1. Introduction
Decisions are made almost every second. For example, we must decide which goods to purchase, which direction to explore, and which risks to avoid. Many studies have examined the decision-making process, and many models predict people ʼ s decisions under various conditions (
Dekay & Kim, 2005;
von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1944). However, the degree to which the bottom-up process can influence these decisions remains unknown, especially when the decisions involve uncertainty and risk in investment activities (
Sui et al., 2020). The current study is aim to test the boundary of the gaze-orientation effect on risky investment decisions.
Risky decisions are used to study how people make decisions, and many models have been provided. For example, the expected utility model suggests that people are rational and make decisions by evenly calculating and comparing the value of each option to receive maximal benefits (
von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1944). Later research indicated that people use heuristic processes to make decisions as per the noncompensatory model (
Simon, 1955,
1956). Heuristics as shortcuts can reduce the complexity of decision making by allowing the decision maker to focus on the most critical information (
Brandstätter et al., 2006). However, more recent studies have found evidence that contradicts the holistic model (
Birnbaum, 2008;
Birnbaum & LaCroix, 2008;
Glöckner & Herbold, 2011;
Glöckner & Pachur, 2012;
Hilbig, 2008). Recent study done by Su et al., (2013) compare the calculation process with the risk choice process, and their result showed that the eye-movement of risky choice task were different from the mere calculation process. These results suggested that many factors could influence people’s decisions. For example, people may be distracted by salient stimuli (salience effect), surface characteristics (framing effect), and subconscious processes (priming effect) (
Kuo et al., 2009;
Milosavljevic et al., 2012).
The latest studies have focused on the importance of gaze duration. A psychological phenomenon known as the mere-exposure effect (
Zajonc, 2001), suggests that extending exposure can induce individual preferences. Based on this phenomenon,
Shimojo et al. (
2003) presented the gaze-cascade model, showing a positive relationship between fixation duration and preference for a particular face, suggesting that people’s fixation duration can influence their preferences. Many studies showed that the longer fixation can predicted people’s preference on pictures (
Schotter et al., 2010;
Glaholt & Reingold, 2011). Researcher further manipulated the presented time of each option to test the causal relationship between the fixation and people’s preference on face (
Shimojo et al., 2003); foods (
Armel et al., 2008); and products (
Milosavljevic et al., 2012).
However, previous studies testing the causal relationship between gaze duration and final choice were limited in that they only manipulated the presentation time of each stimulus. This enables participants to detect the intent of these studies, which may cause demand effects (
Newell & Le Pelley, 2018). Therefore, to effectively avoid such demand effects,
Pärnamets et al. (
2015) developed a novel gaze-contingent prompt paradigm, which passively manipulates participantsʼ gaze times. Using the eye-tracking technique to record participantsʼ eye movements, participants were presented two alternative options simultaneously and were required to make a decision when their gaze on the randomly selected option (target option) reached the time limit. This paradigm assumes that people will accumulate stochastic evidence for one of the two options and thus make their decision when the accumulated evidence reaches a threshold. Therefore, the option that includes a longer process would become more preferable. Many researchers have used this paradigm, showing that gaze duration can manipulate moral judgment decisions (
Pärnamets et al., 2015), perceptual judgments (
Newell & Le Pelley, 2018), gambles (
Stewart et al., 2016), and risky choices (
Ashby et al., 2018;
Sui et al., 2020).
However, research has also suggested that the effect of gaze duration has a boundary. For example,
Shimojo et al. (
2003) found that the gaze cascade effect was stronger when participants were asked to choose their preference between two faces with similar attractiveness than to decide which face was rounder. Another study found that the cascade effect was stronger for neutral stimuli than for more extreme values (
Armel et al., 2008). Newell & Le Pelley (2018) showed that the bias effect existed only among impossible trials and not among possible trials. In their experiment, participants were asked to determine which picture contained more dots. For the impossible trials, participants were shown two pictures that each contained a very similar number of dots (e.g., 101 vs 102 dots). For the possible trials, participants were shown two pictures that each had a very different number of dots (e.g., 1 vs 100 dots).
These different results for perceptual and moral tasks indicated that both top-down and bottom-up processes can simultaneously influence decisions as stipulated by the dual-contribution model (
Shimojo et al., 2003). This model assumes that both the cognitive assessment system and orienting behaviors influence decisions, while gaze information mainly influences decisions through the bottom-up process. The attentional diffusion model (aDDM) proposed by Krajbich et al. (2010) further suggested that the bottom-up process (i.e. the gaze attention) guide the top-down process (i.e.the choice value) by amplify the choice value (
Smith & Krajbich, 2019). Many studies supported the idea and showed that there were some effects of bottom-up information (e.g., gaze position and duration) biasing the top-down process (e.g., attention) on decision making (e.g.
