Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian
Abstract
:Introduction
Experiment 1
Methods
Participants
Materials
Apparatus
Design
Procedure
Results and Discussion
Experiment 2
Methods
Stimuli
Design
Results and Discussion
General Discussion
Ethics and Conflict of Interest
Acknowledgments
References
- Bai, X., G. Yan, S. P. Liversedge, X. Zang, and K. Rayner. 2008. Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 34: 1277–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bailey, I. L., and J. E. Lovie. 1980. The design and use of a new near-vision chart. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 57: 378–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, D. J., R. Levy, C. Scheepers, and H. J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68: 255–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, D., M. Mächler, B. Bolker, and S. Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouma, H. 1970. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature 226: 177–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouma, H. 1973. Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. Vision Research 13: 767–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bricolo, E., C. Salvi, M. Martelli, L. S. Arduino, and R. Daini. 2015. The effects of crowding on eye movement patterns in reading. Acta psychologica 160: 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chung, S. T. L. 2002. The effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 43: 1270–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chung, S. T. L., D. M. Levi, and G. E. Legge. 2001. Spatial-frequency and contrast properties of crowding. Vision Research 41: 1833–1850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, C. J. 2010. The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review 117: 713–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drieghe, D., M. Brysbaert, and T. Desmet. 2005. Parafoveal-on-foveal effects on eye movements in text reading: Does an extra space make a difference? Vision Research 45: 1693–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drieghe, D., G. Fitzsimmons, and S. P. Liversedge. 2017. Parafoveal preview effects in reading unspaced text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43: 1701–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engbert, R., A. Nuthmann, E. M. Richter, and K. Reinhold. 2005. SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review 112: 777–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Epelboim, J., J. R. Booth, and R. M. Steinman. 1994. Reading unspaced text: Implications for theories of reading eye movements. Vision Research 34: 1735–1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epelboim, J., J. R. Booth, and R. M. Steinman. 1996. Much ado about nothing: The place of space in text. Vision Research 36: 465–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomez, P., R. Ratcliff, and M. Perea. 2008. The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychological Review 115: 577–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hermena, E. W., S. P. Liversedge, and D. Drieghe. 2017. The influence of a word’s number of letters, spatial extent, and initial bigram characteristics on eye movement control during reading: Evidence from Arabic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 43: 451–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsu, S., and K. Huang. 2000a. Effects of word spacing on reading Chinese text from a video display terminal. Perceptual and Motor Skills 90: 81–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsu, S., and K. Huang. 2000b. Interword spacing in Chinese text layout. Perceptual and Motor Skills 91: 355–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Inhoff, A. W., R. Radach, and D. Heller. 2000. Complex compounds in German: Interword spaces facilitate segmentation but hinder assignment of meaning. Journal of Memory & Language 42: 23–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, T. R. 1990. Presenting words without interior letters: Superiority of single letters and influence of postmask boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 893–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, T. R. 1995. Perceiving exterior letters of words: Differential influences of letter-fragment and nonletter-fragment masks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 512–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laubrock, J., R. Kliegl, and R. Engbert. 2006. SWIFT explorations of age differences in eye movements during reading. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 872–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, X., K. Rayner, and K. R. Cave. 2009. On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology 58: 525–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lo, S., and S. Andrews. 2015. To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malt, B. C., and J. G. Seamon. 1978. Peripheral and cognitive components of eye guidance in filled-space reading. Perception and Psychophysics 23: 399–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGowan, V. A., S. J. White, and K. B. Paterson. 2015. The effects of interword spacing on the eye movements of young and older readers. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27: 609–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McLeish, E. 2007. A study of the effect of letter spacing on the reading speed of young readers with low vision. British Journal of Visual Impairment 25: 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, R. K., K. Rayner, and A. Pollatsek. 1990. Eye movements in reading: the role of parafoveal letter and space information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 268–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paterson, K. B., and T. R. Jordan. 2010. Effects of increased letter spacing on word identification and eye guidance during reading. Memory & Cognition 38: 502–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pelli, D. G., K. A. Tillman, J. Freeman, M. Su, T. D. Berger, and N. J. Majaj. 2007. Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. Journal of Vision 7: 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perea, M., and J. Acha. 2009. Space information is important for reading. Vision Research 49: 1994–2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perea, M., L. Giner, A. Marcet, and P. Gomez. 2016. Does Extra Interletter Spacing Help Text Reading in Skilled Adult Readers? The Spanish journal of psychology 19: E26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perea, M., and P. Gomez. 2012. Increasing interletter spacing facilitates encoding of words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 19: 332–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perea, M., C. Moret-Tatay, and P. Gomez. 2011. The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition. Acta Psychologica 137: 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pollatsek, A., and K. Rayner. 1982. Eye movement control in reading: The role of word boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8: 817–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pollatsek, A., E. D. Reichle, and K. Rayner. 2006. Tests of the E-Z Reader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cognitive Psychology 52: 1–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. 2016. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R foundation for statistical computing. Available at: http://www.Rproject.org.
