Next Article in Journal
Eye Tracking in Virtual Reality: Vive Pro Eye Spatial Accuracy, Precision, and Calibration Reliability
Previous Article in Journal
Introduction to the Special Thematic Issue “Virtual Reality and Eye Tracking”
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Maintaining Fixation by Children in a Virtual Reality Version of Pupil Perimetry

by
Brendan L. Portengen
1,2,
Marnix Naber
2,
Demi Jansen
2,
Carlijn van den Boomen
2,
Saskia M. Imhof
1 and
Giorgio L. Porro
1
1
Department of Ophthalmology, University Medical Center Utrecht, The Netherlands
2
Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2022, 15(3), 1-10; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.3.2
Submission received: 21 January 2022 / Published: 19 September 2022

Abstract

:
The assessment of the visual field in young children continues to be a challenge. Children often do not sit still, fail to fixate stimuli for longer durations, and have limited verbal capacity to report visibility. Therefore, we introduced a head-mounted VR display with gazecontingent flicker pupil perimetry (VRgcFPP). We presented large flickering patches at different eccentricities and angles in the periphery to evoke pupillary oscillations, and three fixation stimulus conditions to determine best practices for optimal fixation and pupil response quality. A total of twenty children (3-11y) passively fixated a dot, counted the repeated appearance of an animated character (counting task), and watched an animated movie in separate trials of 80s each (20 patch locations, 4s per location). The results showed that gaze precision and accuracy did not differ significantly across the fixation conditions but pupil amplitudes were strongest for the dot and count task. The VR set-up appears to be an ideal apparatus for children to allow free range of movement, an engaging visual task, and reliable eye measurements. We recommend the use of the fixation counting task for pupil perimetry because children enjoyed it the most and it achieved strongest pupil responses.

Introduction

To this day visual field assessment in children remains challenging due to certain characteristics of standard automated perimetry (SAP; e.g., Humphrey Field Analyzer, Octopus perimeter). These include the task’s subjectiveness, the requirement of fixation on a target, uncontrollable learning effects, and the need for prolonged attention. Due to these disadvantages, perimetry tests performed with young children and patients that suffer from cortical damage tend to produce unreliable results (Morales & Brown, 2001; Patel et al., 2015; Tschopp et al., 1998).
Pupil perimetry was developed as an objective alternative to SAP, using the pupillary response to light stimuli across the visual field as a measure of visual sensitivity (Kardon, 1992; Tan et al., 2001; Wilhelm et al., 2000). Conventional pupil perimetry set-ups consist of a monitor and a stand-alone eye tracker. Pupil perimetry has not yet been performed in children even though it circumvents most of the aforementioned challenges in evaluating the visual field with SAP (i.e., subjectiveness and the need for fixation on a target). The reason why pupil perimetry has not yet been applied in children may stem from the remaining requirement to stay seated while fixed in a foreheadchinrest.
Here we propose a novel implementation of gaze-contingent flicker pupil perimetry (Naber et al., 2018) through the use of a head-mounted device (HMD) with virtual reality (VR) technology (VRgcFPP). VR applications in the ophthalmologic practice are relatively new, but promising (Alawa et al., 2021; Deiner et al., 2020; He et al., 2019; Mees et al., 2020; Razeghinejad et al., 2021; Tsapakis et al., 2017, 2018). Particularly, VR allows for freedom of head movement, a child-friendly and engaging environment, and eye measurements using a built-in eye tracker. Eye trackers used for pupil perimetry mostly consist of sophisticated and expensive solutions, such as the Eyelink 1000 Plus (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) or the Tobii Pro Spectrum (Tobii, Danderyd, Sweden), but recent developments now allow high-quality eye-tracking with a HMD.
Also, the immersive environment that VR provides introduces new possibilities to engage children during assessments. Increased attention has been shown to evoke stronger pupillary responses to stimuli (Binda et al., 2013; Mathôt et al., 2013; Naber & Nakayama, 2013) which in turn increases discriminative power (Portengen et al., 2021). Here we questioned how it can be ensured that children show sustained attention for the visual stimuli in a VR environment. An instruction to keep attention will not suffice for young and/or neurologically impaired children. To maintain fixation, a simple fixation point will not be interesting enough to look at, but a fixation object of a type that is too distracting might lead to unwanted pupillary reactions (i.e., noise in the pupil response data). All these aspects could thus hypothetically lead to decreased quality of measurements, denoting its importance to find a balance between increased attention towards fixation and maintaining a good signal-to-noise ratio in the pupillary measurements.

