Next Article in Journal
Silent Versus Reading Out Loud modes: An Eye-Tracking Study
Previous Article in Journal
Object-Gaze Distance: Quantifying Near- Peripheral Gaze Behavior in Real-World Applications
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian

by
Ehab W. Hermena
Cognition and Neuroscience Research Laboratory Zayed University, Dubai, UAE
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2021, 14(1), 1-14; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6
Submission received: 8 April 2021 / Published: 31 May 2021

Abstract

:
Persian is an Indo-Iranian language that features a derivation of Arabic cursive script, where most letters within words are connectable to adjacent letters with ligatures. Two experiments are reported where the properties of Persian script were utilized to investigate the effects of reducing interword spacing and increasing the interletter distance (ligature) within a word. Experiment 1 revealed that decreasing interword spacing while extending interletter ligature by the same amount was detrimental to reading speed. Experiment 2 largely replicated these findings. The experiments show that providing the readers with inaccurate word boundary information is detrimental to reading rate. This was achieved by reducing the interword space that follows letters that do not connect to the next letter in Experiment 1, and replacing the interword space with ligature that connected the words in Experiment 2. In both experiments, readers were able to comprehend the text read, despite the considerable costs to reading rates in the experimental conditions.

Introduction

Persian is an Indo-Iranian language, a subdivision of Indo-European languages (Windfuhr, 1987). It is estimat to be spoken by about 110 million people worldwide (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Persian_language). Modern Persian alphabet, a derivation of the Arabic script, features thirty-two letters. Similar to Arabic script, Persian script is written from right-to-left. It is also a cursive script, with the majority of the letters connected with ligatures within words. As the case with the majority of Arabic letters, most Persian letters change form depending on where they occur in a word: beginning (connected to subsequent letters), middle (connected to letters on both sides), or end, connected only to previous letters (e.g., the letter س /s/ which appears a s Jemr 14 00006 i001 at the beginning, Jemr 14 00006 i002 in the middle, or Jemr 14 00006 i003 at the end of a word). When letters are connected, this connection is established by adding a flat ligature that is typically extendable (e.g., ordinary size ligatures: Jemr 14 00006 i001 Jemr 14 00006 i002or س, compared to when each ligature is extended by a factor of three: Jemr 14 00006 i004 or Jemr 14 00006 i005). An important implication of this is that the distance between letters within a word in this script is mostly demarcated by a horizontal black line, not white space (e.g., the trigram Jemr 14 00006 i006 /bsi/, which looks like this if the distance between the letters is increased by expanding the ligature: Jemr 14 00006 i007). Only seven Persian letters do not connect to the following letter: ا, د, ذ, ر , ز , ژ , and و. Thus, whereas there is always white space to demarcate word boundaries in Persian, that is, interword spacing, the distance between the letters in a word, or interletter spacing, may be composed of a combination of ligatures if the letters are conning, and white spaces (e.g., the word Jemr 14 00006 i008 features white space between the last two letters, Jemr 14 00006 i009, but the rest but the rest of the letters before are ‘separated’ by ligature. Thus, the allographic nature of Persian letters and cursive script, similar to Arabic, makes it an interesting script to use in studying eye movement control during reading. In the reported experiments the effects of manipulating word spacing and the distance between letters were investigated in reading Persian sentences.
Interword spacing, or the space between words in text, has been shown to play an important role in facilitating reading. This space allows readers to segment the text and perform word identification. Previous findings showed that removing or filling this space resulted in delaying word identification in sentence reading and thus slowing reading rate considerably (estimated 30-50% decrement, e.g., Drieghe, Fitzsimmons, & Liversedge, 2017; Morris, Rayner, & Pollatsek, 1990; Perea & Acha, 2009; Rayner, Fischer, & Pollatsek, 1998; Rayner, Yang, Schuett, & Slattery, 2013; Sheridan, Rayner, & Reingold, 2013; Sheridan, Reichle, & Reingold, 2016; Veldre, Drieghe, & Andrews, 2017; Yang & McConkie, 2001). The interword spacing also allows readers to benefit from the important information conveyed by each word’s first and last letters that play an important role in word identification (Davis, 2010; Gomez, Ratcliff, & Perea, 2008; Jordan, 1990, 1995). It is not surprising thus that moderate increases in interword spacing was found to facilitate reading in numerous studies (Drieghe, Brysbaert, & Desmet, 2005; Inhoff, Radach, & Heller, 2000; Paterson & Jordan, 2010; Slattery & Rayner, 2013). Increasing interword spacing was suggested to aid in text segmentation and word identification, and this improves reading performance given that words are the main units of linguistic processing during reading (e.g., Slattery & Rayner, 2013).
Furthermore, when considering the readers’ eye movements during text reading, the presence of space between words provides the necessary spatial frequency information needed in saccade targeting so that fixations may land on optimal spot for uptake of visual information (i.e., the preferred viewing location, PVL, Rayner, 1979). Removing these spaces results in significant changes to the saccadic targeting system, with readers’ fixations landing in suboptimal locations, closer to word beginning as well as shortening saccade amplitude (e.g., Paterson & Jordan, 2010; Perea & Acha, 2009; Rayner et al., 1998; Sheridan et al., 2013; Yang & McConkie, 2001).
Beyond the superficial visual processing of text, removing the space between words was found to disrupt the core linguistic processes of word identification. Several studies showed evidence of this by including in the sentences target words of high and low frequency. Word frequency effects are typically considered an indicator of the time course of lexical processing, with higher frequency words being identified earlier (i.e., faster) than lower frequency words (Rayner, 1998; Reingold, Reichle, Glaholt, & Sheridan, 2012). Indeed, several investigations showed that in the absence of interword spacing, frequency effects are amplified (e.g., Paterson & Jordan, 2010; Perea & Acha, 2009; Rayner et al., 1998; Sheridan et al., 2013; Sheridan et al., 2016). Furthermore, and more indicative of the disruption to word identification when interword spacing was removed, analyses of distributions of fixation times showed that the onset of word frequency effects was delayed, relative to when the interword spaces were preserved (Sheridan et al., 2013; Sheridan et al., 2016). There are languages that do not feature interword spacing (e.g., Chinese and Thai), however, segmentation and word boundary identification is of equal importance in these languages (e.g., Bai, Yan, Liversedge, Zang, & Rayner, 2008; Hsu & Huang, 2000a; 2000b; Li, Rayner, & Cave, 2009; Winskel, Radach, & Luksaneeyanawin, 2009).