- Rayner, K. 1979. Eye Guidance in reading: Fixation locations within words. Perception 8: 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124: 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. 2009. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., J. Ashby, A. Pollatsek, and E. D. Reichle. 2004. The effects of frequency and predictability on eye fixations in reading: Implications for the E-Z Reader model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 720–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., M. H. Fischer, and A. Pollatsek. 1998. Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vision Research 38: 1129–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., J. Yang, S. Schuett, and T. J. Slattery. 2013. Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading unspaced text. Experimental Psychology 60: 354–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reichle, E. D. 2011. Edited by S. P. Liversedge, I. D. Gilchrist and S. Everling. Serial attention models of reading. In The Oxford handbook on eye movements. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press, pp. 767–786. [Google Scholar]
- Reichle, E. D. 2020. Computational models of reading: A handbook. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Reichle, E. D., A. Pollatsek, D. L. Fisher, and K. Rayner. 1998. Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review 105: 125–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reichle, E. D., A. Pollatsek, and K. Rayner. 2012. Using E-Z reader to simulate eye movements in nonreading tasks: A unified framework for understanding the eye-mind link. Psychological Review 119: 155–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reingold, E. M., E. D. Reichle, M. G. Glaholt, and H. Sheridan. 2012. Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology 65: 177–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richter, E. M., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2006. Current advances in SWIFT. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Risko, E. F., S. N. Lanthier, and D. Besner. 2011. Basic processes in reading: The effect of interletter spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37: 1440–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schotter, E. R., B. Angele, and K. Rayner. 2012. Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 74: 5–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheridan, H., K. Rayner, and E. M. Reingold. 2013. Unsegmented text delays word identification: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Visual Cognition 21: 38–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheridan, H., E. D. Reichle, and E. M. Reingold. 2016. Why does removing inter-word spaces produce reading deficits? The role of parafoveal processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 1543–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skottun, B. C., and R. D. Freeman. 1983. Perceived size of letters depends on inter-letter spacing: A new visual illusion. Vision Research 23: 111–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slattery, T. J. 2016. Edited by M. C. Dyson and C. Y. Suen. Eye movements: from psycholinguistics to font design. In Digital fonts and reading. pp. 54–78. Available online: https://www.worldscientific.com/doi/abs/10.1142/9789814759540_0004.
- Slattery, T. J., and K. Rayner. 2013. Effects of intraword and interword spacing on eye movements during reading: Exploring the optimal use of space in a line of text. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 75: 1275–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slattery, T. J., M. Yates, and B. Angele. 2016. Interword and interletter spacing effects during reading revisited: Interactions with word and font characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 22: 406–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Townsend, J. T., S. G. Taylor, and D. R. Brown. 1971. Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time. Perception and Psychophysics 10: 375–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van den Boer, M., and B. E. Hakvoort. 2015. Default spacing is the optimal spacing for word reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 68: 697–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veldre, A., D. Drieghe, and S. Andrews. 2017. Spelling ability selectively predicts the magnitude of disruption in unspaced text reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43: 1612–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Windfuhr, G. L. 1987. Edited by Comrie Berard. Persian. In The World’s Major Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 523–546. [Google Scholar]
- Winskel, H., R. Radach, and S. Luksaneeyanawin. 2009. Eye movements when reading spaced and unspaced Thai and English: A comparison of Thai-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. Journal of Memory and Language 61: 339–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, S.-N., and G. W. McConkie. 2001. Eye movements during reading: A theory of saccade initiation times. Vision Research 41: 3567–3585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yen, M.-H., R. Radach, O. J.-L. Tzeng, and J.-L. Tsai. 2012. Usage of statistical cues for word boundary in reading Chinese sentences. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 25: 1007–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Spaced | Pixel-Spaced | |
---|---|---|
Average Fixation Duration | 246 | 265 |
(42.53) | (40.77) | |
Average Fixation Count | 12.9 | 16.4 |
(5.37) | (8.49) | |
Total Reading Time | 3687 | 5002 |
(1628.95) | (2931.01) | |
Average Saccade Amplitude | 1.6 | 1.5 |
(0.50) | (0.65) | |
Sentence Comprehension | 96% | 95% |
(0.21) | (0.22) |
. | B | SE | t | p |
Average Fixation Duration | ||||
(Intercept) | 253.33 | 9.16 | 27.66 | <.0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 17.69 | 4.55 | 3.89 | .0001 |
Average Fixation Count | ||||
(Intercept) | 12.93 | 0.73 | 17.70 | <.0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 3.41 | 0.55 | 6.23 | <.0001 |
Total Reading Time | ||||
(Intercept) | 4097.36 | 16.65 | 246.00 | <.0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 1485.12 | 14.00 | 106.10 | <.0001 |
Average Saccade Amplitude | ||||
(Intercept) | 1.63 | 0.07 | 23.21 | <.0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | -0.16 | 0.04 | -4.09 | .0004 |
Sentence Comprehension | ||||
(Intercept) | 3.68 | 0.46 | 7.94 | <.0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 0.21 | 0.62 | 0.35 | .7290 |
Spaced | Pixel-Spaced | Connected | |
---|---|---|---|
Average Fixation | 236 | 238 | 311 |
Duration | (38.67) | (35.02) | (50.85) |
Average Fixation | 12.8 | 13.5 | 36.1 |
Count | (5.35) | (5.12) | (22.49) |
Total Reading | 3547 | 3742 | 12937 |
Time | (1561.82) | (1528.79) | (9218.88) |
Average Saccade | 1.9 | 1.8 | 1.2 |
Amplitude | (0.65) | (0.60) | (0.65) |
Sentence Compre- | 92% | 91% | 85% |
hension | (0.27) | (0.29) | (0.36) |
B | SE | t | p | |
Average Fixation Duration | ||||
(Intercept) | 242.46 | 7.81 | 31.05 | < .0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 1.34 | 2.66 | 0.50 | .6150 |
Spaced vs. Connected | 76.69 | 5.52 | 13.90 | < .0001 |
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected | -34.17 | 2.77 | -12.35 | < .0001 |
Average Fixation Count | ||||
(Intercept) | 12.80 | 0.87 | 14.64 | < .0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 0.70 | 0.64 | 1.09 | .2760 |
Spaced vs. Connected | 23.26 | 2.01 | 11.55 | < .0001 |
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected | -11.29 | 1.00 | -11.28 | < .0001 |
Total Reading Time | ||||
(Intercept) | 3963.13 | 11.77 | 336.84 | < .0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 200.66 | 12.15 | 16.52 | < .0001 |
Spaced vs. Connected | 8437.33 | 11.37 | 741.78 | < .0001 |
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected | -3235.15 | 9.61 | -336.50 | < .0001 |
Average Saccade Amplitude | ||||
(Intercept) | 1.88 | 0.08 | 22.95 | < .0001 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | -0.11 | 0.03 | -3.24 | .0032 |
Spaced vs. Connected | -0.72 | 0.07 | -10.87 | < .0001 |
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected | 0.30 | 0.03 | 10.58 | < .0001 |
Sentence Comprehension | ||||
(Intercept) | -25.35 | 7.88 | -3.22 | .0013 |
Spaced vs. Pixel-Spaced | 9.89 | 8.08 | 1.22 | .2209 |
Spaced vs. Connected | 14.34 | 8.05 | 1.78 | .0750 |
Pixel-Spaced vs. Connected | -0.89 | 1.31 | -0.68 | .4950 |
Copyright © 2021. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Hermena, E.W. Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2021, 14, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6
Hermena EW. Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2021; 14(1):1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6
Chicago/Turabian StyleHermena, Ehab W. 2021. "Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian" Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, no. 1: 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6
APA StyleHermena, E. W. (2021). Manipulating Interword and Interletter Spacing in Cursive Script: An Eye Movements Investigation of Reading Persian. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 14(1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.14.1.6