Aims

In summary, the aim of this study is to explore whether visual field examination using a virtual reality version of pupil perimetry (VRgcFPP) provides strong pupil responses in children, and what fixation task is best suited for them and what fixation task provides the most reliable results.

Methods

Participants

The participants consisted of 20 healthy children aged 3 to 11 years old (mean age and SD 7.2 ± 2.4, 14 male). The sample size was similar to prior studies in the field (Alawa et al., 2021; Kelbsch et al., 2020; Neumayr et al., 2020; Portengen et al., 2021). All children had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and no history of visual or neurological disorders. Participants were not tested for visual acuity, but parents were questioned about any signs of visual problems to ensure that vision of the child was good (for details, see procedure). The experiment was approved by the local ethical committee of Utrecht University (approval number FETC19-006) and conformed to the ethical considerations of the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants gave written informed consent together with their caretakers prior to participation. Both participants and caretakers were clearly instructed of their right to withdraw consent and informed that the experiment could be halted prematurely. Researchers observed the child during the experiment for any sign of reluctance or distress, after which the experiment would immediately be ended. Lastly, they received (financial) reimbursement (€8,per hour) and a phone-based VR headset for participation.

Apparatus

The tests were conducted either in the laboratory or at the residence of the participants. A BTO 17W1090 laptop (BTO, IJsselstein, The Netherlands) with Windows 10 operating system (Microsoft, Redmond, Washington) was used to run the test. The VR environment was built with Unity software (version 2019.4; Unity Technologies, San Francisco, CA, USA). Connected to the laptop was an HTC (HTC Corporation, Taoyuan, Taiwan) Vive Pro Eye VR headset. It consisted of dual 3.5-inch OLED screens with a resolution of 1440 × 1600 pixels per screen and a refresh rate of 90 Hz to display stimuli. Pupil diameter and gaze were recorded with the built-in Tobii eye tracker (Tobii, Danderyd, Sweden; 90 Hz sampling rate, 0.5-1.1-degree accuracy of gaze angle) and the VIVE SRanipal Runtime and SDK. Adjustment of the HMD and eye tracker calibration (5-point grid) took ~1 min. Two base stations at opposite positions located real-time head position with SteamVR Tracking 2.0. Stimulus properties (i.e., fixation target, frequency, location, size, and order) were inputted with Python software (version 3.7; xml.etree.cElementTree and numpy packages; Python Software Foundation, https://www.python.org/).

Fixation target conditions

The three fixation target conditions used in this study consisted of the presentation of (i) a simple red fixation dot, similar to fixation targets used in standard automated perimetry and conventional pupil perimetry (Figure 1A), (ii) an animated child-friendly video of an archeologist in Egypt (chosen for its relatively low luminance, color and spatial contrast; adopted from https://youtu.be/j6PbonHsqW0) with muted sound (Figure 1B), and (iii) an engaging counting task in which participants were asked to count the appearances of an animated character at fixation (Pikachu; Pokémon, The Pokémon Company, Minato, Tokyo, Japan, see Figure 1C). This character appeared 14 times within the 80 second trial at varying intervals. All fixation targets were placed on a fixed location within the VR environment independent of head or gaze position. To prevent large saccades in reaction to the fixation target conditions, the three fixation targets were made small by placing them at a simulated distance of 16 m.

Environment & stimuli

A dark blue (30% luminance for optimal luminance and color contrast; Portengen et al., submitted) background served as the VR environment. To reduce Simulator Sickness (due to sensory mismatch; Reason, 1978) a sense of depth was simulated through a virtual red platform upon which the participants were placed, and thin dome-like lines. Stimuli were superimposed on the background. The stimuli consisted of black-yellow flickering wedges presented across 20 stimulus locations in randomized order within the inner 60 degrees field of vision and positioned around one of three fixation targets with a simulated distance of 10 m, see Figure 1. Note that the inner 16 degrees of the visual field were not stimulated to allow for the fixation conditions to be visible. The black-yellow wedges flickered for 4 seconds (i.e., to collect sufficient pupil data but also keep the experiment relatively short) at a 2 Hz rate and were superimposed on a complementary dark blue background (Figure 1E). The 2 Hz flicker frequency is the optimal balance between enough number of evoked pupil responses in a relatively short time window and strong enough pupil responses that can be picked up reliably by the eye tracker (Portengen et al., in prep). The gaze-contingent stimulus presentation (i.e., the eye tracking software follows the subject’s direction of gaze fixation and updates the position of the flickering stimuli realtime to reflect changes in direction of gaze; Figure 1F) ensured accurate retinotopic stimulation despite the presence of saccades (Naber et al., 2018).