Tavares et al., 2017). In addition,
Ghaffari and Fiedler (
2018) adapted the gaze-contingent prompt paradigm by allowing participants to choose an option before the prompt (self-determined choice). Their result showed that the self-determined trials were appear when participants decided not to choice the target option and when participants were confident about their decision. These studies show that decision making may involve interactive processes.
Following this logic, gaze information may influence decisions that are difficult for people to make (or lack a top-down process). However, previous research primarily focused on studying the gaze bias effect on more subjective decision-making tasks and only used similarity between options as an indicator of decision difficulty. To date, relatively little is known about how interactive processes influence risk-investing decisions, especially when the task is computationally demanding. The latest study demonstrated only that gaze duration could determine risky decisions as other tasks (
Sui et al., 2020). The investment tasks required making a complex decision by simultaneously considering both the absolute reward and its uncertainty. This enables comparing the top-down and bottom-up processes in the decision making process. In this way, the current study adapted the gaze-contingent prompt paradigm by adding a self-determined option to further examine the effect of gaze on risky investment decisions after controlling the top-down process. Finally, risky investment decisions allow exploring the effect of gaze duration with a different difficulty indicator: computational difficulty. The complex computation process may increase the difficulty in making decisions, which will enable exploring the boundary of the gaze-cascade effect.
Results
Trials were excluded if participants reported that they could not see the options (clarity = 1). The self-determined rate was 22.4%, and the timeout rate was 1.4% for 8004 trials total. As suggested by Newell & Le Pelley (2018), the timeout trials were included in the analysis. Therefore, the current experiment analyzed the proportion of target options chosen under the different conditions when the self-determined trials were included and excluded. Mixed-effect models with the random effects of participants and stimuli were conducted to test the gaze-cascade effect on investment decision making.
Success of the gaze-cascade effect
Prior research showed that passively manipulating participantsʼ gaze duration allowed them to fixate on non-target options longer than on the target option. We defined successfully manipulated trials as those with a longer gaze duration on the target options than on the non-target options. The success rate for the manipulated trials was 55.3% for all trials. When the self-determined trials were excluded, the success rate increased to 60.3%.
Proportion of choosing the target option
When self-determined trials were included, the one sample t-test indicated that the proportion of trials in which the target option was chosen (M=51.10%, SD=3.53) was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%, p = .050). When self-determined trials were excluded, the proportion of trials in which the target option was chosen (M = 54.07%, SD = 8.29) was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%, p = .003). For the self-determined trials, the proportion of trials in which target option was chosen (M = 35.71%, SD = 22.36) was significantly lower than that if the choice had been random (50%, p = .001).
Proportion of choosing the target option under different conditions
When self-determined was included, the one sample t-test indicated that the proportion of trials in which the target option was chosen (M = 52.35%, SD = 4.90) was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%, p = .003) for the easy condition. While the proportion did not differ for the hard condition (M = 49.85%, SD = 8.29, p = .866). In addition, the one sample t-test indi-cated that the proportion of trials in which the target option was chosen(M = 52.53%, SD = 3.85) was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%, p <.001) for the low possibility condition(i.e. expected value difference smaller than 15), while the proportion did not differ for the high possibility condition (M = 50.04%, SD = 4.86, p = .955).
When self-determined trials were excluded, the propor-tion of trials in which the target option was chosen (M = 55.69%, SD = 9.05) was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%, p < .001) for the easy condition, but this proportion did not differ for the hard condition (M = 52.33%, SD =9.35, p = .113). The proportion of trials in which the target option was chosen was significantly higher than that if the choice had been random (50%) in both high (M = 53.06%, SD = 9.30) and low possibility condition (M = 55.69%, SD = 8.79), but the effect was stronger among the low possibility condition (p <.001), as compare to the high possibility condition (p = .039).
Effects of the top-down and bottom-up processes.
Mixed-effect models using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) were conducted to compare the top-down (higher expected value) and bottom-up (target position) processes during the investment decision-making tasks. The generalized linear mix effect model was conducted to examine the participants’ final choice (A = 0; B = 1) was predicted by the target position (A = 0; B = 1), the advance expected value of option B, and their interaction. For the random effect, the stimuli and participants were fit into the random intercept, while the advance expected value was fitted into the random slope varying across participants (see
Table 1). The result showed that the interaction effect was not significant, but the target position (
b = 0.16,
SE = 0.06,
p = .004), and the advance expected value of option B (
b = 1.76,
SE = 0.18,
p < .001) can both significantly predicted the final choice.