Interletter spacing, or the space between the letters in a word also plays a role in word identification, albeit a role that requires further clarification. Reducing this space and making letters appear closer to each other increases visual crowding, or the phenomenon that a middle letter would be slower to identify if flanked by two close outer letters (e.g., Bouma, 1970; 1973; Chung, Levi, & Legge, 2001). Increasing interletter spacing and reducing crowding results in increased perception of letter size (Skottun & Freeman, 1983). Subtle increases in this space were reported to facilitate lexical decision (Perea, Moret-Tatay, & Gomez, 2011; Perea & Gomez, 2012). In sentence reading, subtle increases in letter spacing (+0.5 and +1.0 pixel conditions) was associated with reduction in average fixation duration, but an increase in the total number of fixations, relative to normal, unaltered, letter spacing, with the latter condition resulting in the shortest total sentence reading time (Slattery &Rayner, 2013, Experiment 1). On the other hand, Slattery and Rayner found that decreasing letter spacing by 0.5 pixel resulted in higher average fixation durations, increased number of fixations, and longer total sentence reading time relative to the unaltered interletter spacing condition. Interestingly, Slattery and Rayner found that manipulating interletter spacing had no significant effect on the rate of target word skipping (the word is not fixated at all during first pass reading). Similarly, this letter spacing manipulation had no significant effect on the location of the initial fixation this target word received, with these first fixations always landing at the optimal position between the word beginning and center. Slattery and Rayner concluded that the saccade targeting system rapidly adjusts to the spacing manipulation and continues to optimally serve the process of reading.
Other investigations revealed that any benefits of increasing interletter spacing asymptote, and even reverse, after a certain point in word identification tasks (e.g., Chung, 2002; McLeish, 2007; Paterson & Jordan 2010; Pelli et al., 2007; Risko, Lanthier, & Besner, 2011; Slattery, Yates, & Angele, 2016), with sizable disruptions reported when interletter space extends beyond 2-3 character spaces. Clearly, as interletter spacing increases, more characters are pushed out of foveal vision, and less information becomes available parafoveally as more and more characters are pushed further from fixation location. This has a detrimental effect on sentence reading that depends on the availability of foveal (fixated) and parafoveal (upcoming) information (see Rayner, 1998, 2009; Schotter, Angele, & Rayner, 2012). Some investigations revealed that readers compensate for increased interletter spacing by making more fixations, of shorter duration, relative to when reading normally-spaced texts, thus producing largely comparable overall sentence reading times in both conditions (e.g., Drieghe et al., 2005; McGowan, White, & Paterson, 2015; Perea, Giner, Marcet, & Gomez, 2016; Rayner et al., 1998). Notable inconsistencies of the reported results from interletter spacing manipulations were attributed to the different fonts used in the different investigations, given the natural, and sizable, differences in letter spacing in different fonts. For instance, monospaced fonts that render all characters, including spaces of equal size (e.g., Courier New), feature larger letter spacing than proportional fonts that allow for character spaces to vary naturally (e.g., Times New Roman, where the characters I naturally occupy narrower space than the character W; see relevant discus-sions in Hermena, Liversedge, & Drieghe, 2017 for fonts used in Arabic script; Perea et al., 2011; Slattery, 2016; Slattery & Rayner, 2013; Slattery et al., 2016; van den Boer & Hakvoort, 2015).
The two experiments reported here investigate the effects of reducing interword spacing, and increasing the distance (ligature) between Persian letters within words on eye movement behavior, and on sentence comprehension when reading Persian sentences. These experiments are part of an on-going series of investigations at our labs of interword and interletter spacing in Arabic and Persian as examples of cursive scripts.

Experiment 1

In this experiment the readers were presented with two conditions: A baseline condition with no manipulation of word or letter spacing or distance; and an experimental condition where interword spacing was reduced such that the words were almost touching (what will be referred to as the Pixel-Spaced condition, see Figure 1 for an example). The reduction of interword spacing in the Pixel-Spaced condition was accompanied by an interletter compensation such that the space before each word was added to the word itself in the form of extended ligature, thus increasing the distance between letters within the word. For example, printed with normal spacing, the two words Jemr 14 00006 i010 (= the weather was very…) can appear like this Jemr 14 00006 i011 with the space between them removed and added to the second word Jemr 14 00006 i012, in effect pushing the letters Jemr 14 00006 i013 and Jemr 14 00006 i014 away from each other by the same amount of space that was removed from between the words (see also Figure 1). In a sense, this manipulation is the opposite of one of the manipulations of Slattery and Rayner (2013). In their second experiment, Slattery and Rayner found the combination of reducing interletter spacing, and increasing interword spacing resulted in facilitation in sentence reading (reduced fixation durations). As such, the opposite effects (i.e., processing cost) may be expected in this condition, given that the PixelSpaced manipulation increased the distance (ligature) between the word’s letters and reduced interword spacing.
However, there is an alternative scenario. Namely, increasing the distance between the letters within the words in the Pixel-Spaced condition may equate to reducing crowding and lateral inhibition, and thus some benefit maybe observed. Importantly, the stimuli sentences in this experiment featured words that ended with letters that cannot be connected to the following letter (i.e., one of the letters Jemr 14 00006 i015 and و), that is, letters that naturally insert white space between letter strings and thus may effectively serve as word-end markers. One of the aims of this experiment is thus to determine whether readers use these letters as word-boundary markers. If the presence of these letters at the end of the word, combined with increasing the distance (ligature) between the letters within words facilitates word identification, it would be possible to offset, at least to some extent, the costs expected for dramatically decreasing interword spacing in the Pixel-Spaced condition.