Procedure

After the informed consent procedure, children and their caretakers completed a demographic questionnaire to ensure no neurologic, ophthalmologic or attentional disorders were present. Upon completion participants were seated on a chair in the center of the room, where the VR HMD was fitted to the child’s head (Figure 1D). A short adjustment period (~1 min) followed after this. Here the child could look around the VR environment; young children were made aware of the red platform underneath them: “Stay seated, because the floor is lava!”. Aside from using this joke as a way to make the children feel more comfortable, the platform also created an extra sense of depth in the otherwise “empty” VR environment. After calibration with a 5-point calibration grid, the experiment started. This consisted of three blocks, each with a fixation target, a 5-second adjustment period and flickering stimuli at 20 locations across the visual field. The children were instructed to fixate their gaze at the fixation target in the middle of the environment. The younger children were encouraged to fixate the center of the screen by verbally expressing the following instructions; (i) during the fixation dot condition, the experimenters reminded a child to keep looking at the dot when its gaze strayed from it, (ii) for the animation video, the experimenters occasionally asked the participating child what was going on in the video, and lastly (iii) children were positively reinforced whenever they counted the appearance of a Pikachu during the counting task condition. The experiment lasted for 240 seconds (3 fixation target blocks * 20 stimulus locations * 4 second stimulus duration). The child could take a break between each block. Total experiment duration, including all trials, breaks, and (re)calibration was on average 15 minutes. Pupils were measured binocularly to estimate convergence and thus focus of depth in the VR environment. The dual OLED screens allowed a sense of depth in the VR environment to prevent VR induced Simulator Sickness.

Analysis

First stimulus location onsets functioned as start events for the event-related analysis of the continuous pupil output of the integrated eye tracker. From the pupil data blink episodes were detected and removed using an automated detection blink method by looking for crossings of a speed threshold of 4 standard deviations (SD) above the mean. The removed blink epochs were interpolated with a cubic method. Next, pupil data were baseline-corrected to enhance inter-subject comparability. A high-pass Butterworth filter (3rd order, 1 Hz cut-off frequency) and a lowpass filter (3rd order, 10 Hz cut-off frequency) followed to remove slow pupil diameter changes and high-frequency noise, respectively. Pupil traces per stimulus location were converted to power values in the frequency domain using a fast Fourier transform. The power at 2 Hz reflected the pupil oscillation amplitude and served as the main dependent variable. Furthermore, we were interested in how well each stimulus fixation paradigm retained a child’s attention. For this we calculated gaze distance from the fixation target. Distance means and SDs of saccades across fixation conditions were compared. One-way repeated measures ANOVA and paired double-sided t-tests (post-hoc tests) determined statistical significance of pupil amplitudes, fixation accuracy, and fixation precision between fixation conditions. All analyses were performed using Python software (version 3.7; Python Software Foundation, https://www.python.org/). The raincloud plot was created using software developed by (Allen et al., 2021).

Results

In our study, we set out to investigate whether visual field examination using a virtual reality version of pupil perimetry is feasible and which fixation target condition evoked strongest pupil responses. To do this, pupil data were analyzed to inspect adequate pupil responses to the 2 Hz stimulation. Figure 2A shows the 2 Hz oscillatory pattern of the pupil traces, averaged across all children, reflecting the stimulus onand offsets (see Supplementary Figure S1 for separate plots with 95% confidence intervals).
Next, pupil oscillation powers were compared between fixation target conditions (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; F(2,38) = 3.87, p = .030, partial η2 = 0.17). Post-hoc comparisons demonstrated stronger pupil powers of the fixation dot condition (t(19) = 3.05, p = .007) and the counting task condition (t(19) = 2.12, p = .047) when compared to the video fixation target. There existed, however, no statistical difference between fixation dot and counting task (t(19) = 0.73, p = .470). See Figure S2-4 in the Supplementary Materials for the average pupil traces, the average pupil oscillation powers and the pupil oscillation power spectra per stimulus location across fixation target conditions per participant.
Furthermore, we explored which fixation target ensured best fixation behavior. The fixation error from fixation target center (i.e., gaze distance during fixation loss) provided an indication of interest and attention, see Figure 3. A one-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed that gaze accuracy (i.e., mean gaze deviation from fixation center; F(2,38) = 2.20, p = .120) and gaze precision (i.e., standard deviation of gaze deviation; F(2,38) = 1.18, p = .320) did not differ significantly across fixation target condition. These results imply that the fixation target conditions did not influence fixation error in the children studied.
After every experiment the investigators queried which fixation target conditions participants enjoyed the most. Almost all (18 out of 20 children) preferred the counting task. The two oldest participants (≥10 years old) favored the fixation dot, likely because the Pokémon character and video targeted matched best with the interest of children younger than 10.