In addition, the generalized linear mix effect model was conducted to examine whether the task difficulty, possibility,the expected value of the target option, the timeout and self-determine trials would influence the gaze-cascade effect. The stimulus and participants were fitted into the model as random intercept (see
Table 2). The result showed that people would tend to select the target option when the target options are easy (
b = -2.53,
SE = 0.63,
p < .001), with similar expected value difference (
b = -0.07,
SE = 0.01,
p < .001) and have higher expected value (
b = 5.76,
SE = 0.27,
p < .001). The result also showed that the gaze cascade effect was stronger when the trials are not self-determined (
b = -0.30,
SE = 0.07,
p < .001) and not time-out (
b = -0.77,
SE = 0.22,
p < .001).
Discussion
This experiment manipulated task computational difficulty and examined whether the length of time spent looking at an option influenced investment decisions. The study design was the same as that used in previous studies except that this study allowed participants to self-determine when they had already decided. This adaptation increased the manipulation success rate. First, gaze manipulation influenced risky decisions when self-determined trials were both included and excluded. The effect was stronger when self-determined trials were excluded. Moreover, the target option was more likely to be chosen than was the non-target option in both difficulty (easy v.s hard) and possibility (small v.s large expected value difference) condition. Although the effect was stronger in the easy and small expected value difference condition as compare to the hard condition and large expected value difference condition. Finally, the mix effect logistic regression analysis showed that, the expected value and gaze manipulation can both influence participants final choice. Another mix effect logistic regression analysis showed that the gaze-cascade effect is stronger among the easy, smaller expected value difference and non-self-determine trials.
Sui et al. (
2020) found that gaze duration for the target option was shorter than that for the non-target option when using gaze-contingent manipulation; therefore, they suggest that the effectiveness of this paradigm should be improved. The current study adapted gaze-contingent manipulation by adding a self-determined option before the prompt as did
Ghaffari and Fiedler (
2018). These authors used this setting to separate the top-down and bottom-up processes during decision making. However, they did not test whether this adaptation improved the effectiveness of the paradigm. The current study showed that adding the self-determined option increased the success rate of gaze contingent manipulation. The timeout rate were also decrease in the current study as compare to the prior studies (e.g.
Sui et al., 2020;
Newell & Pelley, 2018). Therefore, the current study found that this adaptation improved the effectiveness of the gaze-contingent paradigm.
Several researchers have debated whether gaze duration manipulates decision making. Some researchers suggest that gaze duration as a bottom-up process could influence decisions (
Pärnamets, et al., 2015); other researchers suggest that gaze duration only reflects the top-down process of decision making (
Newell & Le Pelley, 2018). The current study revealed both cognitive assessment and orienting behaviors during decision making for risky investment decisions. Similar result was found among
Ghaffari and Fiedler (
2018)’s study, suggesting that although bottom-up information exerts some effects, the gaze manipulation can only influence people’s decision when they had no preference. In addition, the current study showed that the gaze-cascade effect was stronger when the target choice have higher value. These results are consistent with the attentional diffusion model(aDDM) proposed by Krajbich et al. (2010), which suggests that gaze can influence the choice process by amplify the value of the choice (
Smith &Krajbich, 2019)
The current study also found some surprising results. Prior research showed that the gaze-cascade effect was stronger when people had difficulty making decisions. The current study also showed that the gaze-cascade effect was stronger when the options are similar (i.e.small expected value difference). However, the current results also showed that the gaze-cascade effect was stronger under easy computational difficulty than under hard computational difficulty.
Su et al. (
2013) found similar results, showing that when computational difficulty increases, people tend to rely more on weighing and adding processes, that is, to calculate the expected value of each option. Therefore,people’s attention might focus more on the calculation process among the hard computation task and less likely to be influenced by the gaze orientation. Further study is needed to test this possibility.
In summary, the current study is aim to test the boundary of the gaze-cascade effect on risky investment decisions. The results showed that after controlling the top-down process, the target option with a longer gaze duration was more likely to be chosen. Therefore, the gaze-cascade effect might be only effective when people do not have clear preference. In addition, the current study showed that the gaze-cascade effect was also limited among the hard computational difficulty tasks. It is possible that the hard computational tasks would attract people’s attention to the calculation process instead of the risky taking process. Future work should investigate the underlying mechanism of the gaze-cascade effect for different levels of decision difficulty induced by option similarity and computational difficulty.