Methods

Participants

The same set of participants took part in both experiments. Twenty-eight participants (six men) took part in the experiments. All participants were native Persian speakers living in the UAE, and all indicated that they regularly read Persian, on daily or weekly basis. The participants’ mean age was 35.7 years (SD = 8.7, range = 18 – 50). All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision as determined by the Bailey-Lovie chart (Bailey & Lovie, 1980).

Materials

Forty simple Persian sentences were used as stimuli, and were presented to the participants in either normally spaced (baseline condition), or with the space between the words (interword spacing) reduced significantly (the Pixel-Spaced condition). An example of the sentence used is available in Figure 1. With the exception of the last word in each sentence, all words ended with letters that cannot be connected to the following letter. The sentences comprised, on average, 10.4 words (SD = 2.1, range = 6 – 15 words). This was about 48.8 characters per sentence (including interword spaces in the Spaced condition, or the within-word ligatures that replaced this space in the Pixel-Spaced condition, SD = 8.4, range = 30 – 64 characters). The amount of physical space the sentences occupied in the Spaced and Pixel-Spaced conditions was thus identical. The sentences were all rendered in Arial font size 14. Arial is a proportional font that allows characters to naturally vary in the amount of physical space they occupy. It is a widely-known and used font in Persian print. Additional four sentences of similar complexity and length were used as practice items for the participants.
Stimuli norming. For assessing the grammaticality and correctness of structure of all stimuli sentences, in both experiments, additional 5 native readers of Persian were asked to rate the sentences on these variables on a 1 – 5 scale (1 = poor grammar / structure, 5 = perfect grammar/ structure), thus providing 5 ratings per sentence. Those 5 participants did not take part in the eye tracking procedure. All sentences for both experiments were rated as grammatically sound, with average rating of 4.5 (SD = 0.2, range = 3 – 5).

Apparatus

A tower-mounted EyeLink 1000+ eye tracker was used to sample readers’ eye movements during reading. Viewing was binocular, but eye movements were recorded from the right eye only. The eye tracker sampling rate was set to 1000Hz. The eye tracker was interfaced with a Silverstone computer, and with a 24-inch BenQ monitor. Monitor resolution was set at 1920 × 1080 pixels, with the maximum vertical refresh rate (144Hz). The participants leaned on a headrest to minimize head movements. The sentences were displayed as a single line, in black on a white background. The participants viewed the screen from 78 cm, and at this distance, on average, 4.3 characters equaled 1° of visual angle.

Design

The spacing manipulation was the within-participants independent variable. The order of sentence presentation was randomized, and the presentation of the two spacing conditions was counterbalanced such that each participant saw each sentence only once, in either the Spaced (baseline) or the Pixel-Spaced conditions.

Procedure

The study was approved by the university’s ethics review board. At the beginning of the testing session, the participants were given the consent form package (including information sheet). Consenting participants took part in a vision acuity test before the start of the eye tracking procedure.
The eye tracker was calibrated using a horizontal 3-point calibration at the beginning of the experiment, and the calibration was validated. Calibration accuracy was always ≤ 0.25°, otherwise calibration and validation were repeated. Prior to the onset of the target sentence, a circular fixation target (diameter = 1°) appeared on the screen in the location of the first character of the sentence. When the tracker registered a stable fixation on the circle, the sentence was displayed.
The participants were told to read silently and press a button on the button box when finished reading each sentence. Additionally, they would be required to use the button box to provide a yes/no answer to the comprehension questions that followed around 40% of the sentences. Before being exposed to the experimental sentences, the participants read 4 practice sentences (also followed by yes/no questions) to become acquainted with the procedure.
In total, the participants read 104 sentences (4 practice sentences + 40 sentences in Experiment 1 + 60 sentences in Experiment 2). The participants were allowed to take breaks followed by re-calibration of the tracker. The testing session lasted around 45-50 minutes, depending on how many breaks a participant took.

Results and Discussion

Global eye movement measures that index sentence processing are reported. These are: the average duration of fixations made during sentence reading, average number of fixations made, total sentence reading time (from the onset of the sentence until the participant pressed the button to change the display), and average amplitude (length) of saccades made during sentence reading (reported in visual angle). In addition, the average sentence comprehension score is also reported as an indicator of whether readers’ comprehension performance was affected by the spacing manipulation. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for these dependent measures for both spacing conditions.
The lme4 package (version 1.1-26, Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) was used within the R environment for statistical computing (R-Core Development Team, 2016) to analyze all dependent measures by fitting generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLMMs), with Gamma-distribution assumed for the fixation duration measures (Average Fixation Duration and Total Reading Time). Using GLMMs to analyze raw positively-skewed response times, including fixation durations, maintains the transparency of the reported analyses while satisfying the necessary normality assumptions, without the need to transform data (Lo & Andrews, 2015). For the sentence comprehension measure, logistic GLMM was used to account for the binary nature of this variable. In these models the spacing condition was the fixed variable, and subjects and items were the random variables. Models with maximal random structure were always the start point (Barr, Levy, Scheepers, & Tily, 2013). Model trimming was carried out when failure to converge occurred, or when singular boundaries (suggesting overparameterization) were identified. All findings reported here are from successfully converging models. For each measure the beta values (b), standard error (SE), t statistic, and the associated p value are reported in Table 2.
As Table 1 and Table 2 show, reducing the interword spacing and increasing the interletter distance in the Pixel-Spaced condition resulted in significant increases in average fixation duration, average fixation count, and in total sentence reading time. By contrast, saccade amplitude was significantly reduced in the Pixel-Spaced condition. Readers comprehension scores, however, indicated that they were still able to successfully comprehend the sentences they were reading, in both conditions, albeit with longer sentence reading time in the Pixel-Spaced condition.
The obtained results replicate previous findings where reducing interword spacing (the space between words) was detrimental to reading speed (see literature review above). Importantly, the results suggest that there was no clear benefit from increasing the distance (ligature) between the letters within a word. Furthermore, if readers used the Persian letters that do not connect to the next letter as word-boundary markers, the results show that dramatically reducing the white space that follows these letters results in similar reduction to reading speed as is the case in other non-cursive scripts. In other words, for Persian readers, word segmentation is more dependent on having normal-sized white space between the words, rather than relying on any cues from the letters that do not connect to the next letter. This is perhaps not surprising since such letters do regularly occur in the middle of words, as well as at word ends. These findings will be discussed in more detail in the General Discussion.

Experiment 2

This experiment aimed to replicate and expand on the findings of Experiment 1. The main difference between the two experiments was that in the current experiment the sentences used words that ended with letters that can be connected to the next letter. Experiment 2 thus featured Spaced (baseline) and Pixel-Spaced conditions, same as in Experiment 1. In addition, there was a third condition where the space between the words was replaced by ligature that connected the words. This condition will be referred to as the Connected condition. As opposed to extending the interletter space (ligature) within words in the Pixel-Spaced condition, in the Connected condition the interword spacing was completely replaced by between-word ligatures, without affecting the interletter distance within words (see Figure 2).
Completely filling the white interword space in previous investigations resulted in significant disruption to reading, as discussed above (e.g., Rayner et al., 1998 replacing the spaces with the character x; Sheridan et al., 2013 replacing the space with random numbers; etc.). However, none of these investigations reported a manipulation that involved cursive, connected-by-ligatures, text. Additionally, none of these investigations, also changed the appearance of the final and first letters of the words being connected as is the case when replacing the space between Persian words with connecting ligatures (see Figure 2). Importantly, connecting words by ligatures can be considered as a very strong manipulation that would compromise word segmentation cues in a way not possible in non-cursive scripts (e.g., European languages). In addition to significantly altering the appearance of the words’ first and last letters, this manipulation provides inaccurate information about word boundaries, which can be expected to slow readers down. Thus, in addition to expecting to replicate the disruption to reading in the Pixel-Spaced condition, it is plausible to expect even greater disruption to reading in the Connected condition given the loss of the white interword spacing, and the profound change to the first and last letters of the words when connected. As detailed above, these letters play an important role in word identification.

Methods

The participants, apparatus, and procedure in this experiment were identical to Experiment 1.

Stimuli

Sixty simple Persian sentences were used as stimuli, and were presented to the participants in the Spaced (baseline) condition, the Pixel-Spaced condition, or the Connected condition, that is, with the interword spacing replaced by ligatures, as explained above. An example of the sentences used are available in Figure 2. The sentences comprised, on average, 8.9 words (SD = 1.5, range = 7 – 13 words). This was about, on average, 47.8 characters per sentence (including interword spaces in the Spaced condition, or the within-word ligatures that replaced this space in the Pixel-Spaced condition, or the between-word ligatures that replaced this space in the Connected condition, SD = 6.5, range = 35 – 62 characters). The amount of physical space the sentences occupied in all three conditions was thus identical. The sentences were also rendered in Arial font size 14.

Design

The spacing manipulation was the within-participants independent variable. The order of sentence presentation was randomized, and the presentation of the three spacing conditions was counterbalanced such that each participant saw each sentence only once, in either the Spaced (baseline), the Pixel-Spaced, or Connected conditions.