Discussion

In our study, we set out to investigate whether visual field examination using a virtual reality version of pupil perimetry (VRgcFPP), is feasible in children by testing whether strong pupil responses could be evoked. Moreover, the secondary objectives of this study were to investigate (i) what fixation task is best suited for children, and (ii) which fixation target best captured a child’s attention. The fixation dot and counting task conditions provided strongest pupil responses. One possible explanation for the weaker pupil responses during the animated video fixation target task is the lack of covert attention for the flickering stimuli (Mathôt et al., 2013; Mathôt & Van der Stigchel, 2015; Naber et al., 2013; Portengen et al., 2021).
In addition to attention, the pupil also responds to luminance contrast (Ukai, 1985), color hue (Gamlin et al., 1998; Kelbsch et al., 2019; Tsujimura et al., 2006; Walkey et al., 2005), and spatial frequency (Barbur et al., 1992; Maeda et al., 2017; Ukai, 1985). The video’s higher luminance and spatial contrast in comparison to the other two fixation targets could have interfered with the luminance and color contrast between stimulus and background. Interestingly, one participant (S2; see Figure S1) showed higher oscillation power. This participant (aged 7) was hypermetropic and his positive diopter lenses probably enlarged the stimuli resulting in stronger stimulation of the pupil. Elimination of this outlier did not alter the results.
Fixation dot and counting task conditions did not differ in pupil response amplitudes. However, all children seemed to enjoy the counting task the most. Although pupil perimetry is an objective testing method, higher intrinsic motivation and attention seem to result in stronger pupil responses (Binda et al., 2013; Binda & Murray, 2015; Mathôt & Van der Stigchel, 2015; Naber et al., 2013; Portengen et al., 2021). Attention was drawn away only a couple times to the appearing Pikachu in the counting task, meaning that attention was still relatively often at the flickering stimuli, leading to strong pupil responses. On the contrary, attention was almost continuously drawn away from the flickering stimuli towards the central stimuli in the video condition, explaining the weaker pupil oscillations. Thus, providing an engaging and more enjoyable task during a diagnostic visual field test (e.g., a counting or object finding task) is a preferred method for young children. For this reason, some alternatives to SAP have already been introduced (Miranda et al., 2016; Murray et al., 2018; Porro, Hofmann, et al., 1998). The Behavioural Visual Field screening (BEFIE) test (Koenraads et al., 2015) and SVOP (Murray et al., 2018) are examples of visual field tests, specifically developed with very young and neurologically impaired children in mind, that are tolerated better than conventional SAP methods. To illustrate, the BEFIE test managed to shorten time-to-diagnosis of visual field defects substantially in children suffering from brain disease (Portengen et al., 2020) whereas SAP methods are generally performed unreliably in young children due to inability to cooperate, lack of comprehension, and psychomotor impairment (Neumayr et al., 2020; Patel et al., 2015; Porro, Dekker, et al., 1998; Tschopp et al., 1998). Despite these efforts with subjective and/or confrontational and behavioral perimetry tests, pupil perimetry in VR may even enhance the reliability as well. Additionally, this study showed VRgcFPP is applicable in children as young as three years old, filling a clinical gap where reliable visual field testing up until now was extremely difficult.
Our novel virtual reality implementation of pupil perimetry successfully evoked pupil responses comparable to responses found in previous studies with adults (Naber et al., 2018; Portengen et al., 2021). Gaze-contingent flicker pupil perimetry, as well as other variations of pupil perimetry (Kelbsch et al., 2021; Rosli et al., 2018), proved to objectively measure visual field defects. Our results support the application of a virtual reality version of pupil perimetry in children both in a busy clinical setting, and in a telemedicine setting, or even at familiar places for the child, such as home or school. The experimenters experienced no difficulties when conducting the experiment at the participants’ residence. Indeed, various VR-based perimetry methods using inexpensive or smartphone-based VR HMDs have recently been studied with telemedicine in mind (Alawa et al., 2021; Deiner et al., 2020; Tsapakis et al., 2017, 2018). Some feature subjective active report tasks comparable to SAP (Mees et al., 2020; Razeghinejad et al., 2021; Tsapakis et al., 2018) and others apply eye tracking to objectively measure looking responses (He et al., 2019; Wroblewski et al., 2014) in order to assess the visual field. None, however, harnessed the objective pupillary responses to light stimuli like in pupil perimetry.
Gaze distance from fixation target was studied to investigate whether any of the fixation targets captured the child’s gaze best. Some of the older children (aged approximately 8 years or older) were more capable of inhibiting saccades during fixation. Children under the age of 6 experienced more trouble maintaining fixation; they seemed to lose interest in the fixation dot earlier than the older children. However, this conclusion is merely based on the qualitative inspection of the data and the sample size was too small to statistically differentiate between age groups.
A limitation to the current study comprises of the lack of assessment of diagnostic accuracy of the VRgcFPP method with respect to detecting scotomas as all children tested did not suffer from visual field defects. Next to that, the eye tracker used in the current HMD is of inferior quality when compared to eye trackers used in standard pupil perimetry (e.g., Eyelink 1000 or Tobii Pro Spectrum; Sipatchin et al., 2021). It is unclear whether the lower quality has impact on the intended use. We did find clear changes in pupil diameter in response to the flickering stimuli (Figure 2B), suggesting that, in line with previous work on gaze-contingent flicker pupil perimetry (Naber et al., 2018; Portengen et al., 2021), the apparatus offers the opportunity to measure differences in sensitivities across the visual field in patients and healthy observers; future experiments with pediatric and adult patients suffering from visual field loss and comparative studies between more expensive eye tracking systems and the VR system used in this study might help shed some light on questions about diagnostic accuracy and applicability of eye-tracking in VR. Since the VR apparatus is an off-the-shelf device, it could not be modified to the smaller head sizes of young children. This resulted in suboptimal calibration and relatively smaller pupil powers in our youngest participants (see S1, S15, and S18 in the Supplementary Figure S1).
To conclude, our results support the application of this virtual reality version of pupil perimetry (VRgcFPP) for binocularly testing the visual field of children in a busy clinical setting. The VR set-up appears to be an ideal apparatus for children to allow free range of movement, an engaging visual task, and reliable eye measurements. A fixation counting task is recommended for use of pupil perimetry in young children as they enjoyed it the most and it achieved pupil responses as strong as the generally used fixation dot.

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://bib-lio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from UitZicht (grant 2017-18, funds involved: the ODAS foundation [grant number 2017-03]; the Rotterdamse Stichting Blindenbelangen [grant number B20170004]; and the F.P. Fischer Foundation [grant number 170511]), and a grant from the Janivo Foundation [grant number 2017170]. M.N. is supported by a grant from UitZicht (grant 2018-10, fund involved: Rotterdamse Stichting Blindenbelangen).

References

  1. Alawa, K. A., R. P. Nolan, E. Han, A. Arboleda, H. Durkee, M. S. Sayed, M. C. Aguilar, and R. K. Lee. 2021. Low-cost, smartphone-based frequency doubling technology visual field testing using a head-mounted display. British Journal of Ophthalmology 105, 3: 440–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Allen, M., D. Poggiali, K. Whitaker, T. R. Marshall, J. van Langen, and R. A. Kievit. 2021. Raincloud plots: a multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Research 4: 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barbur, Harlow, and Sahraie. 1992. Pupillary responses to stimulus structure, colour and movement. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 12, 2: 137–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Binda, P., and S. O. Murray. 2015. Spatial attention increases the pupillary response to light changes. Journal of Vision 15, 2: 1–1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Binda, P., M. Pereverzeva, and S. O. Murray. 2013. Attention to bright surfaces enhances the pupillary light reflex. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 5: 2199–2204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Deiner, M. S., B. E. Damato, and Y. Ou. 2020. Implementing and Monitoring At-Home Virtual Reality Oculo-kinetic Perimetry During COVID-19. In Ophthalmology. Elsevier: Vol. 127, Issue 9, p. 1258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gamlin, P. D. R., H. Zhang, A. Harlow, and J. L. Barbur. 1998. Pupil responses to stimulus color, structure and light flux increments in the rhesus monkey. Vision Research 38, 21: 3353–3358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. He, J., S. Zhang, P. Wu, Y. Zhang, X. Zheng, and L. Zhou. 2019. A Novel Virtual Reality Design of Portable Automatic Perimetry. IEEE MTT-S 2019 International Microwave Biomedical Conference, IMBioC 2019 - Proceedings, May 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kardon, R. H. 1992. Pupil perimetry. In Current Opinion in Ophthalmology. Vol. 3, Issue 5, pp. 565–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kelbsch, C., J. Lange, H. Wilhelm, B. Wilhelm, T. Peters, M. Kempf, L. Kuehlewein, and K. Stingl. 2020. Chromatic pupil campimetry reveals functional defects in exudative age-related macular degeneration with differences related to disease activity. Translational Vision Science and Technology 9, 6: 5–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kelbsch, C., K. Stingl, R. Jung, M. Kempf, P. Richter, T. Strasser, T. Peters, B. Wilhelm, H. Wilhelm, and F. Tonagel. 2021. How lesions at different locations along the visual pathway influence pupillary reactions to chromatic stimuli. Graefe’s Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 1: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kelbsch, C., K. Stingl, M. Kempf, T. Strasser, R. Jung, L. Kuehlewein, H. Wilhelm, T. Peters, B. Wilhelm, and K. Stingl. 2019. Objective measurement of local rod and cone function using gaze-controlled chromatic pupil campimetry in healthy subjects. Translational Vision Science and Technology 8, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Koenraads, Y., K. P. J. Braun, D. C. P. van der Linden, S. M. Imhof, and G. L. Porro. 2015. Perimetry in young and neurologically impaired children: the Behavioral Visual Field (BEFIE) Screening Test revisited. JAMA Ophthalmology 133, 3: 319–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Maeda, F., C. Kelbsch, T. Straßer, K. Skorkovská, T. Peters, B. Wilhelm, and H. Wilhelm. 2017. Chromatic pupillography in hemianopia patients with homonymous visual field defects. Graefe’s Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 255, 9: 1837–1842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mathôt, S., L. van der Linden, J. Grainger, and F. Vitu. 2013. The pupillary light response reveals the focus of covert visual attention. PloS One 8, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Mathôt, S., and S. Van der Stigchel. 2015. New Light on the Mind’s Eye: The Pupillary Light Response as Active Vision. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24, 5: 374–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mees, L., S. Upadhyaya, P. Kumar, S. Kotawala, S. Haran, S. Rajasekar, D. S. Friedman, and R. Venkatesh. 2020. Validation of a Headmounted Virtual Reality Visual Field Screening Device. Journal of Glaucoma 29, 2: 86–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Miranda, M. A., D. B. Henson, C. Fenerty, S. Biswas, and T. Aslam. 2016. Development of a pediatric visual field test. Translational Vision Science and Technology 5, 6: 13–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Morales, J., and S. M. Brown. 2001. The feasibility of short automated static perimetry in children. Ophthalmology 108, 1: 157–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Murray, I. C., C. Schmoll, A. Perperidis, H. M. Brash, A. D. McTrusty, L. A. Cameron, A. G. Wilkinson, A. O. Mulvihill, B. W. Fleck, and R. A. Minns. 2018. Detection and characterisation of visual field defects using Saccadic Vector Optokinetic Perimetry in children with brain tumours. Eye 32, 10: 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Naber, M., G. A. Alvarez, and K. Nakayama. 2013. Tracking the allocation of attention using human pupillary oscillations. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Naber, M., and K. Nakayama. 2013. Pupil responses to high-level image content. Journal of Vision 13, 6: 7–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Naber, M., C. Roelofzen, A. Fracasso, D. P. Bergsma, M. van Genderen, G. L. Porro, S. O. Dumoulin, and Y. T. van der Schouw. 2018. Gaze-Contingent Flicker Pupil Perimetry Detects Scotomas in Patients With Cerebral Visual Impairments or Glaucoma. Frontiers in Neurology 9, July: 558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Neumayr, L., T. Pieper, M. Kudernatsch, S. TrauzettelKlosinski, and M. Staudt. 2020. Uncovering homonymous visual field defects in candidates for pediatric epilepsy surgery. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology 25: 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Patel, D. E., P. M. Cumberland, B. C. Walters, I. RussellEggitt, J. S. Rahi, and O. study. OPTIC study group. 2015. Study of Optimal Perimetric Testing in Children (OPTIC): Feasibility, Reliability and Repeatability of Perimetry in Children. PloS One 10, 6: e0130895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Porro, G., E. M. Dekker, O. Van Nieuwenhuizen, D. Wittebol-Post, M. B. H. Schilder, A. J. F. SchenkRootlieb, and W. F. Treffers. 1998. Visual behaviours of neurologically impaired children with cerebral visual impairment: An ethological study. British Journal of Ophthalmology 82, 11: 1231–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Porro, G., J. Hofmann, D. Wittebol-Post, O. Van Nieuwenhuizen, Y. T. Van Der Schouw, M. B. H. H. Schilder, M. E. M. M. Dekker, and W. F. Treffers. 1998. A new behavioral visual field test for clinical use in pediatric neuroophthalmology. Neuro-Ophthalmology 19, 4: 205–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Portengen, B. L., Y. Koenraads, S. M. Imhof, and G. L. Porro. 2020. Lessons Learned from 23 Years of Experience in Testing Visual Fields of Neurologically Impaired Children. NeuroOphthalmology 44, 6: 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Portengen, B. L., C. Roelofzen, G. L. Porro, S. M. Imhof, A. Fracasso, and M. Naber. 2021. Blind spot and visual field anisotropy detection with flicker pupil perimetry across brightness and task variations. Vision Research 178, October 2020: 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Razeghinejad, R., A. Gonzalez-Garcia, J. S. Myers, and L. J. Katz. 2021. Preliminary Report on a Novel Virtual Reality Perimeter Compared with Standard Automated Perimetry. Journal of Glaucoma 30, 1: 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Reason, J. T. 1978. Motion sickness adaptation: A neural mismatch model. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 71, 11: 819–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Rosli, Y., C. F. Carle, Y. Ho, A. C. James, M. Kolic, E. M. F. Rohan, and T. Maddess. 2018. Retinotopic effects of visual attention revealed by dichoptic multifocal pupillography. Scientific Reports 8, 1: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sipatchin, A., S. Wahl, and K. Rifai. 2021. Eye-tracking for clinical ophthalmology with virtual reality (Vr): A case study of the htc vive pro eye’s usability. Healthcare (Switzerland) 9, 2: 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Tan, L., M. Kondo, M. Sato, N. Kondo, and Y. Miyake. 2001. Multifocal pupillary light response fields in normal subjects and patients with visual field defects. Vision Research 41, 8: 1073–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Tsapakis, S., D. Papaconstantinou, A. Diagourtas, K. Droutsas, K. Andreanos, M. M. Moschos, and D. Brouzas. 2017. Visual field examination method using virtual reality glasses compared with the humphrey perimeter. Clinical Ophthalmology 11: 1431–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Tsapakis, S., D. Papaconstantinou, A. Diagourtas, S. Kandarakis, K. Droutsas, K. Andreanos, and D. Brouzas. 2018. Home-based visual field test for glaucoma screening comparison with Humphrey perimeter. Clinical Ophthalmology 12: 2597–2606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tschopp, C., A. B. Safran, P. Viviani, A. Bullinger, M. Reicherts, and C. Mermoud. 1998. Automated visual field examination in children aged 5–8 years: Part I: Experimental validation of a testing procedure. Vision Research 38, 14: 2203–2210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Tsujimura, S. I., J. S. Wolffsohn, and B. Gilmartin. 2006. Pupil response to color signals in cone-contrast space. Current Eye Research 31, 5: 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Ukai, K. 1985. Spatial pattern as a stimulus to the pupillary system. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 2, 7: 1094–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Walkey, H. C., A. Hurden, I. R. Moorhead, J. A. F. Taylor, J. L. Barbur, and J. A. Harlow. 2005. Effective contrast of colored stimuli in the mesopic range: a metric for perceived contrast based on achromatic luminance contrast. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 22, 1: 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Wilhelm, H., J. Neitzel, B. Wilhelm, S. Beuel, H. Lüdtke, U. Kretschmann, and E. Zrenner. 2000. Pupil Perimetry using M-Sequence Stimulation Technique. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 41, 5: 1229–1238. [Google Scholar]
  42. Wroblewski, D., B. A. Francis, A. Sadun, G. Vakili, and V. Chopra. 2014. Testing of visual field with virtual reality goggles in manual and visual grasp modes. BioMed Research International, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The three fixation target conditions used in this study; a red fixation point (A), an animation video (B), and a counting task (C). Children were seated in a chair where the headset was positioned. A picture of a participant at home (6 years old) is shown in (D). All fixation targets were displayed at a fixed position in the middle of a dark blue virtual reality environment. The 2 Hz flickering yellow-and black stimuli consecutively appeared at the 20 stimulus locations (E). To ensure accurate retinotopic stimulation stimuli were presented in a gaze-contingent manner (F), i.e., online correction of stimulus locations for saccades from fixation target. Note that thin white lines were added to the background to create a sense of depth in the virtual reality environment. Note that the green gaze position cross was not shown during the experiment but is here shown to illustrate the gaze-contingent presentation paradigm.
Figure 1. The three fixation target conditions used in this study; a red fixation point (A), an animation video (B), and a counting task (C). Children were seated in a chair where the headset was positioned. A picture of a participant at home (6 years old) is shown in (D). All fixation targets were displayed at a fixed position in the middle of a dark blue virtual reality environment. The 2 Hz flickering yellow-and black stimuli consecutively appeared at the 20 stimulus locations (E). To ensure accurate retinotopic stimulation stimuli were presented in a gaze-contingent manner (F), i.e., online correction of stimulus locations for saccades from fixation target. Note that thin white lines were added to the background to create a sense of depth in the virtual reality environment. Note that the green gaze position cross was not shown during the experiment but is here shown to illustrate the gaze-contingent presentation paradigm.
Jemr 15 00014 g001
Figure 2. Relative pupil diameter over time for all subjects across fixation target conditions are shown in (A). Pupil traces are averaged across stimulus locations and participants. A raincloud plot depicting the average pupil oscillation powers per fixation target condition are plotted in (B) where the video fixation task (left) is red, the counting task (middle) is blue, and the fixation dot task (right) is green. Individual participants and their age (in years) and test location (i.e., lab or at home (n = 4)) are plotted across fixation conditions. The results show no distinct differences in patterns across age groups or test locations. The fixation dot and counting task conditions provided significantly larger pupil powers than the video fixation target (* = p < .05, ** = p < .01).
Figure 2. Relative pupil diameter over time for all subjects across fixation target conditions are shown in (A). Pupil traces are averaged across stimulus locations and participants. A raincloud plot depicting the average pupil oscillation powers per fixation target condition are plotted in (B) where the video fixation task (left) is red, the counting task (middle) is blue, and the fixation dot task (right) is green. Individual participants and their age (in years) and test location (i.e., lab or at home (n = 4)) are plotted across fixation conditions. The results show no distinct differences in patterns across age groups or test locations. The fixation dot and counting task conditions provided significantly larger pupil powers than the video fixation target (* = p < .05, ** = p < .01).
Jemr 15 00014 g002
Figure 3. Gaze accuracy and precision of participants; accuracy is defined as the mean gaze deviation from fixation target and precision as the standard deviation of this gaze deviation (A). The gaze accuracy (B) and precision (C) did not significantly differ across fixation target conditions.
Figure 3. Gaze accuracy and precision of participants; accuracy is defined as the mean gaze deviation from fixation target and precision as the standard deviation of this gaze deviation (A). The gaze accuracy (B) and precision (C) did not significantly differ across fixation target conditions.
Jemr 15 00014 g003
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Portengen, B.L.; Naber, M.; Jansen, D.; van den Boomen, C.; Imhof, S.M.; Porro, G.L. Maintaining Fixation by Children in a Virtual Reality Version of Pupil Perimetry. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2022, 15, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.3.2

AMA Style

Portengen BL, Naber M, Jansen D, van den Boomen C, Imhof SM, Porro GL. Maintaining Fixation by Children in a Virtual Reality Version of Pupil Perimetry. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2022; 15(3):1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.3.2

Chicago/Turabian Style

Portengen, Brendan L., Marnix Naber, Demi Jansen, Carlijn van den Boomen, Saskia M. Imhof, and Giorgio L. Porro. 2022. "Maintaining Fixation by Children in a Virtual Reality Version of Pupil Perimetry" Journal of Eye Movement Research 15, no. 3: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.3.2

APA Style

Portengen, B. L., Naber, M., Jansen, D., van den Boomen, C., Imhof, S. M., & Porro, G. L. (2022). Maintaining Fixation by Children in a Virtual Reality Version of Pupil Perimetry. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 15(3), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.15.3.2

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop