches. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2025, 18, 31. https://doi.org/10.3390/jemr18040031
Cambronero-Delgadillo AJ, Körner C, Gilchrist ID, Höfler M. Influence of Time Pressure on Successive Visual Searches. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2025; 18(4):31. https://doi.org/10.3390/jemr18040031
Chicago/Turabian StyleCambronero-Delgadillo, Alejandro J., Christof Körner, Iain D. Gilchrist, and Margit Höfler. 2025. "Influence of Time Pressure on Successive Visual Searches" Journal of Eye Movement Research 18, no. 4: 31. https://doi.org/10.3390/jemr18040031
APA StyleCambronero-Delgadillo, A. J., Körner, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Höfler, M. (2025). Influence of Time Pressure on Successive Visual Searches. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 18(4), 31. https://doi.org/10.3390/jemr18040031