Results and Discussion

The same dependent measures reported in Experiment 1 are reported in Experiment 2. Table 3 provides the descriptive statistics for these dependent measures for all three spacing conditions.
The same inferential analyses described in Experiment 1 were used in Experiment 2, using GLMMs within the R environment. Two contrast matrices were prespecified for the GLMM models. In the first matrix, the Spaced condition was treated as the baseline against which the Pixel-Spaced and Connected conditions were contrasted. In the second matrix, Pixel-Spaced and Connected conditions were contrasted. The full output of these analyses is reported in Table 4.
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced Contrast. As Table 3 and Table 4 show, the results obtained largely replicate those reported in Experiment 1, albeit with a reduced magnitude of effects for the Pixel-Spaced condition in Experiment 2. The small (and non-significant) increases in average fixation duration and average fixation count measures in the Pixel-Spaced condition translated into a significant increase in total sentence reading time in this condition. Also as seen in Experiment 1, saccade amplitude was significantly reduced in the Pixel-Spaced condition. And there was no significant difference in reading comprehension between the two conditions.
Spaced vs. Connected Contrast. There were sizable and significant costs for filling the interword spacing with ligature in the Connected condition whereby significant increases in average fixation duration, average fixation count, and total sentence reading time were observed. Saccade amplitude was also significantly shorter in the Connected condition. The difference between the two conditions in sentence comprehension was however not statistically reliable, with readers still scoring 85% accuracy on the Connected condition.
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected Contrast. Once again, there were sizable and significant costs for filling the interword spacing with ligature in the Connected condition relative to the Pixel-Spaced condition. Significant increases in average fixation duration, average fixation count, and total sentence reading time were observed. Saccade amplitude was also significantly shorter in the Connected condition. And the two conditions did not differ significantly in sentence comprehension.
In addition to largely replicating the sentence reading disruption observed in the Pixel-Spaced condition in Experiment 1, the results clearly show a massive disruption to reading in the Connected condition. Replacing interword spacing with ligature, and connecting the words thus altering the form of the first and last letters proved to be detrimental to the speed of reading, as predicted. Participants’ performance on reading comprehension was, however, still comparable in all conditions. These findings will be discussed in more detail below in the General Discussion.

General Discussion

The reported experiments aimed to use the properties of Persian script to explore how eye movement behavior and reading performance were affected by: (a) reducing interword (between words) spacing, while increasing the interletter distance (ligature) within words (the Pixel-Spaced conditions in both experiments), and (b) replacing the interword space with connecting ligature (the Connected condition in Experiment 2). In addition, Experiment 1 aimed to explore whether readers use Persian letters that do not connect to the following letter as word boundary markers.
The results obtained were unequivocal. With regards to reading rate, the severe reduction in interword spacing combined with increasing the interletter distance (ligature) within words in the Pixel-Spaced condition resulted in significant reading disruption in Experiment 1. This was largely replicated in Experiment 2. Amongst the mechanisms that can account for the reported results is that decreasing the interword white space in the PixelSpaced conditions may have resulted in the words’ first and last letters suffering increased crowding effects and lateral masking (e.g., Bouma, 1970; 1973; Townsend, Taylor, & Brown, 1971). The significant disruption to reading rate observed in the Connected condition indicates that not only the distance between characters is important, but also preserving the physical allographic form of these characters is also important for successful text segmentation and word identification. It will be recalled that connecting the first and last letters by ligature resulted in altering the physical forms of these letters.
With regards to the question whether readers use letters that do not connect to the next letter as word boundary markers, contrasting the results from both experiments was informative. Specifically, the results showed that reducing the white space that followed these letters (Experiment 1) yielded more sizable effects (e.g., the measures of average fixation duration and count, and total reading time) than reducing the space that followed the other letters that can be connected to the next letter (Experiment 2). One explanation may have to do with the fact that the letters that do not connect to the next letter regularly fall in the middle of words, and when they do, they are followed by a very small white space that separates them from the next letter within the word. As such, reducing the space that followed these letters in Experiment 1 potentially provided inaccurate word segmentation information to the readers (i.e., making them look like interletter and not interword spaces). Reading was thus significantly disrupted in the Pixel-Spaced condition in Experiment 1. By contrast, in Experiment 2, the fact that letters that can be connected to the next letter by ligatures remained unconnected in the Pixel-Spaced condition may have provided a valuable word boundary cue that somewhat attenuated the effect of reducing the interword space (see illustration in Figure 3). We can thus conclude that preserving or violating word segmentation cues is more important for reading than particular letter properties, such as the possibility of connecting to the next letter, per se. This is in line with findings that showed that text spacing that violates word boundaries was found to be particularly detrimental to reading rate (e.g., Bai et al., 2008, see also Epelboim et al., 1994; 1996; Morris et al., 1990; Pollatsek & Rayner, 1982; Rayner et al., 1998; Sheridan et al., 2013; Slattery et al., 2016).
A sizable disruption to reading was observed when the interword spaces were replaced by word-connecting ligature that also altered the form of the words’ first and final letters. As outlined above, connecting words by ligatures is a strong manipulation that compromise word segmentation cues in a way not possible in non-cursive scripts (e.g., European languages). These results replicate the findings discussed above with regards to the costs of reducing interword spacing (e.g., Drieghe et al., 2017; Morris et al., 1990; Perea & Acha, 2009; Rayner et al., 1998; Rayner et al., 2013; etc.), and the importance of the first and last letters in word identification (e.g., Davis, 2010; Gomez et al., 2008; Jordan, 1990; 1995). The reported results also add further support to the suggestion that text segmentation and word identification are vital for smooth reading (e.g., Slattery & Rayner, 2013).
Furthermore, in the Connected condition (Exp. 2), reading may have been disrupted because unsegmented text is an unfamiliar visual format for Persian readers. Bai et al. (2008) suggested that unfamiliar visual text format may disrupt reading and result in longer reading times relative to a more familiar format. However, as Sheridan et al. (2013) pointed out, visual familiarity cannot solely account for the observed disruption to reading rate. Indeed, in English, with its relative less complex visual characteristics (e.g., letters do not change shape depending on their location in the word, save some instances of initial capitalization), lengthy training and familiarization of participants to read unspaced texts did not result in reading facilitation (e.g., Malt & Seamon, 1978). Future investigations should further explore the psychological reality of visual familiarity in allographic scripts (e.g., Arabic and Persian), relative to the scripts of European languages.
Thus far the main focus was on the findings concerning how reading rate was affected by the reported experimental manipulations. The reported sentence comprehension scores in both experiments, even in the Connected condition in Experiment 2, replicated previous findings that readers are still able to comprehend unspaced text (e.g., Epelboim, Booth, & Steinman, 1994; 1996), albeit with the sizable decreases in reading rate and efficiency (e.g., Rayner et al., 1998). It is plausible to suggest that whereas the Pixel-Spaced condition posed significant difficulty, the difficulty readers encountered in reading the Connected condition in Experiment 2 makes the obtained comprehension scores more akin to solving a visual puzzle rather than natural sentence reading. The increases in readers’ average fixation duration, fixation counts, and total reading time suggest that the eye movement behavior was guided by the attempts to segment the text, identify words (and test hypotheses about where this should be done), to facilitate the processes of extracting meaning from the visual stimuli. That readers were able to obtain such high comprehension scores indicates the resilience of the linguistic processing system, and its role in guiding eye movements.
Finally, the obtained results may be considered to lend further support to models of eye movements control that postulates serial processing and word identification in reading (e.g., the E-Z Reader model: Pollatsek, Reichle, & Rayner, 2006; Rayner, Ashby, Pollatsek, & Reichle, 2004; Reichle, 2011; Reichle, Pollatsek, Fisher, & Rayner, 1998; Reichle, Pollatsek, & Rayner, 2012, see also Reichle, 2020), rather than models that postulate distributed attentional grade and parallel processing of multiple words (e.g., the SWIFT model: Engbert, Nuthmann, Richter, & Reinhold, 2005; Laubrock, Kliegl, & Engbert, 2006; Richter, Engbert, & Kliegl, 2006). Specifically, in the reported experiments, disrupting word identification by increasing the interletter distance (ligature), and bringing the upcoming word closer by reducing the interword spacing (the Pixel-Spaced conditions) did not result in any facilitation to reading, rather, the opposite. However, this may not be a completely fair assumption since bringing the parafoveal word closer was achieved by reducing the interword spacing, and thus dramatically reducing the ability to segment words. Presumably, both models would predict some sort of cost associated with this. For interletter spacing, both E-Z Reader and SWIFT models would predict costs for increasing interletter spacing, given that increasing this space would lead to placing the letters further from the point of fixation (e.g., see Bricolo, Salvi, Martelli, Arduino, & Daini, 2015; Pelli et al., 2007 for discussion of interletter spacing and crowding effects). As neither model has yet simulated the effects of interword or interletter spacing manipulations, making model-derived predictions about the effects of such manipulations is not possible (e.g., Perea et al., 2016; also Reichle, 2020). Further modeling activity is thus necessary to obtain further clarity.
Future investigations should further utilize the properties of cursive scripts (e.g., Arabic and Persian) to formally expand and update current theories and models to accommodate the characteristics of non-European scripts. In this regard, developing corpuses that provide letter positional probabilities (particularly the probability of letters occurring at word beginning or end, see e.g., Yen, Radach, Tzeng, & Tsai, 2012) can further elucidate the extent to which readers may use such properties and rely on certain letters (more than others) as markers of word boundaries.

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.ht ml and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Hajar Amani for her help in planning the experiments and data collection. Also many thanks to anonymous reviewer #1 and to Erik D. Reichle (reviewer #2) for their very helpful comments on the first draft of this article.

References

  1. Bai, X., G. Yan, S. P. Liversedge, X. Zang, and K. Rayner. 2008. Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 34: 1277–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bailey, I. L., and J. E. Lovie. 1980. The design and use of a new near-vision chart. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 57: 378–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barr, D. J., R. Levy, C. Scheepers, and H. J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68: 255–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bates, D., M. Mächler, B. Bolker, and S. Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bouma, H. 1970. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature 226: 177–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bouma, H. 1973. Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. Vision Research 13: 767–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bricolo, E., C. Salvi, M. Martelli, L. S. Arduino, and R. Daini. 2015. The effects of crowding on eye movement patterns in reading. Acta psychologica 160: 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chung, S. T. L. 2002. The effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 43: 1270–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chung, S. T. L., D. M. Levi, and G. E. Legge. 2001. Spatial-frequency and contrast properties of crowding. Vision Research 41: 1833–1850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Davis, C. J. 2010. The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review 117: 713–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Drieghe, D., M. Brysbaert, and T. Desmet. 2005. Parafoveal-on-foveal effects on eye movements in text reading: Does an extra space make a difference? Vision Research 45: 1693–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Drieghe, D., G. Fitzsimmons, and S. P. Liversedge. 2017. Parafoveal preview effects in reading unspaced text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43: 1701–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Engbert, R., A. Nuthmann, E. M. Richter, and K. Reinhold. 2005. SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review 112: 777–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Epelboim, J., J. R. Booth, and R. M. Steinman. 1994. Reading unspaced text: Implications for theories of reading eye movements. Vision Research 34: 1735–1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Epelboim, J., J. R. Booth, and R. M. Steinman. 1996. Much ado about nothing: The place of space in text. Vision Research 36: 465–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gomez, P., R. Ratcliff, and M. Perea. 2008. The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychological Review 115: 577–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hermena, E. W., S. P. Liversedge, and D. Drieghe. 2017. The influence of a word’s number of letters, spatial extent, and initial bigram characteristics on eye movement control during reading: Evidence from Arabic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 43: 451–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hsu, S., and K. Huang. 2000a. Effects of word spacing on reading Chinese text from a video display terminal. Perceptual and Motor Skills 90: 81–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hsu, S., and K. Huang. 2000b. Interword spacing in Chinese text layout. Perceptual and Motor Skills 91: 355–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Inhoff, A. W., R. Radach, and D. Heller. 2000. Complex compounds in German: Interword spaces facilitate segmentation but hinder assignment of meaning. Journal of Memory & Language 42: 23–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Jordan, T. R. 1990. Presenting words without interior letters: Superiority of single letters and influence of postmask boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 893–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jordan, T. R. 1995. Perceiving exterior letters of words: Differential influences of letter-fragment and nonletter-fragment masks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 512–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Laubrock, J., R. Kliegl, and R. Engbert. 2006. SWIFT explorations of age differences in eye movements during reading. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 872–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Li, X., K. Rayner, and K. R. Cave. 2009. On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology 58: 525–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lo, S., and S. Andrews. 2015. To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Malt, B. C., and J. G. Seamon. 1978. Peripheral and cognitive components of eye guidance in filled-space reading. Perception and Psychophysics 23: 399–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. McGowan, V. A., S. J. White, and K. B. Paterson. 2015. The effects of interword spacing on the eye movements of young and older readers. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27: 609–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. McLeish, E. 2007. A study of the effect of letter spacing on the reading speed of young readers with low vision. British Journal of Visual Impairment 25: 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Morris, R. K., K. Rayner, and A. Pollatsek. 1990. Eye movements in reading: the role of parafoveal letter and space information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 268–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Paterson, K. B., and T. R. Jordan. 2010. Effects of increased letter spacing on word identification and eye guidance during reading. Memory & Cognition 38: 502–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pelli, D. G., K. A. Tillman, J. Freeman, M. Su, T. D. Berger, and N. J. Majaj. 2007. Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. Journal of Vision 7: 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Perea, M., and J. Acha. 2009. Space information is important for reading. Vision Research 49: 1994–2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Perea, M., L. Giner, A. Marcet, and P. Gomez. 2016. Does Extra Interletter Spacing Help Text Reading in Skilled Adult Readers? The Spanish journal of psychology 19: E26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Perea, M., and P. Gomez. 2012. Increasing interletter spacing facilitates encoding of words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 19: 332–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Perea, M., C. Moret-Tatay, and P. Gomez. 2011. The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition. Acta Psychologica 137: 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pollatsek, A., and K. Rayner. 1982. Eye movement control in reading: The role of word boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8: 817–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Pollatsek, A., E. D. Reichle, and K. Rayner. 2006. Tests of the E-Z Reader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cognitive Psychology 52: 1–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. R Development Core Team. 2016. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R foundation for statistical computing. Available at: http://www.Rproject.org.
  39. Rayner, K. 1979. Eye Guidance in reading: Fixation locations within words. Perception 8: 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124: 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rayner, K. 2009. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rayner, K., J. Ashby, A. Pollatsek, and E. D. Reichle. 2004. The effects of frequency and predictability on eye fixations in reading: Implications for the E-Z Reader model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 720–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Rayner, K., M. H. Fischer, and A. Pollatsek. 1998. Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vision Research 38: 1129–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Rayner, K., J. Yang, S. Schuett, and T. J. Slattery. 2013. Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading unspaced text. Experimental Psychology 60: 354–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Reichle, E. D. 2011. Edited by S. P. Liversedge, I. D. Gilchrist and S. Everling. Serial attention models of reading. In The Oxford handbook on eye movements. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press, pp. 767–786. [Google Scholar]
  46. Reichle, E. D. 2020. Computational models of reading: A handbook. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  47. Reichle, E. D., A. Pollatsek, D. L. Fisher, and K. Rayner. 1998. Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review 105: 125–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Reichle, E. D., A. Pollatsek, and K. Rayner. 2012. Using E-Z reader to simulate eye movements in nonreading tasks: A unified framework for understanding the eye-mind link. Psychological Review 119: 155–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Reingold, E. M., E. D. Reichle, M. G. Glaholt, and H. Sheridan. 2012. Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology 65: 177–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Richter, E. M., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2006. Current advances in SWIFT. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Risko, E. F., S. N. Lanthier, and D. Besner. 2011. Basic processes in reading: The effect of interletter spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37: 1440–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Schotter, E. R., B. Angele, and K. Rayner. 2012. Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 74: 5–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Sheridan, H., K. Rayner, and E. M. Reingold. 2013. Unsegmented text delays word identification: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Visual Cognition 21: 38–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Sheridan, H., E. D. Reichle, and E. M. Reingold. 2016. Why does removing inter-word spaces produce reading deficits? The role of parafoveal processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 1543–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Skottun, B. C., and R. D. Freeman. 1983. Perceived size of letters depends on inter-letter spacing: A new visual illusion. Vision Research 23: 111–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Slattery, T. J. 2016. Edited by M. C. Dyson and C. Y. Suen. Eye movements: from psycholinguistics to font design. In Digital fonts and reading. pp. 54–78. Available online: https://www.worldscientific.com/doi/abs/10.1142/9789814759540_0004.
  57. Slattery, T. J., and K. Rayner. 2013. Effects of intraword and interword spacing on eye movements during reading: Exploring the optimal use of space in a line of text. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 75: 1275–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Slattery, T. J., M. Yates, and B. Angele. 2016. Interword and interletter spacing effects during reading revisited: Interactions with word and font characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 22: 406–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Townsend, J. T., S. G. Taylor, and D. R. Brown. 1971. Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time. Perception and Psychophysics 10: 375–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. van den Boer, M., and B. E. Hakvoort. 2015. Default spacing is the optimal spacing for word reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 68: 697–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Veldre, A., D. Drieghe, and S. Andrews. 2017. Spelling ability selectively predicts the magnitude of disruption in unspaced text reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43: 1612–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Windfuhr, G. L. 1987. Edited by Comrie Berard. Persian. In The World’s Major Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 523–546. [Google Scholar]
  63. Winskel, H., R. Radach, and S. Luksaneeyanawin. 2009. Eye movements when reading spaced and unspaced Thai and English: A comparison of Thai-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. Journal of Memory and Language 61: 339–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Yang, S.-N., and G. W. McConkie. 2001. Eye movements during reading: A theory of saccade initiation times. Vision Research 41: 3567–3585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Yen, M.-H., R. Radach, O. J.-L. Tzeng, and J.-L. Tsai. 2012. Usage of statistical cues for word boundary in reading Chinese sentences. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 25: 1007–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Sample of the stimuli sentences used in Experiment 1. The sample shows both the Spaced (baseline) and the Pixel-Spaced conditions.
Figure 1. Sample of the stimuli sentences used in Experiment 1. The sample shows both the Spaced (baseline) and the Pixel-Spaced conditions.
Jemr 14 00006 g001
Figure 2. Sample of the stimuli sentences used in Experiment 2. The sample shows the Spaced (baseline), the Pixel-Spaced, and Connected conditions.
Figure 2. Sample of the stimuli sentences used in Experiment 2. The sample shows the Spaced (baseline), the Pixel-Spaced, and Connected conditions.
Jemr 14 00006 g002
Figure 3. The Pixel-Spaced conditions in both experiments. The black arrows indicate where reducing the interword space, following the letters that do not connect to the next letter may have resulted in inaccurate word boundary cues in Experiment. 1. The grey arrows indicate where the words’ final letters in Experiment 2 could be connected to the next letter, but were not, thus perhaps providing word segmentation cues to the readers.
Figure 3. The Pixel-Spaced conditions in both experiments. The black arrows indicate where reducing the interword space, following the letters that do not connect to the next letter may have resulted in inaccurate word boundary cues in Experiment. 1. The grey arrows indicate where the words’ final letters in Experiment 2 could be connected to the next letter, but were not, thus perhaps providing word segmentation cues to the readers.
Jemr 14 00006 g003
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Dependent Measures in Experiment 1.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Dependent Measures in Experiment 1.
SpacedPixel-Spaced
Average Fixation Duration246265
(42.53)(40.77)
Average Fixation Count12.916.4
(5.37)(8.49)
Total Reading Time36875002
(1628.95)(2931.01)
Average Saccade Amplitude1.61.5
(0.50)(0.65)
Sentence Comprehension96%95%
(0.21)(0.22)
Note. Average fixation duration and total reading time are reported in milliseconds. Saccade amplitude is reported in degrees of visual angle. Standard deviations are between parentheses.
Table 2. Linear Mixed Models Outputs for the Measures Reported in Experiment 1.
Table 2. Linear Mixed Models Outputs for the Measures Reported in Experiment 1.
.BSEtp
Average Fixation Duration
(Intercept)253.339.1627.66<.0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced17.694.553.89.0001
Average Fixation Count
(Intercept)12.930.7317.70<.0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced3.410.556.23<.0001
Total Reading Time
(Intercept)4097.3616.65246.00<.0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced1485.1214.00106.10<.0001
Average Saccade Amplitude
(Intercept)1.630.0723.21<.0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced-0.160.04-4.09.0004
Sentence Comprehension
(Intercept)3.680.467.94<.0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced0.210.620.35.7290
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for the Dependent Measures in Experiment 2.
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for the Dependent Measures in Experiment 2.
SpacedPixel-SpacedConnected
Average Fixation236238311
Duration(38.67)(35.02)(50.85)
Average Fixation12.813.536.1
Count(5.35)(5.12)(22.49)
Total Reading3547374212937
Time(1561.82)(1528.79)(9218.88)
Average Saccade1.91.81.2
Amplitude(0.65)(0.60)(0.65)
Sentence Compre-92%91%85%
hension(0.27)(0.29)(0.36)
Note. Average fixation duration and total reading time are reported in milliseconds. Saccade amplitude is reported in degrees of visual angle. Standard deviations are between parentheses.
Table 4. Linear Mixed Models Outputs for the Measures Reported in Experiment 2.
Table 4. Linear Mixed Models Outputs for the Measures Reported in Experiment 2.
BSEtp
Average Fixation Duration
(Intercept)242.467.8131.05< .0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced1.342.660.50.6150
Spaced vs. Connected76.695.5213.90< .0001
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected-34.172.77-12.35< .0001
Average Fixation Count
(Intercept)12.800.8714.64< .0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced0.700.641.09.2760
Spaced vs. Connected23.262.0111.55< .0001
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected-11.291.00-11.28< .0001
Total Reading Time
(Intercept)3963.1311.77336.84< .0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced200.6612.1516.52< .0001
Spaced vs. Connected8437.3311.37741.78< .0001
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected-3235.159.61-336.50< .0001
Average Saccade Amplitude
(Intercept)1.880.0822.95< .0001
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced-0.110.03-3.24.0032
Spaced vs. Connected-0.720.07-10.87< .0001
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected0.300.0310.58< .0001
Sentence Comprehension
(Intercept)-25.357.88-3.22.0013
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced9.898.081.22.2209
Spaced vs. Connected14.348.051.78.0750
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected-0.891.31-0.68.4950

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hermena, E.W. Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2021, 14, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6

AMA Style

Hermena EW. Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2021; 14(1):1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hermena, Ehab W. 2021. "Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian" Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, no. 1: 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6

APA Style

Hermena, E. W. (2021). Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 14(1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop